You are on page 1of 11

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF GERIATRIC PSYCHIATRY

Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008; 23: 8594.


Published online 25 May 2007 in Wiley InterScience
(www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/gps.1846

A randomized, two-year study of the efficacy of


cognitive intervention on elderly people: the Donostia
Longitudinal Study
Cristina Buiza 1, Igone Etxeberria 1, Nerea Galdona 1, Mara Feliciana Gonzalez 1,
Enrique Arriola 1, Adolfo Lopez de Munain 2, Elena Urdaneta 1 and Jose Javier Yanguas 1*
1
Department of Research and Development, Matia Gerontological Institute Foundation, Donostia, Spain
2
Department of Neurology, Hospital Donostia, Donostia, Spain

SUMMARY
Background Research on non-pharmacological therapies (cognitive rehabilitation) in old age has been very limited,
and most has not considered the effect of interventions of this type over extended periods of time.
Objective To investigate a new cognitive therapy in a randomized study with elderly people who did not suffer cognitive
impairment.
Methods The efficacy of this therapy was evaluated by means of post-hoc analysis of 238 people using biomedical,
cognitive, behavioural, quality of life (QoL), subjective memory, and affective assessments.
Results Scores for learning potential and different types of memory (working memory, immediate memory, logic memory)
for the treatment group improved significantly relative to the untreated controls.
Conclusions The most significant finding in this study was that learning potential continued at enhanced levels in trained
subjects over an intervention period lasting two years, thereby increasing rehabilitation potential and contributing to
successful ageing. Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

key words Donostia Longitudinal Study (DLS); learning potential; cognitive intervention; multidisciplinary assessment

INTRODUCTION understanding of the changes taking place in the


individual patterns of specific cognitive functions
The rising population of older people is a fact of (Newman et al., 2005).
21st century life. Today, senior citizens make up 16% of A review of the literature on the status of cognitive
the total population, and this percentage is expected to functions in persons over 65 suggests that even though
climb to 27% in 2010 (Eurostat, 2004). This growing neuroanatomical and neurochemical changes occur in
population means growing demand for specialized, the brain in the elderly, cognitive functioning stays at
multidisciplinary attention appropriate to the circum- tolerable levels during normal ageing (Jonides et al.,
stances of the elderly. 2000; Mell et al., 2005).
Looking at the pattern of human cognition during Studies into memory complaints have suggested
normal ageing, cross-over studies have shown that complaints of this kind in the elderly are ass-
differential associations between specific cognitive ociated with current cognitive performance and are
functions and the underlying neural basis susceptible not a predictor of the development of dementia
to degeneration (Fotenos et al., 2005). For this reason, (Flicker et al., 1993). Subjective complaints concern-
longitudinal studies are needed to provide a better ing memory tend to be related more to personality
traits and with depression than to dementia (Capella,
*Correspondence to: Dr J. Javier Yanguas, INGEMA, Usandizaga 6,
2005).
20002, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain. However, cognitive functioning is closely related
E-mail: jyanguas@fmatia.net to the cognitive reserve, meaning the scope for
Received 6 March 2006
Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted 15 August 2006
86 c. buiza ET AL.

positive cognitive change, also called learning potent- Scale) groupings, namely, GDS 12, GDS 34, and
ial. Learning potential can be defined as a subjects GDS 56 (Reisberg et al., 1982). The present paper
capacity to improve his or her cognitive yield considers only the portion of the investigation that
subsequent to training (Zhu, 2005). In other words, dealt with the GDS 12 group, i.e. elderly people with
individual variability after training is a basis for undamaged memories.
predicting the cognitive potential. Indeed, as Baltes The principal DLS objective was to study the
et al. (1995) has observed, a subjects learning effectiveness of non-pharmacological cognitive inter-
potential or cognitive reserve can be used to predict vention in older people who either exhibited no
the likelihood of his developing dementia. Learning cognitive deterioration or expressed AAMI (Age
potential could be a measure of an individuals reserve Associated Memory Impairment), effectiveness being
capacity and as such a good indicator of successful measured as objective improvement in the scores for
ageing (Fernandez-Ballesteros et al., 2005). the cognitive functions tested, both after treatment and
Numerous studies have demonstrated the important without treatment.
role played by cognitive training in averting age- The purpose was to investigate whether benefits to
associated memory alterations. Therefore, it is likely cognition and quality of life resulted from or were
that brain plasticity is distinctly possible in the elderly maintained by participating in 180 weekly sessions
(Kopytova et al., 2004). The ACTIVE (Advanced (2 years of follow-up) after the initial programme,
Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly) compared with a control group and a experimental
group has published a longitudinal study on cognitive group number 2, both likewise drawn from partici-
training and older people (Ball et al., 2002). Their pants in the initial programme.
results showed that cognitive interventions helped
older people to improve their cognitive yield (episodic
memory, reasoning, and processing speed) over METHODS
two years.
Subjects
Rapp et al. (2002) carried out a randomized clinical
trial to test the efficacy of a cognitive and behavioural Patients were selected from a variety of settings
treatment designed to improve memory execution and in order to obtain a broad range of people. All
participants attitudes toward their own memory of the participants were recruited from the comm-
performance. The results indicated that the exper- unity the Guipuzcoa region in Spain. Sample socio-
imental group had significantly better performance demographic characteristics are summarized in
and short-term memory evaluations than the controls Table 1.
at the end of the treatment period and over a 6-month Written informed consent or witnessed oral consent
follow-up period. to participate in the programme and to obtain a
These studies show the difference between cogni- serum sample was obtained from each patient
tive rehabilitation and cognitive stimulation. Some before the beginning of the study. The requirements
studies concluded that cognitive rehabilitation is not for inclusion in the study were to be over 65 years
related with improvement of cognitive function in of age without cognitive impairment or with standard
patients with dementia (Clare et al., 2005). On the criteria of AAMI (Age Associated Memory Impair-
other hand, frequent participation in cognitively ment; Crook et al., 1986). It requires a complaint of
stimulating activities is associated with reduced risk memory impairment, a score on a memory test one
of AD (Wilson et al., 2002). standard deviation below the mean performance of
Still, very few studies have maintained follow-up healthy young adults, adequate intellectual function-
for more protracted periods and/or have employed ing, absence of dementia, and absence of diseases that
sufficiently large samples to enable their findings to may cause memory impairment. The main causes of
be viewed with confidence, and hence the results must exclusion from the study were: (a) any degenerative
be considered with caution (Selwood et al., 2005). neurological disorder other than AD (Parkinsons
However, recent pharmacological studies are very disease, epilepsy, progressive supranuclear paraly-
short time to study the Alzheimer onset development sis, subdural bruise or other injury, convulsive
(Cummings et al., 2005). disorders, multiple sclerosis, etc.); (b) severe psycho-
For all these reasons, the Donostia Longitudinal tic traits, depression, agitation or behavioural pro-
Study (DLS) was designed to determine the benefits of blems that might prevent successful completion of
cognitive intervention in a large sample of subjects the programme; (c) a history of alcohol or sub-
divided into three separate GDS (Global Deterioration stance abuse; (d) schizophrenia; (e) systemic disease

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008; 23: 8594.
DOI: 10.1002/gps
donostia longitudinal study 87

Table 1. Demographic by group at baseline

Group Number of participants Gender Mean age

Experimental group number 1 85 29% males 73.28 (SD: 6.53)


71% females
Experimental group number 2 68 30% males 70.18 (SD: 5.58)
70% females
Control 85 26% males 73.14 (SD: 7.24)
74% females
Total 238 27% males 74.43 (SD: 8.31)
73% females

estimated as being likely to yield a life expectancy of elderly people and can produce feelings of anxiety and
less than one year. depression (Verhaeghen et al., 2000). In addition to
cognitive functions, issues relating to well-being
were also addressed in the experimental group number
Design
1, e.g. individuals health self-care, social skills,
The DLS employed a quasi-experimental, double- problem-solving, relaxation, self-esteem, feeding,
blind design that included different treatment con- physical activity, music-therapy, laught-therapy, cul-
ditions, namely, an experimental group number 1, an tural aspects (association, volunteering, gastronomy),
experimental group number 2, and a control group. liking, daily living (how to act in case of domestics
The total sample (n 238) was then divided into the accidents pets, how to act in case of medical
three treatment groupings (experimental group num- emergencies) spa-therapy, etc.
ber 1, experimental group number 2, control). Final Training addressed the cognitive functions of
group sizes were 85 subjects in the experimental group attention and orientation (special, temporal, and per-
number 1, 68 in the experimental group number 2, and sonal), memory (immediate execution memory, recent
85 in the control group. logic execution memory, recent word list memory,
After the baseline evaluation, the randomisation short-term memory, working memory, learning poten-
agency randomly assigned each subject to one of the tial), language (designation, repetition, oral compre-
three conditions described. Randomisation was hension, written comprehension, written language,
stratified by the participants age, gender and reading), visuoconstructive ability, executive func-
cardiovascular risk. tions (phonetic fluency, semantic fluency, abstraction,
categorization, planning, bimanual coordination, pre-
motor functioning), visomanual coordination (speed
Programme
and execution) and praxia (ideational and ideomotor).
The DLS lasted two years, during which time each The neuropsychologist avoided the use of any
patient underwent six evaluations, an initial assess- exercise that could have similarity with test assess-
ment followed by another every 6 months. The ment scales.
intervention grew out of a theoretical model developed Intervention programme content was structured so
by the authors based on Braak and Braaks model of as to be both weekly and monthly. In the experimental
Alzheimers staging (Braak and Braak, 1991, 1994, group number 1, weekly cognitive function training
1997), which recognizes the existence of pathological sessions were designed to reinforce the functions
anatomical correlates that affect different areas of the concerned (Figure 1). At the same time, in addition to
brain at the various stages of Alzheimers disease the weekly cognitive training, each month there was
(AD), with a specific disease course. one weekly session dealing with issues relating to
In view of this models basis in the current research, well-being. The intervention was similar in the
several steps were designed and implemented to experimental group number 2 in terms of session
enhance the interventions ability. length and frequency, venue, monitors, and the
The intervention was directed at all cognitive number of subjects in each group. The final content
functions, especially those considered essential to was the same in both the experimental group number 1
maintaining subjects independence and quality of and experimental group number 2, but in the latter
life. Memory loss is one of the main concerns of group the content was unstructured and did not follow

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008; 23: 8594.
DOI: 10.1002/gps
88 c. buiza ET AL.

any organized schedule. Lastly, the control group First, a multicomponent assessment (biomedical,
underwent the same evaluations as the subjects in the neuropsychological, affective, and personality) was
other two groups but did not experience training of any performed by an interdisciplinary team made up of a
kind and simply carried on with their lives as before. neurologist, four neuropsychologists, a geriatrist,
Training in the number 1 and number 2 exper- and a social worker, all suitably experienced and
imental groups took place in a group context. Session specifically trained for this study. The assessment was
frequency was twice weekly and session length was approved by the Matia Foundation Ethics Committee.
one hour and a half. There were a total of 180 sessions Only the neuropsychological evaluations are being
during the 2 years of intervention. considered and presented here.

Measurement of neuropsychological assessment Statistical methods


Over the course of this study six assessments were Statistical analysis was carried out using the SPSS
carried out per subject. Of these, four followed an program, version 12.0 using a level of significance
established protocol set out in Table 2, and two were of less than 0.05. Values are the mean and the
control assessments using the ADAS-COG test. standard deviation. The Kruskal-Wallis test was

Table 2. Biomedical and neuropsychological assessment protocol

Assessment Variable Tests Reference

Family and personal antecedents Medical interview


Medical exploration
Cognitive deteriorate of vascular origin Hachinski Scale Hachinski et al., 1975
Basic activities of daily living Barthel index Mahoney and Barthel, 1965
Instrumental activities of daily living Lawton scale Lawton and Brody, 1969
Temporal, spatial and personal orientation Information and orientation Wechsler, 1987
of WMS-R
Attention Direct and inverse digits Wechsler, 1987
of WMS-R
Working memory
Inmediate execution memory Logic memory of WMS-R Wechsler, 1987
Logic memory
Recent word list memory Auditory Verbal Learning Rey, 1964
Test (AVLT)
Short term memory
Learning potential
-Designation language -Boston vocabulary test Goodglass and Kaplan, 1996
Repetition language Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Goodglass and Kaplan, 1996
Examination (BDAE)
Audit compression
Written compression
Written language
Reading language
Visuo-constructive ability WAIS-III (BLOCKS) Wechsler, 1999
Planning Clock drawing (order and copy) Goodglass and Kaplan, 1983
Bimanual coordination Motor sequences of Luria Manga and Ramos, 2001
Pre-motor function
Visomanual coordination speed Trail Making Test, part A Army Individual Test Battery, 1944
Visomanual coordination execution Manga y Ramos, 2001

Phonetic fluency FAS Benton and Hamsher, 1989


Semantic fluency
Abstraction Proverbs
Categorization Similarities of WAIS-III Wechsler, 1999
ICPR Motor sequences of Luria Manga and Ramos, 2001
IMPR

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008; 23: 8594.
DOI: 10.1002/gps
donostia longitudinal study 89

Cerebral changes Clinical.


associated with normal Remote,
aging. autobiographical and
-NFT (neurofibrillar tangles) semantic memory are
in entorrinal region, maintained.
-Volume decrease in
subcortical areas

Neuropsychological correlate
Suitable activities. underlies.
-Implicit and explicit learning, -Medial structures of temporal lobe
-free recall and recognition, (hypothalamus: temporal back cortex;
-verbal and visual memory, amygdale: affective components of
-remote and autobiographic the memory; adjacent cortical areas
memory (thought tasks like to the hippocampus: entorrinal cortex,
narration of passed events, perirrinal and parahyppocampus),
memory of last dates), -Structures of front basal brain (septal
-semantic memory (to guess area and Broca band diagonal)
personages) -Medial thalamus

Figure 1. Example of the training of semantic memory.

used to establish differences within each group over 18.19  7.68. Finally, the mean scores for the control
time. group were: mean starting score: 12.45  7.04, mean
score at year one: 14.28  7.80, and mean score at year
two: 16.21  8.43.
RESULTS
The mean and the standard deviation (SD) values for Short term memory (STM). A large decrease was
each of the three groups are presented in Table 3 and in observed in all three groups, although the decreases
Figures 2 and 3. were statistically significant only for the experimental
group number 1 ( p < 0.001) and the control group
( p < 0.01).
Memory
Immediate memory (IMEME). There was a statisti- Working memory (WM). The scores for all the groups
cally significant improvement ( p < 0.001) in the increased both at one year and at two years, but the
experimental group number 1 which was more distinct increase was only statistically significant ( p < 0.003)
in year two than in year one (mean starting score: in the experimental group number 1 at year two (mean
18.66  7.50, mean score at year one: 18.94  7.55, starting score: 3.45  1.54, mean score at year one:
mean score at year two: 21.75  7.81). In the other two 4.07  1.40, and mean score at year two: 4.06  1.63)
groups there was no change after one year of (Figure 2).
intervention, although there was a tendency towards
improvement after two years.
Learning potential
Recent logic execution memory (RELEM). All three The results show an improvement in the scores for the
groups improved significantly ( p < 0.03) after two experimental group number 1 with respect to the
years of intervention. The mean scores for the starting score both at year one (mean starting score:
experimental group number 1 were: mean starting 5.18  2.48, mean score at year one: 5.87  2.26) and
score: 14.17  7.19, mean score at year one: 14.59  at year two (mean starting score: 5.18  2.48, mean
7.42, and mean score at year two: 17.37  8.65. The score at year two: 5.85  2.56). The increase after two
mean scores for the experimental group number 2 years of training was statistically significant ( p <
were: mean starting score: 13.84  7.68, mean score at 0.044). In the other two groups there was no
year one: 16.78  6.11, and mean score at year two: improvement in the scores for this function (Figure 3).

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008; 23: 8594.
DOI: 10.1002/gps
90 c. buiza ET AL.

Table 3. Means (and standard deviations) for the three groups on each measure, at the three assessment phases

Variable Experimental Experimental Control group


group number 1 group 2

Biannual IMEME 21.75 (7.5)*** 22.22 (7.55) 21 (7.61)


Annual IMEME 18.94 (6.62) 20.53 (6.02) 18.22 (6.72)
Initial IMEME 18.66 (6.32) 18.74 (7.51) 17.65 (6.68)
Biannual RELEM 17.37 (8.65)*** 18.19 (7.68)** 16.21 (8.43)**
Annual RELEM 14.59 (7.42) 16.78 (6.11) 14.28 (7.80)
Initial RELEM 14.17 (7.19) 13.84 (7.39) 12.45 (7.04)
Biannual STM 4.1 (2.32)*** 4.37 (2.11) 3.47 (1.70)**
Annual STM 3.43 (1.64) 3.78 (1.80) 3.32 (1.86)
Initial STM 5.71 (3.06) 5.21 (2.37) 4.88 (2.98)
Biannual VICOS 132.16 (65.70)*** 177.04 (211.28)** 132.07 (59.01)**
Annual VICOS 110.02 (57.78) 147.28 (74.19) 122.38 (67.95)
Initial VICOS 89.73 (75.02) 110.89 (60.80) 107.57 (66.64)
Biannual VICOC 1.15 (2.02) 1.00 (1.96) 1.88 (3.6)
Annual VICOC 0.58 (1.54) 1.56 (3.41) 1.08 (2.51)
Initial VICOC 0.79 (1.48) 1.37 (2.44) 0.84 (1.91)
Biannual PFLE 27.26 (11.47)* 22.74 (9.05) 24.07 (10.64)
Annual PFLE 26.07 (12.98) 20.75 (7.34) 22.32 (9.53)
Initial PFLE 23.58 (9.98) 20.21 (8.61) 22.06 (9.48)
Biannual ABSTRAC 3.88 (1.60) 3.22 (1.21)** 2.84 (1.96)**
Annual ABSTRAC 3.82 (1.91) 3.78 (1.99) 3.60 (2.01)
Initial ABSTRAC 4.34 (1.75) 4.26 (1.76) 4.12 (1.88)
Biannual ICPR 4.9 (0.37)* 4.85 (0.45) 4.88 (0.39)
Annual ICPR 4.98 (0) 5 (0) 4.96 (0.28)
Initial ICPR 4.98 (0.20) 5 (0) 4.96 (0.24)
Biannual IMPR 47.06 (2.21)** 47.26 (1.58) 47.72 (1.03)
Annual IMPR 47.76 (1.11) 47.88 (0.70) 47.92 (0.56)
Initial IMPR 47.90 (0.72) 47.89 (0.64) 47.70 (1.62)

ABSTRAC Abstraction; ICPR ideational praxias; IMEME immediate execution memory; IMPR ideomotor praxias; PFLE pho-
phonetic fluency execution; RELEM recent logic execution memory; STM short term memory; VICOS visomanual coordination
speed; VICOC visomanual coordination execution.
*p < 0.05.
**p < 0.01.
***p < 0.001 with respect to initial measure.

4,2
**
4

3,8
Experimental 1
3,6 Experimental 2
Control
3,4

3,2

3
Initial Annual Biannual
Figure 2. Change in Working Memory over time.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008; 23: 8594.
DOI: 10.1002/gps
donostia longitudinal study 91

Control

Biennial
Experimental 2 Annual
Initial

Experimental 1
*

4,5 5 5,5 6 6,5

Figure 3. Change in learning potential over time.

Visomanual coordination was 3.78  1.99. Lastly, in the control group the mean
starting score was 4.12  1.88, and the mean score at
Speed (VICOS)i. All three groups experienced a
year one was 2.84  1.96. However, at the end of year
decline in the first year, and in the second year the
two the experimental group number 1 had regained the
decline grew larger and became statistically signifi-
lost ground, whereas the experimental group number
cant in the three groups (experimental group number
2 (mean starting score: 4.26  1.76, mean score at year
1: p < 0.001, experimental group number 2: p < 0.007,
two: 3.22  1.21) and the control group (mean starting
control: p < 0.003).
score: 4.12  1.88, mean score at year two: 2.84 
1.96) underwent statistically significant declines ( p <
Motor execution (VICOC). The scores for all 0.002 and p < 0.007, respectively).
participants held steady in both years one and two
of the intervention, except for a small but statistically
significant ( p < 0.028) decrease in the control group at Praxia
year two (mean starting score: 0.84  0.11, mean Praxia remained constant in the three groups over the
score at year one: 1.08  0.51, mean score at year two: 2 years the intervention lasted, statistically significant
1.88  0.6). ( p < 0.004) results being achieved only in the
experimental group number 1 after year 2.
Executive functions
Phonetic fluency (PFLE). This function improved in DISCUSSION
all three groups at the end of the study period, This cognitive intervention grew out of a theoretical
slightly in the experimental group number 2 and model developed by the authors based on Braak and
control groups and substantially and significan- Braaks model of Alzheimers staging which recog-
tly ( p < 0.029) in the experimental group number 1 nizes the existence of pathological anatomical
(mean starting score: 23.58  9.98, mean score at correlates that affect different areas of the brain at
year one: 26.07  12.98, mean score at year two: the various stages of AD.
27.26  11.47).
Memory
Capacity for abstraction (ABSTRAC). The scores for
all the groups worsened considerably over the The scores for the memory functions assessed
first year of the study. In the experimental group (IMEME, RELEM, and learning potential) improved
number 1 the mean starting score was 4.34  1.75, in all the groups during the 2 years of follow-up. The
while the mean score at year one was 3.82  1.91. In findings can in some measure be attributed to
the experimental group number 2 the mean starting methodological issues attaching to the tests employed,
score was 4.26  1.76, and the mean score at year one with a repetition factor that affects test scores having

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008; 23: 8594.
DOI: 10.1002/gps
92 c. buiza ET AL.

been described in the literature (Dikmen et al., 1999). gramme considered in this study training focused
Since the same tests were administered at each heavily on this function. Results showed improve-
assessment, the subjects could have benefited from ments in the experimental group number 1 both at year
learning about the test content or the assessment one and at year two, the latter improvement being
procedure, thereby enhancing execution. Neverthe- statistically significant ( p < 0.044). In the other two
less, it should be noted that the improvement was in all groups, this function remained unchanged. These results
cases highest in the experimental group number 1, thus are an extremely important finding in ageing research,
reflecting the effect of intervention in addition to since the purpose of trying to improve the cognitive
possible learning through repetition of the tests. reserve is to make up for possible losses in abilities both
Short-term memory losses consistent with normal during the ageing process and in potential degenerative
ageing (Baddeley, 1986; Craik, 1991) and biological disorders (Fernandez-Ballesteros et al., 2005).
processes taking place at this time of life (Korol and
Gold, 1998) were recorded in all the groups. Generally
Visomanual coordination
speaking, short-term memory tends to decline over a
persons lifetime. Normal scores for adults are 7  2 Visomanual coordination decreased in the three
items, dropping to 5  2 items in the elderly not groups in year one and underwent a still larger and,
affected by brain disorders. These age-related memory moreover, statistically significant decline in year 2. As
deficits have been documented in formal laboratory has been found for manual dexterity (Francis and
tests (Light, 1991; Kausler, 1994; Small et al., 1999). Spirduso, 2000), these results can be ascribed to
Moreover, short-term memory decreases inexorably slower execution times in the elderly as they age (Van
with age, and training capable of offsetting these Gorp et al., 1990; Yan, 2000). The 2-year intervention
losses is difficult. Avila et al. (2004) reported similar programme proved to be unable to halt the decline,
results for memory in a Brazilian population. highlighting the need for a more specific intervention
While initially, then, it might seem that this study design for this function.
corroborates the findings of previous studies without Based on all the foregoing, it can be concluded that
contributing important new data, it is important to cognitive interventions, at least those built around
realize that this is a mistaken impression, because structured sessions which last for a sufficient length
most previous studies have been cross-over studies of time, are effective for elderly persons who have
whose results need to be validated by longitudinal not suffered cognitive impairment. This is borne out
studies (Delgado and Llorca, 2004), like the Donostia by the fact that the scores of the some cognitive
Longitudinal Study discussed here. functions for which training was provided improved or
Working memory improved in all three groups at held steady in those subjects who completed the
both year 1 and year 2. These results, too, can at intervention programme considered here. These results
least partly be explained by issues relating to the suggest that with intervention these subjects were able
methodology and assessment tools (Lezak, 1995). to tap into their cognitive reserve. A recent study
Still, it is important to underline the positive effect has proven that persons in GDS stage 2 show a pre- Mild
exerted by the intervention programme in the Cognitive Impairment (Reisberg et al., 2005).
experimental group number 1. The same results have Cappa et al. (2005) studied the effectiveness of
been obtained by other workers (Veltmeyer et al., non-pharmacological treatments in improving older
2005), who reported specific impairment in the peoples memory in the framework of the recommen-
operation of working memory systems that guide dations of the European Federation of Neurological
ongoing, planned behaviour and facilitate the acqui- Societies (EFNS), and they concluded that memory
sition and retention of new memories. improved significantly following cognitive training.
The SIMA Project for maintaining and supporting
independent living in old age (Oswald et al., 1996)
Learning potential
tested different interventions offering competence train-
In recent years it has been suggested that learning ing, memory training, psychomotor training, com-
potential (also called cognitive plasticity or rehabilita- bined psychomotor and competence training, and
tion potential) may be a good predictor of the course of combined memory and psychomotor training. The
cognitive impairment and dementia (Baltes and Willis, results demonstrated that memory and competence
1982). The consensus of opinion holds that learning training were the interventions that resulted in
potential continues throughout a persons entire the greatest improvement in the ability of older
lifetime. For this reason, in the intervention pro- people to lead independent lives.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008; 23: 8594.
DOI: 10.1002/gps
donostia longitudinal study 93

Social Caja Madrid a la mejor investigacion social.


KEY POINTS The authors also thank all those patients and their
 The experimental group number 1 exhibited relatives, monitors, Matia Foundation nursing homes,
significant gains in immediate memory, short- social services, municipal arts centres, Navarran
term memory, and working memory. Medical Service (OSASUNBIDEA), Fundacion La
 Learning potential increased for those partici- Caixa, parish churches, that contributed to this
pants who received cognitive therapy in both study.
years of the study, whereas in the other two
groups learning potential stayed flat.
 Cognitive therapy may act to prevent or
REFERENCES
postpone cognitive decline in patients at GDS
levels 12 at risk for dementia. Army Individual Test Battery. 1944. Manual of Directions and
Scoring. DC War Department, Adjuvant Generals Office:
Washington.
Avila R, Bottino CM, Carvalho IA, et al. 2004. Neuropsycho-
Limitations logical rehabilitation of memory deficits and activities of daily
The first limitation observed in this study concerns the living in patients with Alzheimers disease: a pilot study. Braz J
Med Biol Res 37: 17211729.
assessment tools employed themselves. Despite being Baddeley A. 1986. Working Memory. Clarendon: Oxford.
the tools recommended in the scientific literature, in Ball K, Berch D, Helmers K, et al. 2002. Effects of cognitive
some cases their discriminating ability is poor, mainly training interventions with older adults. JAMA 18: 22712281.
on account of limited effectiveness and a failure to Baltes MM, Kuhl KP, Gutzmann H, Sowarka D. 1995. Potential of
cognitive plasticity as a diagnostic instrument: a cross-validation
offer multiple versions. In the EADC (European and extension. Psychol Aging 2: 167172.
Alzheimers Disease Consortium) survey, Ramirez Baltes PB, Willis SL. 1982. Plasticity and Enhancement of
et al. (2005) mentioned the importance of harmoniza- Intellectual Functioning. In Old Age. Penn States Adult Devel-
tion of the tests used to evaluate cognitive deteriora- opment and Enrichment Project (ADEPT), Craik FIM, Trehub S
tion. The second limitation was that because there (eds). Cambridge University Press: London; 353389.
Benton AL, Hamsher K. 1989. Controlled Oral Word Association.
were a number of intervention groups, they obviously Multilingual Aphasia Examination. AJA Associates: Iowa.
could not all be led by the same person, the best Braak H, Braak E. 1991. Neuropathologycal staging of Alzheimer
situation methodologically to achieve optimally related changes. Acta Neuropathologica 82: 239259.
uniform results in the intervention. Steps were taken Braak H, Braak E. 1994. Morphological criteria for the recognition
of Alzheimers disease and the distribution pattern of cortical
to minimize the effect of multiple group leaders by changes related to this disorder. Neurobiol Aging 15: 355356.
having all monitors take a preparatory course in which Braak H, Braak E. 1997. Staging of Alzheimer-related cortical
they were trained in standard working procedures. destruction. Int Psychogeriatr 9: 257261.
However, the authors realize that there can be Capella E. 2005. The effects of single and dual sensory loss on symptoms
considerable differences between monitors and that of depression in the elderly. Int J Geriatr Psych 20: 855861.
Cappa SF, Benke T, Clarke S, et al. 2005. And Members of the
course satisfaction and the results achieved by the Task Force on Cognitive Rehabilitation. EFNS guidelines on
different groups may depend on monitor input. Lastly, cognitive rehabilitation: report of an EFNS task force. Eur J
the authors also realize that a experimental group Neurol 12: 665680.
number 2 intervention wholly devoid of any cognitive Clare L, Woods RT, Moniz ED, et al. 2005. Cognitive rehabilita-
tion and cognitive training for early-stage Alzheimers disease
training at all would have been best and might and vascular dementia. (Cochrane Review). The Cochrane Data-
have yielded different results, but by the same base of Systematic Reviews, Issue 4
token the subjects continued participation depended Craik FIM. 1991. Memory function in normal aging. In Memory
on offering meaningful course content as part of the Disorders, Research and Clinical Practice, Yanagihara T,
intervention programme. Offering only routine, Petersen RC (eds). Marcel Dekker: Nueva York; 402415.
Crook T, Bartus RT, Ferris SH, et al. 1986. Age associated memory
repetitive activities would in all likelihood have led impairment: proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical
most of the participants to drop out of the study. change. Report of a National Institute of Mental health work
group. Development Neuropsychol 2: 261276.
Cummings JL, Koumaras B, Chen M, Mirski D and the
Rivastigmine Nursing Home Study Team. 2005. Effects of
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS rivastigmine treatment on the neuropsychiatric and behavioral
This study was made possible through the assistance disturbances of nursing home residents with moderate to severe
probable Alzheimers disease: a 26-week, multicenter, open-label
of IMSERSO (Institute of Migrations and Social study. Am J Geriatr Pharmacother 3: 137148.
Services, specially Mara Teresa Sancho Castiello). Delgado M, Llorca J. 2004. Estudios longitudinales: concepto y
This study was awarded in 2005 with Premio Obra particularidades. Rev Esp Sa Pub 78: 141148.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008; 23: 8594.
DOI: 10.1002/gps
94 c. buiza ET AL.

Dikmen SS, Heaton RK, Grant I, Temkin NR. 1999. Test-retest disease in the Cardiovascular Health Study cohort. J Am Geriatr
reliability and practice effects of expanded Halstead-Reitan Neu- Soc 53: 11011107.
ropsychological Test Battery. J Int Neuropsychologic Soc 5: Oswald WD, Rupprecht R, Gunzelmann T, Tritt K. 1996. The
346356. SIMA-project: effects of 1 year cognitive and psychomotor
Eurostat Yearbook. 2004. The Statistical Guide to EuropeData training on cognitive abilities of the elderly. Behav Brain Res
19922002. Electronic version europa.eu.int/comm/eurostat/ 78: 6772.
Fernandez-Ballesteros R, Zamarron MD, Tarraga L. 2005. Ramirez S, Gregorio P, Ribera Casado JM, et al. and the EADC
Learning potential: a new method for assessing cognitive impair- Centres and Investigators. 2005. The need for a consensus in the
ment. Int Psychogeriatr 17: 119128. use of assessment tools for Alzheimers disease: the Feasibility
Flicker C, Ferris SH, Reisberg BA. 1993. A longitudinal study of Study (assessment tools for dementia in Alzheimer Centres across
cognitive function in elderly persons with subjective memory Europe), a European Alzheimers Disease Consortiums (EADC)
complaints. J Am Geriatr Soc 41: 10291032. survey. Int J Geriatr Psych 20: 744748.
Fotenos A, Snyder Z, Girton LE, et al. 2005. Normative estimates Rapp S, Brenes G, Marsh AP. 2002. Memory enhancement
of cross-sectional an longitudinal brain volume decline in aging training for older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a
and AD. Neurology 64: 10321039. preliminary study. Aging Ment Health 1: 511.
Francis KL, Spirduso WW. 2000. Age differences in the expression Reisberg B, Ferris SH, De Leon MJ, Crook T. 1982. The Global
of manual asymmetry. Exp Aging Res 2: 169180. Deterioration Scale of Assessment of primary degenerative
Goodglass H, Kaplan E. 1983. Evaluacion de la afasia y de trastornos dementia. Am J Psychiatry 139: 11361139.
relacionados. Editorial Medica Panamericana: Madrid. Reisberg B, Ferris SH, De Leon MJ, et al. 2005. Subjective
Goodglass H, Kaplan E. 1996. Test de vocabulario de Boston. Cognitive Impairment: The pre-Mild Cognitive Impairment stage
Editorial medica panamericana: Madrid. of brain degeneration: longitudinal outcome after a mean of 7
Hachinski CV, Iliff LD, Zilkha E. 1975. Cerebral blood flow of years follow-up. Neuropsychopharmacol 30 (Suppl. 1): S81.
dementia. Arch Neurology 32: 632637. Rey A. 1964. Lexamen clinique en psychologie. Presses universi-
Jonides J, Marshuetz C, Smith EE, et al. 2000. Age differences in taires de France: Paris.
behavior and PET activation reveal differences in interference Selwood A, Thorgrimsen L, Orrell M. 2005. Quality of live in
resolution in verbal working memory. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 188196. dementia. A one year follow-up study. In J Geriatr Psychiatry 20:
Kausler DH. 1994. Learning and Memory in Normal Aging. 232237.
Academic Press: New York. Small SA, Stern Y, Tang M, Mayeux R. 1999. Selective decline in
Korol DL, Gold PE. 1998. Memory, glucose and aging. Am J Clin memory function among healthy elderly. Neurology 52:
Nutr 4: 764771. 13921396.
Kopytova FV, Mednikova YS, Popova EN. 2004. Age-related Van Gorp WG, Satz P, Mitrushina M. 1990. Neuropsychologycal
structural and functional characteristics of rabbit hippocampal processes associated with normal aging. Development Neurop-
neurons during the formation of temporal associations. Neurosci sychol 6: 279290.
Behav Physiol 34: 889896. Veltmeyer MD, Clark CR, McFarlane AC, et al. 2005. Integrative
Lawton MP, Brody EM. 1969. Assesment of older people: assessment of brain and cognitive function in post-traumatic
self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Ger- stress disorder. J Integr Neurosci 4: 145159.
ontologist 9: 179186. Verhaeghen P, Geraerts N, Marcoen A. 2000. Memory complaints,
Lezak MD. 1995. Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford Uni- coping, and well-being in old age: a systemic approach. Ger-
versity Press: New York. ontologist 40: 540548.
Light LL. 1991. Memory in aging: four hyphoteses in search of Wechsler D. 1987. Wechsler Memory Scale Revised Manual.
data. Ann Rev Psychol 42: 333376. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX.
Mahoney FI, Barthel D. 1965. Functional evaluation: the Barthel Wechsler D. 1999. Adaptacion espanola de la Wechsler Adult
index. MD State Medic J 14: 6165. Intelligence Scale-III. TEA Ediciones, SA: Madrid.
Manga D, Ramos F. 2001. Secuencias Motoras de Luria. Luria DNA. Wilson RS, Mendes de Leon CF, Barnes LL, et al. 2002.
Diagnostico neuropsicologico de adultos. TEA ediciones: Madrid. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of
Mell T, Heekeren HR, Marschner A, et al. 2005. Effect of aging incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA 287: 742748.
on stimulus-reward association learning. Neuropsychologia Yan JH. 2000. Effects of aging on linear and curvilinear aiming arm
43: 554563. movements. Exp Aging Res 4: 393407.
Newman AB, Fitzpatrick Al, Lopez O, et al. 2005. Dementia and Zhu DM. 2005. Task workload and cognitive abilities in dynamic
Alzheimers disease incidence in relationship to cardiovascular decision making. Hum Factors 47: 92101.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008; 23: 8594.
DOI: 10.1002/gps

You might also like