You are on page 1of 28

Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1981 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1981, Vol. 90. No. I, 125-152 0033-2909/81/9001-0125SOO. 75

Goal Setting and Task Performance: 1969-1980


Edwin A. Locke Karyll N. Shaw
College of Business and Management and College of Business and Management
Department of Psychology University of Maryland
University of Maryland
Lise M. Saari Gary P. Latham
Department of Psychology Graduate School of Business Administration
University of Washington University of Washington

A review of both laboratory and field studies on the effects of setting goals when
performing a task found that in 90% of the studies, specific and challenging goals
lead to higher performance than easy goals, "do your best" goals, or no goals.
Goals affect performance by directing attention, mobilizing effort, increasing
persistence, and motivating strategy development. Goal setting is most likely to
improve task performance when the goals are specific and sufficiently challenging,
the subjects have sufficient ability (and ability differences are controlled), feed-
back is provided to show progress in relation to the goal, rewards such as money
are given for goal attainment, the experimenter or manager is supportive, and
assigned goals are accepted by the individual. No reliable individual differences
have emerged in goal-setting studies, probably because the goals were typically
assigned rather than self-set. Need for achievement and self-esteem may be the
most promising individual difference variables.

In this article we summarize research re- and the other organizational. The academic
lating to (a) the effects of setting various source extends back in time from Ryan
types of goals or objectives on task perfor- (1970) and G. Miller, Galanter, and Pri-
mance and (b) the factors (other than the brani (1960), through Lewin, to the Wurz-
goals themselves) that influence the effec- burg School and the associated concepts of
tiveness of goal setting. intention, task, set, and level of aspiration
Ail-encompassing theories of motivation (see Ryan, 1970, for a summary). The or-
based on such concepts as instinct, drive, and ganizational source is traced from Manage-
conditioning have not succeeded in explain- ment by Objectives programs, now widely
ing human action. Such theories have been used in industry (see Odiorne, 1978, for a
gradually replaced by more modest and lim- summary), back to the Scientific Manage-
ited approaches to motivation. These ap- ment movement founded by Frederick W,
proaches do not presume to explain all mo- Taylor (1911/1967). These two strains of
tivational phenomena; their domains are thought converge in the more recent work
more restricted. The study of goal setting is of Locke (1968), Latham (Latham & Yukl,
one such limited approach. 1975b), and others who have studied the
The concept of goal setting falls within the effects of goal setting on task performance.
broad domain of cognitive psychology and Goal setting is also an important component
is consistent with recent trends such as cog- of social learning theory (Bandura, 1977),
nitive behavior modification (Meichenbaum, which has become increasingly influential in
1977). The present interest of researchers in recent years. Even the literature on organi-
goal setting has two sources, one academic zational behavior modification can be inter-
preted largely within a goal-setting frame-
work (Locke, 1977).
Preparation of this manuscript was supported by Of- Research on goal setting is proliferating
fice of Naval Research Contract N00014-79-C-0680.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Edwin A. so rapidly that recent reviews (Latham &
Locke, College of Business and Management, University Yukl, I975b; Locke, 1968; Steers & Porter,
of Maryland, College Park, Maryland 20742. 1974) are now outdated. To provide a longer
125
126 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

term perspective than just the last 6 years, review here. We also examine the mecha-
our review includes research published since nisms by which goals affect action, the ef-
1968. Studies that are explicitly clinical and fects of feedback, participation, and money
social-psychological in nature are not in- on goal-setting effectiveness, the role of in-
cluded (for a detailed review of the latter, dividual differences, and the determinants
see Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). of goal commitment.

The Concept of Goal Setting


Goal-Setting Attributes'
A goal is what an individual is trying to
accomplish; it is the object or aim of an ac- Mental processes have two major attri-
tion. The concept is similar in meaning to butes, content and intensity (Rand, 1967).
the concepts of purpose and intent (Locke, The content of a goal is the object or result
1969). Other frequently used concepts that being sought. The main dimensions of goal
are also similar in meaning to that of goal content that have been studied so far are
include performance standard (a measuring specificity or clarity (the degree of quanti-
rod for evaluating performance), quota (a tative precision with which the aim is spec-
minimum amount of work or production), ified) and difficulty (the degree of profi-
work norm (a standard of acceptable be- ciency or level of performance sought). The
havior defined by a work group), task (a terms task difficulty and goal difficulty are
piece of work to be accomplished), objective often used interchangeably, but a distinction
(the ultimate aim of an action or series of between them can be made.
actions), deadline (a time limit for complet- A task is a piece of work to be accom-
ing a task), and budget (a spending goal or plished. A difficult task is one that is hard
limit). to do. A task can be hard because it is com-
Earlier attempts by behaviorists to reduce plex, that is, requires a high level of skill and
concepts like goal and purpose to physical knowledge. For example, writing a book on
events have been strongly criticized (e.g., see physics is a harder task than writing a thank-
Locke, 1969, 1972). Goal setting might be you note. A task can also be hard because
called "stimulus control" by a modern be- it requires a great deal of effort: digging the
haviorist, but the key question then becomes, foundation for a pool takes more effort than
What is the stimulus? If it is only an as- digging a hole to plant a flower seed.
signed goal (an environmental event), then Since a goal is the object or aim of an
the importance of goal acceptance is ignored; action, it is possible for the completion of a
an assigned goal that is rejected can hardly task to be a goal. However, in most goal-
regulate performance. If goal acceptance is setting studies, the term goal refers to at-
considered relevant, then the regulating taining a specific standard of proficiency on
stimulus must be a mental eventultimately a task, usually within a specified time limit.
the individual's goal. The environment, of For example, two individuals are given the
course, can influence goal setting as well as same task (e.g., simple addition), but one is
goal acceptance, an issue that is dealt with asked to complete a large number of prob-
in some of the recent research, lems within 30 minutes, and the other, a
The basic assumption of goal-setting re- small number. The harder goal would be
search is that goals are immediate regulators achieved by expending greater effort and
of human action. However, no one-to-one attention than would be expended to achieve
correspondence between goals and action is the easy goal. Harder goals, like harder
assumed because people may make errors, tasks, also can require more knowledge and
lack the ability to attain their objectives skill than easier goals (e.g., winning a chess
(Locke, 1968), or have subconscious con-
flicts or premises that subvert their conscious ' Our view of what constitutes a goal attribute differs
from that of Steers and Porter (1974) who, for example,
goals. The precise degree of association be- called participation an attribute of goals. We treat par-
tween goals and action is an empirical ques- ticipation as a mechanism that may affect goal content
tion that is dealt with in the research we or goal acceptance.
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 127

tournament vs. coming in next to last). To figure out how to attain them than the pro-
summarize the distinction between the terms, cess needed to set and attain simple goals.
goal difficulty specifies a certain level of task Goal intensity has not been studied as such,
proficiency measured against a standard, although a related concept, goal commit-
whereas task difficulty refers simply to the ment, has been measured in a number of
nature of the work to be accomplished. experiments.
Although greater task difficulty should
lead to greater effort (Kahneman, 1973;
Relation of Goal Dimensions
Kaplan & Rothkopf, 1974; Shapira, Note to Performance
1), the relation of task difficulty to perfor-
mance is problematic. If more work is trans- Goal Difficulty
lated into a goal to get more done, task
difficulty may be positively related to In an earlier review of the goal-setting lit-
performance (Sales, 1970). On the other erature, Locke (1968) found evidence for a
hand, if harder tasks require more ability or positive, linear relation between goal diffi-
knowledge, most people will, at least ini- culty and task performance (assuming suf-
tially, perform less well on them, even if they ficient ability), and more recent studies have
try harder, than they would on easier tasks supported these findings. Four results in
(e.g., Shapira, Note 1). three experimental field studies demon-
An experiment by Campbell and Ilgen strated that harder goals led to better per-
(1976) demonstrated that the distinction formance than easy goals: Latham and
between task and goal difficulty has prac- Locke (1975) with logging crews; Yukl and
tical utility. They manipulated both dimen- Latham (1978) with typists; and a simulated
sions independently, On chess problems dif- field study by Bassett (1979). In a separate
ficult goals led to better performance than manipulation, Bassett also found that shorter
easy goals; training subjects on hard prob- time limits led to a faster work pace than
lems (tasks) led at first to poorer perfor- longer time limits.
mance but later to better performance than Twenty-five experimental laboratory stud-
training subjects on easier problems (tasks). ies have obtained similar results with a wide
Presumably the harder goals led to greater variety of tasks: Bavelas (1978), with a fig-
effort than the easier goals, and training on ure-selection task; Bavelas and Lee (1978)
the harder chess problems led to the acqui- in five of six experiments involving brain-
sition of more skill and knowledge than storming, figure selection, and sum estima-
training on easier ones. tion tasks; Campbell and Ilgen (1976) with
Although there has been extensive re- chess; Hannan (1975) with a coding (credit
search on the effects of goal specificity and applications) task; LaPorte and Nath (1976)
difficulty on performance, little attention has with prose learning; Latham and Saari
been paid to two other dimensions of goal (1979b) with brainstorming; Locke and
content: goal complexity (the number and Bryan (1969b) with simple addition; Locke,
interrelation of the results aimed for) and Cartledge, and Knerr (1970) in four studies,
goal conflict (the degree to which attaining three with reaction time and one with simple
one goal negates or subverts attaining an- addition; Locke, Mento, and Katcher (1978)
other). with perceptual speed; London and Oldham
The second attribute of goals, intensity, (1976) with card sorting; Masters, Furman,
pertains to the process of setting the goal or and Barden (1977) in two studies of 4- and
of determining how to reach it. Intensity 5-year-old children working on a color dis-
would be measured by such factors as the crimination task; Mento, Cartledge, and
scope of the cognitive process, the degree of Locke (1980) in two experiments using a
effort required, the importance of the goal, perceptual speed task; Rothkopf and Bil-
and the context in which it is set. Goal in- lington (1975) and Rothkopf and Kaplan
tensity may be related to goal content; for (1972) in more complex prose-learning stud-
example, a more intense psychological pro- ies than that of LaPorte and Nath (1976);
cess is needed to set complex goals and to and Sales (1970), using anagrams. In Sales's
128 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

study, task rather than goal difficulty was even reached the level of the moderate goal,
manipulated by means of varying the work the hard goal may have been totally un-
load given to the subjects. Presumably sub- realistic.
jects developed implicit goals based on the The fifth negative study, by Motowidlo,
amount of work assigned to them. Ness and Loehr, and Dunnette (1978), using a com-
Patton (1979) also found that a harder task plex computation task, examined the goal
led to better weight-lifting performance than theory-expectancy theory controversy. Goal
an easier task when subjects were deceived theory predicts that harder goals lead to bet-
as to the actual weights. ter performance than easy goals, despite
Four studies found conditional2 support their lower probability of being fully reached.
for a positive relation between goal difficulty In contrast, expectancy theory predicts (other
and performance. Becker (1978) with an things being equal) a positive relation be-
energy conservation task, Erez (1977) with tween expectancy and performance, the op-
a clerical task, and Strang, Lawrence, and posite of the goal theory prediction. Motow-
Fowler (1978) with a computation task, all idlo et al. found a positive relation between
found that only subjects who had high goals expectancy and performance, which is in
and who received feedback regarding their agreement with expectancy theory. One pos-
performance in relation to those goals per- sible confounding factor is that the subjects
formed better than subjects with low goals. in the Motowidlo et al. study did not make
This pattern of results seems also to have their expectancy ratings conditional upon
been present in Frost and Mahoney's (1976) trying their hardest to reach the goal or to
first study using a reading task (see their win (pointed out by Mento et al., 1980,
Table 1, p. 339). Subjects with high and based on Yates & Kulick, 1977, among oth-
moderately high goals who apparently re- ers). Thus, low expectancy ratings could
ceived frequent feedback performed better mean that a subject was not planning to ex-
than those with average goals, whereas the ert maximum effort, whereas high ratings
opposite pattern was obtained for subjects would mean the opposite. This would yield
given no feedback during the 42-minute a spurious positive correlation between ex-
work period (interaction p = . 11; / tests were pectancy and performance. Furthermore,
not performed). Motowidlo et al. did not provide their sub-
Six experimental laboratory studies found jects with feedback regarding how close they
no relation between goal level and task per- were coming to their goals during task per-
formance. Bavelas and Lee (1978) allowed formance. (The importance of this factor is
only 15 minutes for an addition task and documented below.) The two studies by
gave subjects no information either before Mento et al. (1980), which avoided the er-
or during the task of how fast they needed rors of the Motowidlo et al. study and in-
to go to attain the goal. Frost and Mahoney corporated other methodological improve-
(1976) found negative results with a jigsaw ments, found the usual positive relation
puzzle task, although their range of goal between goal level and performance and no
difficulty was limited: from medium to hard relation between expectancy and perfor-
to very hard (actual probabilities of success mance.
were .50, .135, and .026, respectively). The Forward and Zander (1971) used goals
same narrow range of difficulty (very diffi- set by groups of high school boys on a team-
cult to moderately difficult) may explain the coding task as both independent and depen-
negative results of Oldham (1975) using a dent variables. Success and failure as well
time sheet computation task. Moreover, not as outside pressures were covertly manipu-
all subjects accepted the assigned goals in
that study, and it is not clear that ability was 2
Partially or conditionally supportive studies were
controlled when Oldham (1975, pp. 471- distinguished from nonsupportive studies as follows: A
472) did his post hoc analysis by personal study was called partially supportive if the treatment
was significant for one subsample of the full sample of
goal level. Organ (1977) also compared subjects or for one of several experimental treatments
moderate goals with hard goals using an ana- or criteria. If an entire sample or study found no sig-
gram task. However, since no group average nificant effects, it was called nonsupportive.
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 129

lated to influence goal setting, which oc- Negative results were obtained by For-
curred before each trial of the task. Under ward and Zander (1971) with United Fund
these somewhat complex conditions, goal campaign workers, Hall and Foster (1977)
discrepancy (goal minus previous perfor- with participants in a simulated manage-
mance level) was either unrelated or nega- ment game, and Steers (1975) with first-
tively related to subsequent performance. level supervisors.
The results of the experimental studies All the correlational studies are, of course,
were, to varying degrees, supported by the open to multiple causal interpretations. For
results of 15 correlational studies. Andrews example, Dossett et al. (1979) implied that
and Farris (1972) found that time pressure their results may be an artifact of ability,
was associated with high performance among since ability was considered when setting
scientists and engineers. Hall and Lawler goals in the participative groups but not in
(1971), with a similar sample, found no re- the assigned groups. In fact, none of the cor-
lation between time pressure and perfor- relational studies had controls for ability.
mance but found a significant relation be- Also, many relied on self-ratings of goal dif-
tween both quality and financial pressure ficulty or performance. The Yukl and La-
(implied goals?) and work performance. tham (1978) study found that only objective
Ashworth and Mobley (Note 2) found a sig- goal level, not subjective goal difficulty, was
nificant relation between performance goal related to typing performance. None of the
level and training performance for Marine correlational studies measured the individ-
recruits. Blurnenfeld and Leidy (1969), in ual's personal goal level, a measure that
what also could be called a natural field ex- Mento et al. (1980) found to be the single
periment, found that soft-drink servicemen best motivational predictor of performance.
who were assigned higher goals serviced Their measures of subjective goal difficulty
more machines than those assigned lower did not explain any variance in performance
goals. Hamner and Harnett (1974) found over and above that explained by objective
that subjects in an experimental study of and personal goal levels.
bargaining who expected (tried?) to earn a
high amount of money earned more than Goal Specificity
those who expected (tried?) to earn less
money. Locke et al. (1970), in the last of Specific hard goals versus "do best" goals
their five studies, found a significant corre- or no goals. Previous research found that
lation between grade goals on an hourly specific, challenging (difficult) goals led to
exam and actual grade earned. higher output than vague goals such as "do
The majority of the correlational studies your best" (Locke, 1968). Subsequent re-
found only a conditional positive relation search has strongly supported these results,
between goal difficulty and performance although in a number of studies, no distinc-
and/or effort. Carroll and Tosi (1970) found tion was made between groups told to do
a positive relation only for managers who their best and those assigned no specific
were mature and high in self-assurance; goals. The latter were typically labeled no
Dachler and Mobley (1973) found it only goal groups. We have not found any differ-
for production workers (in two plants) with ences in the results obtained by studies in
long tenure (1 or 2 years or more); Dossett, which no goals are assigned and those in
Latham, and Mitchell (1979), found it in which subjects are explicitly told to do their
two studies of clerical personnel, but only for best. No goal subjects, it appears, typically
those who set goals participatively; Hall and try to do as well as they can on the assigned
Hall (1976) found it for the class perfor- task.
mance of second through fourth grade stu- Twenty-four field experiments all found
dents in high-support schools; and Ivancev- that individuals given specific, challenging
ich and McMahon (1977a, 1977b, 1977c) goals either outperformed those trying to do
found it for skilled technicians who had their best or surpassed their own previous
higher order (growth) need strength, were performance when they were not trying for
white, and had higher levels of education. specific goals: Bandura and Simon (1977)
130 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

with dieting; Dockstader (Note 3) with key and LaPorte and Nath (1976) with prose
punching; Dossett et al. (1979) in two stud- learning; Latham and Saari (1979a) with
ies, one using a clerical test and the other brainstorming; Latham and Saari (1979b)
performance evaluations for clerical work- with brainstorming again, but only for sub-
ers; Ivancevich (1977) with maintenance jects who set goals participatively (though
technicians; Ivancevich (1974) in two plants this may have been an artifact since the au-
with marketing and production workers (for thors reported that the assigned goal subjects
one or more performance criteria); Ivancev- may not have understood the instructions
ich (1976) with sales personnel; Kim and clearly); Locke and Bryan (1969a) with a
Hamner (1976) with telephone service jobs; driving task; Locke et al. (1978) with per-
Kolb and Boyatzis (1970) with personality ceptual speed (comparing the hard-goal vs.
change in a T-group; Latham and Baldes do-best groups only); Mossholder (1980)
(1975) with truck loading; Latham and using two assembly tasks; Organ (1977) with
Kinne (1974) with logging; and Latham and anagrams; Pritchard and Curtis (1973) with
Yukl (1975a) with woods workers who par- card sorting; Reynolds, Standiford, and An-
ticipated in goal setting; Latham and Yukl derson (1979) with learning prose; Rosswork
(1976) with typing; Latham, Mitchell, and (1977) with a sentence construction task
Dossett (1978) with engineering and scien- used with sixth graders; Rothkopf and Bil-
tific work; Migliore (1977) with canning lington (1975) and Rothkopf and Kaplan
(press department) and ship loading (two (1972), again with learning prose; Strang,
studies); Nemeroff and Cosentino (1979) Lawrence, and Fowler (1978) with arith-
with performance appraisal activities; Um- metic computation (but only for hard-goal
stot, Bell, and Mitchell (1976) with coding subjects who had feedback); and Terborg
land parcels; Wexley and Nemeroff (1975) and Miller (1978) with tinker-toy assembly.
with managerial training; and White, Mitch- A negative result was obtained by Organ
ell, and Bell (1977) with card sorting. The (1977) on a proofreading task. Evidently the
studies by Adam (1975) with die casters, goals set were moderate rather than hard,
Feeney with customer service workers ("At since they were set at the median scores for
Emery Air Freight," 1973), and Komaki, pretest subjects and were surpassed by sub-
Barwick, and Scott (1978) with pastry work- jects in all conditions. Moderate goals are
ers are also included in this group. Although not predicted to lead to higher performance
these investigations claimed that they were than do-best goals. Locke et al. (1978), for
doing behavior modification, the major tech- example, found that although hard-goal sub-
nique actually used was goal setting plus jects exceeded the performance of do-best
feedback regarding goal attainment (Locke, subjects, moderate-goal subjects did not.
1977). Seven correlational field studies also sup-
A negative result was obtained by Latham ported or partially supported the superiority
and Yukl (1975a) with one sample of woods of specific hard goals over do-best goals or
workers. Either individual differences or no goals: Blumenfeld and Leidy (1969) with
lack of organizational support may have soft drink servicemen; Brass and Oldham
been responsible for this failure. (Ivancevich, (1976) and Oldham (1976) with foremen;
1974, also cited differences in organizational Burke and Wilcox (1969) with telephone
support as the reason for obtaining better operators; Ronan, Latham, and Kinne (1973)
results in one of his plants than the other.) with pulpwood producers; Steers (1975)
The generally positive results of the field with supervisors (but only those high on need
studies were supported by the results of 20 for achievement); and Terborg (1976) with
laboratory studies: Chung and Vickery (1976; students studying programmed texts.
their KR condition included implicit goal Clear versus unclear goals or intentions.
setting) with a clerical task; Frost and Ma- Relatively few studies have been concerned
honey (1976) with a reading task (but only with the effect of goal clarity on perfor-
for subjects given frequent feedback) and mance. Two experimental studies (Kaplan
with a puzzle task; Hannan (1975) with a & Rothkopf, 1974; Rothkopf & Kaplan,
coding task; Kaplan and Rothkopf (1974) 1972) found that specific prose-learning
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 131

goals led to more learning than generally variety of tasks and situations. Thus, con-
stated goals. Carroll and Tosi (1970) found siderable confidence can be placed in them
that goal clarity correlated with increased in terms of both internal and external val-
effort only for managers who were mature idity.
and decisive and who had low job interest
and low support from their managers. Ivan-
cevich and McMahon (1977a, 1977b, 1977c) Mechanisms for Goal-Setting Effects
found that goal clarity correlated with per-
Given that goal setting works, it is relevant
formance mainly for technicians who were to ask how it affects task performance. We
black, less educated, and high on higher or-
view goal setting primarily as a motivational
der need strength. These correlational stud-
mechanism (although cognitive elements are
ies seem to provide no consistent pattern,
which is not surprising in view of the prob- necessarily involved). The concept of moti-
vation is used to explain the direction, am-
lems inherent in concurrent, self-report de-
plitude (effort), and duration (persistence)
signs.
of action. Not surprisingly, all three are af-
The borderline and negative results of
fected by goal setting. One additional, in-
Hall and Hall (1976) and Hall and Foster
(1977) with respect to goal difficulty and direct mechanism is also described.
performance may have been because their
goals did not consist of clear objectives but Direction
of the self-rated strength of the subjects' in-
tentions to perform well. Most fundamentally, goals direct atten-
The findings of these studies involving tion and action. Perhaps the most obvious
vague intentions can be contrasted with the demonstration of this mechanism is the
organizational studies by H. Miller, Kater- study by Locke and Bryan (1969a) in which
berg, and Hulin (1979), Mobley, Homer, drivers were given feedback regarding five
and Hollingsworth (1978), and Mobley, different dimensions of driving performance
Hand, Baker, and Meglino (1979). They but were assigned goals with respect to only
found significant longitudinal correlations one dimension. The dimension for which a
between the specific intention to remain in goal was assigned showed significantly more
or leave the organization and the corre- improvement than the remaining dimen-
sponding action. sions. Similarly, Locke et al. (1970) found
that subjects modified their speed of reaction
Conclusions (to make it faster or slower) on a simple
reaction-time task in the direction of their
Overall, 48 studies partly or wholly sup- overall objective. Reynolds et al. (1979)
ported the hypothesis that hard goals lead found that subjects spent more time reading
to better performance than medium or easy prose passages that were relevant to their
goals; 9 studies failed to support it. Fifty-one goals (consisting of questions inserted in the
studies partially or wholly supported the text) than to reading parts that were not
view that specific hard goals lead to better relevant. Terborg (1976) found that subjects
performance than do-your-best or no goals; with specific goals spent a greater percentage
2 studies did not support it. Combining these of the time looking at the text material to
two sets of studies, we found that 99 out of be learned than did subjects with nonspecific
110 studies found that specific, hard goals goals or no goals. (Terborg labeled this mea-
produced better performance than medium, sure effort in his study.) Rothkopf and Bil-
easy, do-your-best, or no goals. This repre- lington (1979) found that subjects with spe-
sents a success rate of 90%. cific learning goals, as compared with
Most of these studies (at least the exper- subjects with no specific learning goals (do-
imental ones) were well designed; they in- your-best instructions), spent an equal or
cluded control groups, random assignment, greater amount of time inspecting passages
negligible attrition, controls for ability, ob- with goal-relevant material and significantly
jective performance measures, and a great less time looking at incidental passages.
132 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

Effort limits typically have been imposed; field


studies to date have measured only the end
Since different goals may require different results of goal setting rather than how they
amounts of effort, effort is mobilized simul- were obtained. LaPorte and Nath (1976)
taneously with direction in proportion to the allowed some subjects unlimited time to read
perceived requirements of the goal or task. a prose passage. Those asked to read the
Thus, as Kahneman (1973) and Shapira passage to get 90% of 20 postreading ques-
(Note 1) have argued, more effort is ex- tions correct spent more time on the passage
pended on hard tasks (which are accepted) than subjects asked to get 25% of the post-
than on easy tasks. Sales (1970) found that reading questions correct. Rothkopf and Bil-
higher work loads produce higher subjective lington (1979) found that more time was
effort, faster heart rates, and higher output spent on goal-relevant than on incidental
per unit time than lower work loads. Latham passages. More studies of this type would be
and Locke (1975) and Bassett (1979) found highly desirable.
that people work faster under shorter than
under longer time limits. In summary, higher
goals produce higher performance than lower Strategy Development
goals or no goals because people simply work Whereas the first three mechanisms are
harder for the former (Locke, 1968; Ter- relatively direct in their effects, this last
borg, 1976; Terborg & Miller, 1978; for ear- mechanism is indirect. It involves developing
lier documentation see Locke & Bryan, strategies or action plans for attaining one's
1966). goals. Although strategy development is
This hypothesis of a positive linear rela- motivated by goals, the mechanism itself is
tion between motivation or effort and per- cognitive in essence; it involves skill devel-
formance (also stated in Locke, 1968, and opment or creative problem solving.
Yates & Kulick 1977), contradicts the Bandura and Simon (1977), for example,
Yerkes-Dodson inverted-U "law," which as- found that dieting subjects with specific quo-
serts that performance is maximal at mod- tas for number of mouthfuls eaten changed
erate levels of motivation. Although it is true their eating patterns (e.g., by eating more
that with any given subject, performance low-calorie foods that did not count in their
eventually will level off as the limit of ca- quotas). They also engaged in more planning
pacity or ability is reached (Bavelas & Lee, (e.g., by saving part of their quota for a din-
1978; Kahneman, 1973), this is a separate ner out). Latham and Baldes (1975) ob-
issue from that of motivation. Of course, served that some of the truck drivers as-
subjects may abandon their goals if they signed specific hard goals with respect to
become too difficult, but the hypothesized truck weight recommended minor modifi-
function assumes goal commitment. Perfor- cations of their trucks to help them increase
mance may also drop if subjects become the accuracy of their judgments of weight.
highly anxious, especially on a complex or In Terborg's (1976) study, the subjects
underlearned task. But a state of high anx- who set specific goals were more likely to
iety should not be labeled high motivation employ relevant learning strategies (e.g.,
in the positive sense because it represents a writing notes in the margins) than those who
state of conflict rather than of single-minded did not set goals. A unique aspect of Ter-
goal pursuit. borg's (1976) design was that he was able
to obtain separate measures of direction of
Persistence effort (which he called "effort") and of strat-
egy use (which he called "direction"). He
Persistence is nothing more than directed found that when these mechanisms were par-
effort extended over time; thus, it is a com- tialed out, there was no relation between
bination of the previous two mechanisms. goals and task performance. This supports
Most laboratory experiments on goal setting the argument that these are some of the
have not been designed to allow for the mea- mechanisms by which goals affect perfor-
surement of persistence effects, since time mance.
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 133

In a similar vein, Kolb and Boyatzis KR No KR


(1970) found that behavior change in a T-
Specific
group was greatest for participants who de- hard goal
1 2
veloped plans for evaluating their perfor-
mance in relation to their goals. Such plans No specific
evidently were developed only for behavior goal or do- 3 4
dimensions that the subjects were trying to best goal
change.
Figure 1. Model for analyzing goal-KR studies.
Bavelas and Lee (1978) made detailed (KR = knowledge of results.)
analyses in three experiments to determine
the strategies subjects used to attain hard
goals. They found that subjects would fre- setting activity (Locke, 1967; Locke &
quently redefine the task in a way that would Bryan, 1968, 1969a, 1969b; Locke, Car-
permit them to give "looser" or lower quality tledge, & Koeppel, 1968). In the most care-
answers. For example, subjects asked to list fully controlled of these studies, all subjects
very large numbers of "white, hard, edible with specific goals also received knowledge
objects" were more likely to list objects that of their performance in relation to their
were white but not very hard or hard but not goals; individuals in the KR conditions re-
very edible than were subjects given easier ceived knowledge of their actual scores pre-
goals. Similarly, with appropriate training, sented in such a way as to preclude their use
subjects given hard addition goals would in setting goals. Such knowledge of scores
more often estimate rather than calculate did not lead to better performance than no
their answers as compared to subjects with knowledge of scores. The evidence from
easy goals. these and related studies indicated that
Subjects given hard goals in Rosswork's knowledge of scores was not sufficient to im-
(1977) study simply wrote shorter sentences prove task performance. However, since
to meet their quota, which was expressed in groups with goals and no KR were not in-
terms of total sentences written. The subjects cluded, these studies did not test the possi-
in Sales's (1970) study who were given a bility that KR may be a necessary condition
high work load made more errors, presum- for goals to affect performance. Few studies
ably by lowering their standards, than those relevant to this hypothesis had been con-
given a low work load. Christensen-Szalan- ducted at the time of the Latham and Yukl
ski (1980) found that subjects who were (1975b) review.
given a short time limit in problem solving
used less complex and less adequate strate- A number of such studies have since been
gies than subjects given a longer time limit. completed in both the laboratory and the
Strategy development is especially impor- field. Figure 1 illustrates the conditions of
tant in complex tasks. If the requisite strat- interest. Cell 1 represents specific, hard goals
egies are not developed, the increased mo- combined with KR; Cell 2, specific, hard
tivation provided by the goals will not be goals without KR; Cell 3, KR with no spe-
translated into effective performance. cific goals (or do-best goals that are equiv-
We now examine the influence of feed- alent to no assigned goals); and Cell 4, nei-
back, money, and participation on the ef- ther specific goals nor KR.
fectiveness of goal setting. The studies reviewed here included at
least three of the four cells in Figure 1. Table
1 summarizes the results of these compari-
Knowledge of Results (Feedback) sons.
In early goal-setting studies, attempts Two types of studies are evident in Table
were made to separate the effects of feed- 1. The first set consists of comparisons be-
back (i.e., knowledge of results [KR]) from tween Cells 1, 3, and 4. Consistent with
the effects of goal setting to determine Locke's (1968) mediating hypothesis, these
whether KR directly influenced performance studies indicate that although KR alone is
or whether its effects were mediated by goal- not sufficient to improve performance (3 =
134 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

Table 1
Studies Comparing the Effects of Goals and KR on Performance
Comparison performed

Study I vs. 2 1 vs. 3 2 vs. 4 3 vs. 4

Bandura & Simon (1977) 1 > 3 3=4


Dockstader (Note 3) 1 > 3 3=4
Latham, Mitchell, & Dossett (1978) 1 > 3 3=4
Nemeroff & Cosentino (1979) 1 > 3 3=4
"At Emery Air Freight" (1973) 1 > 2 2=4
Komaki, Barwick, & Scott (1978) 1 > 2 2=4
Becker (1978)" 1 > 2 2=4
Strang, Lawrence, & Fowler (1978) 1 > 2 2=4
2 <4b
/Vo(e. KR = knowledge of results. 1 = specific, hard goals combined with KR; 2 = specific, hard goals without
KR; 3 = KR with no specific goals (or do-best goals); 4 = neither specific goals nor KR.
" Included both hard and easy goal plus KR conditions. The performance of easy-goal subjects was no better than
that in the control condition, b Results differed, depending on performance criterion utilized.

4), KR plus goals results in performance in- improve subsequent performance. Those su-
creases (1 > 3). pervisors who set specific goals in response
In a study of overweight clients in a weight to the feedback performed significantly bet-
clinic, participants who kept daily records ter on the 12 behaviors for which they set
of all the food they consumed but did not goals and conducted significantly more suc-
set goals to reduce food intake did not alter cessful appraisal interviews.
their eating habits and performed no differ- This first set of studies demonstrates that
ently than a control group who kept no rec- KR without goals is not sufficient to improve
ords and set no specific goals (Bandura & performance (3 = 4), but given KR, goals
Simon, 1977). However, participants who are sufficient for performance to be im-
set goals based on their daily records sig- proved (1 > 3). Thus, goals seem necessary
nificantly decreased food consumption com- for KR to improve performance.
pared with the KR-only group. The second set of studies consists of com-
Dockstader (Note 3) found no apparent parisons between Cells 1, 2, and 4. In what
effect of KR alone on the performance of was called a "positive reinforcement" pro-
key punch operators, but those provided with gram ("At Emery Air Freight," 1973), em-
KR and a performance standard signifi- ployees in the customer service department
cantly exceeded their own previous perfor- and on the shipping docks were given a
mance and that of the KR-only group. group-performance goal, progress toward
Latham et al. (1978) found no differences the goal was posted, and each employee also
between engineers and scientists with do- kept a personal record of performance. Per-
best goals who were provided with feedback formance levels increased markedly, but
concerning their performance on certain ap- when KR was removed and self-reports were
praisal criteria and those who received no not kept, employee performance returned to
feedback; however, the subjects who set or baseline levels "or was almost as bad" ("At
were assigned specific, hard goals in response Emery Air Freight," 1973, p. 45), even
to the feedback performed significantly bet- though the performance target remained in
ter than those in the do-best and control effect (1 > 2, 2 = 4).
groups. In another behavior modification program
Nemeroff and Cosentino (1979) found (actually a goals and KR study; see Locke,
that supervisors who were provided with 1980), Komaki, Barwick, and Scott (1978)
feedback concerning their behavior during examined safe behavior in the making and
performance appraisal sessions but who did wrapping of pastry products. The authors
not use the KR to set specific goals did not introduced a specific, hard safety goal and
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 135

displayed performance results on a graph in Goal X KR interaction. When all four vari-
view of all the workers. Substantial perfor- ables were placed in the regression simul-
mance improvements occurred, but when the taneously, the interaction effect was
KR was eliminated in a reversal phase, per- significant, but beta weights for goals and
formance returned to baseline levels. KR were not significantly different from
In a study of residential electricity use, zero. The goal-performance correlation in
Becker (1978) manipulated specific goals the KR group was .60 and in the no-KR
and KR. Families included in his study rep- group, .01. These findings led Erez to con-
resent Cells 1, 2, and 4 of Figure 1; he also clude that KR is necessary for goals to affect
included easy-goal groups with and without performance.
KR. The only families whose conservation Kim and Hamner's (1976) study of goals
performance improved significantly from and feedback was not included in this anal-
baseline levels were those with hard goals ysis because they acknowledged that their
plus KR. All other groups performed no bet- goals-only group actually may have received
ter than a control group. Strang et al. (1978) informal feedback. Thus, their study only
conducted a laboratory study utilizing a de- includes two cells: Cell 1, with different
sign similar to Becker's (Cells 1, 2, and 4 groups having different amounts and types
plus the same two easy-goal conditions as of feedback, and Cell 4, which comprised the
above). Subjects worked on an arithmetic "before" scores of the various groups. In this
computation task. The performance of sub- study, as in the one by Frost and Mahoney
jects with hard goals and feedback was sig- (1976, Task A), providing explicit or fre-
nificantly better than that of the goals-only quent feedback clearly facilitated perfor-
subjects (1 > 2). Using time to finish as a mance.
criterion, there were no differences between Integrating the two sets of studies points
the performance of the goals-only subjects to one unequivocal conclusion: neither KR
and that of control group subjects (2 = 4). alone nor goals alone is sufficient to improve
In terms of number of errors, however, the performance. Both are necessary. This view
control group's performance was signifi- of goals and feedback as reciprocally depen-
cantly better than that of the goals-only dent seems more useful and more accurate
group (4 > 2), suggesting that goals without than Locke's (1968) earlier position, which
KR may even inhibit accurate performance. viewed goals as mediating the effects of feed-
The results of this second group of studies back on performance. Together, goals and
indicate that goals without KR are not suf- feedback appear sufficient to improve task
ficient to improve performance (2 = 4), but performance (given the obvious contextual
given goals, KR is sufficient to effect per- variables such as adequate ability and lack
formance improvement (1 > 2). Thus, KR of external blocks to performance). The
seems necessary for goals to affect perfor- studies demonstrate that action is regulated
mance. by both cognition (knowledge) and motiva-
Although her study is not included in Ta- tion.
ble 1 because she used a correlational anal- Table 1 demonstrates that not a single
ysis, Erez (1977) was the first to suggest that study was designed to allow all of the four
KR is a necessary condition for the goal- possible comparisons. In other words, no
performance relation. In her laboratory study involved a complete 2 X 2 design with
study, subjects worked on a number com- KR/no-KR and specific, hard goals/"do-
parison task. At the end of one performance best" goals, or no goals as the variables. Even
trial, they set goals for a second trial. Half the studies reported did not always involve
of the subjects were provided with KR at the total control over the variables; for example,
end of the first trial and half were not. Erez spontaneous goal setting among KR-only
used a multiple regression analysis to iden- subjects was not always prevented. Such a
tify the unique contribution of the Goal X complete, controlled study is now being con-
KR interaction. The regression equation in- ducted by two of the present authors. It is
cluded Stage 1 performance, the two main predicted that Cell 1 (see Figure 1) will show
effects variables (goals, KR), and the better performance than the remaining cells,
136 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

which should not differ among themselves. McCaleb, Shaw, and Denny (1980), for ex-
This would parallel the results of Becker ample, found that individual money incen-
(1978) and Strang et al. (1978) using KR/ tives increased worker performance by a
no-KR and hard/easy goal conditions. median of 30%. Locke (1968) argued that
Other issues remain to be explored re- goal setting may be one mechanism by which
garding the role of KR. For example, Cum- money affects task performance.
mings, Schwab, and Rosen (1971) found There are several possible ways that this
might occur. First, money could affect the
that providing KR can lead to the setting of
level at which goals are set or the level at
higher goals than not providing KR; this in-
dicates that subjects may underestimate which intentions are established. In five stud-
their capacity without correct informationies, Locke, Bryan, and Kendall (1968) found
about their previous performance. Related that in some cases, money did affect goal or
intention level. Furthermore, in line with the
to this, Greller (1980) found that supervisors
incorrectly estimated the importance of var-
mediating hypothesis, goals and intentions
ious sources of feedback to subordinates. affected performance even when the effects
These issues deserve further study. of incentives were partialed out, whereas in-
One issue that does not seem to deservecentives were unrelated to performance when
goal and intention level were controlled.
further study is that of feedback as a rein-
forcer. The findings and arguments of An- Generally these results have not been rep-
nett (1969), Bandura (1977), and Locke licated. For example, Pritchard and Curtis
(1973) found that although there was no
(1977, 1980) speak convincingly against the
thesis that feedback conditions behavior. It
difference in the performance effects of no
seems more useful and valid to treat feed-incentive versus a small incentive, subjects
back or KR as information, the effect of who were offered high incentives performed
better on a sorting task than those offered
which depends on how it is processed (e.g.,
see Locke, Cartledge, & Koeppel, 1968). small or no incentives even when goal level
was controlled. Similarly, Terborg (1976)
A recent article (Ilgen, Fisher, & Taylor,
1979) specifies several dimensions along found that partialing out the effects of self-
which KR can vary: amount, type, fre- set goals in a programmed learning task
failed to vitiate the difference between con-
quency, specificity, timing, source, sign, and
recency. Experimental studies of these di-tingent and noncontingent pay on perfor-
mensions could reveal the most effective mance. Terborg and Miller (1978) found
form in which to provide KR in conjunctionsimilar results using a toy assembly task,
assigned goals, and piece-rate versus hourly
with goals. Unfortunately, the studies to date
have not been systematic enough to allow pay. Latham et al. (1978) found a significant
any conclusions about these dimensions. main effect for an anticipated monetary bo-
Our major conclusion, that both goals and
nus independent of a significant goal-level
KR are necessary to improve performance, effect on the job performance of engineers
provides a clear prescription for task man-
and scientists. In all four of these studies,
agement. Not only should specific, hard goals and money had independent effects on
performance. This was also the case in Lon-
goals be established, but KR should be pro-
vided to show performance in relation to don and Oldham's (1976) study, although
these goals. The "At Emery Air Freight" their incentive effects were not easily inter-
(1973), Komaki et al. (1978), Latham and pretable. Chung and Vickery (1976) also
Kinne (1974), and Latham and Baldes found independent effects for money and
(1975) studies emphasize how inexpensive goals (their KR condition included a goal-
such goals-plus-KR programs can be in field
setting treatment).
settings relative to their benefits. A second possibility is that money might
induce more spontaneous goal setting than
Monetary Rewards would occur without incentives. In support
of this hypothesis, Saari and Latham (Note
It is known that money can be a powerful 4) found that the introduction of an incentive
motivator of performance. Locke, Feren, system led mountain beaver trappers to set
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 137

specific goals for themselves. However, in the the likelihood of spontaneous goal setting or
laboratory studies by Terborg (1976) and of setting high goals, but there has not yet
Terborg and Miller (1978), incentive pay did been enough research to provide support for
not lead to more specific goal setting than these mechanisms.
hourly pay.
A third possibility, which was stressed by Participation and Supportiveness
Locke (1968), is that rather than increasing
the likelihood of spontaneous goal setting or Participation has long been recommended
increasing the level at which goals are set by social scientists as a means of obtaining
(an hypothesis that has not yet been fully employee commitment to organizational
tested), incentives affect the individual's de- goals and of reducing resistance to change.
gree of goal commitment. In other words, Nevertheless, an extensive review of the par-
offering money may arouse the willingness ticipation in decision-making literature by
to expend more effort to attain a given ob- Locke and Schweiger (1979) found no con-
jective than not offering money. In terms of sistent difference in the effectiveness of top-
expectancy theory, money rewards endow down ("autocratic") decision making and
goal success with a higher valence or value decisions made with subordinate participa-
than no money. This is our interpretation of tion. We specifically review those studies
the results obtained by Latham et al. (1978), that involved participation in goal setting.
London and Oldham (1976), Pritchard and Carroll and Tosi (1970) included a mea-
Curtis (1973), Terborg (1976), and Terborg sure of perceived participation in goal setting
and Miller (1978). in a questionnaire administered at a manu-
Attempts to measure this commitment facturing firm that had a Management by
effect through self reports have not been suc- Objectives program. The results indicated
cessful (e.g., Latham et al., 1978; Pritchard that participation did not correlate signifi-
& Curtis, 1973). The whole issue of why cantly with employee perceptions of goal
goal commitment measures have not been attainment or employee perception of in-
related to performance in goal-setting re- creases in effort.
search will be discussed at length in a later Negative results were also obtained in a
section of this article. field experiment by Ivancevich (1976). This
The effectiveness of money in mobilizing study compared participative and assigned
effort undoubtedly depends on the amount goal setting for sales personnel. Goals were
of money offered. Pritchard and Curtis set for each of four quantitative performance
(1973) found an incentive effect only when criteria. Although both goal-setting groups
they offered $3 compared with 50$ or no showed performance increases, no signifi-
money at all for 10 minutes of work. Simi- cant differences in performance were found
larly, Rosswork (1977) found a substantial between the participative and assigned goal
goal effect but no incentive effect when conditions.
school children were offered up to 6$ for In a second study, Ivancevich (1977) ob-
each sentence composed during two 5-min- tained mixed results with maintenance de-
ute periods. partment technicians. Four performance
The findings indicate that money can af- variables were measured. With regard to
fect task performance independently of goal service complaints and costs, the assigned
level. The most plausible mechanism for this goal-setting group showed more improve-
effect appears to be goal commitment, with ment than the participative group; however,
the degree of increased commitment de- for safety the participative goal group per-
pending on the amount of the incentive of- formed better than the assigned group.
fered. Although direct questions regarding There was no significant difference between
commitment used in several studies do not the two groups in absenteeism.
support this interpretation, the fault may lie A possible drawback of these studies is
in poor experimental design, poor measures, that goal difficulty levels were not assessed
or poor introspection by subjects (issues we for the different goal groups. Conceivably,
discuss later). Incentives may also increase goal difficulty could have been confounded
138 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

with the assigned versus participative ma- and performance appraisal. In the first ex-
nipulations. periment employees who participated in set-
The following studies all included mea- ting their goals on a test attained the same
surements of goal difficulty. In a field ex- performance level as individuals who were
periment involving logging crews, Latham assigned goals of the same difficulty level.
and Yukl (1975a) found that participative This same finding was obtained in a second
goal setting resulted in higher performance study, which involved setting goals on a per-
than assigned goal setting for uneducated formance appraisal form.
(less than 9 years of education) loggers in Hannan (1975), using a simulated credit
the South. The superiority of participative application evaluation task, also found that
goal setting may have been due in part to assigned and participatively set goals led to
the higher goals that were set in the partic- the same level of performance when goal
ipative rather than the assigned condition. level was controlled. (There was a small
In a second field experiment, Latham and Goal X Participation interaction, however.)
Yukl (1976) found no significant differences Likert (Note 5) has pointed out that when
in the performance of typists with partici- assigned goal setting is effective, it may be
pative and assigned goals. Consistent with because the supervisors who assign the goals
these results, there was no difference in the behave in a supportive manner. Latham and
difficulty levels of the goals in each condi- Saari (1979b) tested this assumption in a
tion. Both groups, however, improved their second laboratory study using a brainstorm-
performance significantly after specific goals ing task. Goal difficulty again was held con-
had been set. stant between the participative and assigned
Latham et al. (1978) found that engineers goal groups. However, the supportiveness of
and scientists in a participative goal condi- the experimenter was varied. The results in-
tion set more difficult goals than their peers dicated that a supportive supervisory style
who had assigned goals. However, the per- led to higher goals being set than a nonsup-
ceptions of goal difficulty did not differ, and portive style. It was also found that it took
no significant differences in goal acceptance significantly longer to set goals in the par-
were found between the two goal conditions. ticipative goal conditions than in the as-
The participative and assigned groups did signed conditions because the subjects asked
not differ significantly in performance, al- more questions regarding what answers were
though only the participative group signifi- acceptable. Latham and Saari (1979b) con-
cantly outperformed the control group. cluded that the importance of participation
These three studies indicate that partici- in goal setting may be that it not only leads
pation in goal setting may affect perfor- to the setting of high goals but it can also
mance through its influence on goal diffi- lead to increased understanding of how to
culty. Thus, if goal difficulty is held constant, attain themtwo variables that can have a
participation should not affect performance. direct impact on performance.
Participation may affect performance only Although few consistent differences in
if it leads to higher goals being set than is task performance appear between assigned
the case when a supervisor assigns them uni- and participatively set goal groups, several
laterally. tentative conclusions regarding the influence
Latham and Saari (1979a) systematically of participation can be drawn. There appear
tested this hypothesis in a laboratory study to be two possible mechanisms by which par-
using a brainstorming task. Goal difficulty ticipation could affect task motivation. First,
levels were held constant across the partic- it can lead to the setting of higher goals than
ipative and assigned goal conditions. As pre- would be the case without participation, al-
dicted, no significant differences in perfor- though theoretically, assigned goals can be
mance were found between the two goal set at any level the supervisor or experi-
setting groups. Moreover, no difference on menter chooses. Second, participation could,
a measure of goal acceptance was found. in some cases, lead to greater goal accep-
Dossett et al. (1979) replicated this find- tance or commitment than assigned goals.
ing in two field experiments involving testing The first effect has been found twice (La-
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 139

tham et al., 1978; Latham & Yukl, 1975a). were significantly related to performance
(We discuss the second effect in the section only for less educated technicians (fewer
on goal acceptance.) than 12 years of education).
It may be that supportiveness, as discussed In their field experiment with loggers,
in studies by Latham and Saari (1979b), Latham and Yukl (1975a) compared as-
Hall and Hall (1976), and Ivancevich (1974, signed, participative, and do-best goal-set-
who called it "reinforcement"), is more cru- ting conditions for educated white (12-16
cial than participation in achieving goal ac- years of education) and uneducated black
ceptance. Participation itself, of course, may (0-9 years of education) logging crews. Par-
entail supportiveness. Other factors, such as ticipative goal setting significantly affected
the power of the supervisors and the rewards the performance of the uneducated crews
and punishments given for goal attainment but did not affect the performance of the
and nonattainment, also may be important, educated crews. The goal-setting program
but these have not been systematically in- may not have been administered effectively
vestigated. in the latter sample, however; in addition,
Further, it is possible that the motiva- education was confounded with race.
tional effects of participation are not as im- These findings were not replicated in La-
portant in gaining performance improve- tham and Yukl's (1976) field experiment in-
ment as are its cognitive effects. Locke et volving female typists. In that study edu-
al. (1980) found that the single most suc- cation did not moderate the effects of either
cessful field experiment on participation to participative or assigned goal setting. Sim-
date stressed the cognitive benefits; partici- ilarly, Steers (1975) found no moderating
pation was used to get good ideas from work- effect of education on goal setting in a study
ers as to how to improve performance effi- of 113 female supervisors.
ciency (Bragg & Andrews, 1973). The Although Latham et al. (1978) did not
potential cognitive benefits of participation examine education as a moderator variable,
are discussed in some detail in Locke and we mention the study here because of the
Schweiger (1979) and were implied in the education level of the subjects: Goal setting
Latham and Saari (1979b) study. had a significant effect on the performance
of engineers and scientists with master's and
Individual Differences doctoral degrees.
We must conclude that there is no con-
Thus far we have been discussing goal set- sistent evidence for the effect of education
ting as though it affected every individual as a moderator of goal setting, nor is there
in the same manner. To date, individual dif- any convincing theoretical reason why there
ferences have received minimal attention in should be. Goal setting appears to be effec-
the goal-setting literature, although several tive for individuals of all educational levels,
variables have been examined in one or more ranging from elementary school children
studies. (Masters et al., 1977) to loggers with a mean
education of 7.2 years (Latham & Yukl,
Demographic Variables 1975a) to engineers and scientists (Latham
et al., 1978) with advanced degrees.
Of the few goal-setting studies that have Race, As already noted, Latham and
investigated demographic variables, most Yukl (1975a) found that less educated black
have dealt with the effects of education, loggers who participated in setting their
race, and job tenure. goals were more productive and attained
Education. In a study involving elec- their goals more frequently than crews who
tronics technicians, Ivancevich and Mc- were assigned goals by their supervisors or
Mahon (1977b) found that perceived goal told to do their best. However, for the more
challenge was significantly related to per- educated white loggers there were no sig-
formance only for educated technicians (12 nificant differences among the goal-setting
years or more of education). In contrast, conditions.
perceived goal clarity and goal feedback A study by Ivancevich and McMahon
140 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

(1977a) of technicians supported these find- goal setting, though goal setting has been
ings. Perceived participation in goal setting shown to significantly increase the perfor-
was related to several measures of perfor- mance of both males (e.g., Ivancevich &
mance for black technicians but not for McMahon, 1977b; Latham & Yukl, 1975a)
whites. Goal clarity and feedback were also and females (Latham & Yukl, 1976; Steers,
related to performance for blacks only, 1975).
whereas goal challenge was related to per-
formance for the whites only. Perhaps goal Personality Variables
clarity, feedback, and participation affected
the performance of blacks because, as Ivan- Need for achievement. Steers (1975), in
cevich and McMahon (1977a) stated, his study of female supervisors, found that
It has been found that blacks have a higher need for performance was related to feedback and
security in performing their jobs.. . . Oneway to derive goal specificity only for high-need-achieve-
more security in a goal setting program is to have goal ment individuals. Participation in goal set-
clarity, receive feedback, and participate in the process, ting, on the other hand, was related to per-
(p. 298)
formance only among low-need-achievement
Clearly more studies are needed before this supervisors. These findings indicate that high
interpretation can be verified. If it is valid, need achievers perform best when they are
then the racial factor would be reducible to assigned specific goals and receive feedback
a personality attribute that presumably would on their progress toward these goals. Con-
cut across racial lines. versely, low need achievers (who are perhaps
Job tenure. Five studies have examined less confident) perform best when they are
tenure as a moderator variable in the goal- allowed to participate in the setting of their
setting process. Three of them (Ivancevich goals.
& McMahon, 1977a; Latham & Yukl, In his study using anagrams, Sales (1970)
1976; Steers, 1975) found no moderating varied the work load given to subjects. Over-
effect. Two studies by Dachler and Mobley all, productivity for subjects high in need for
(1973), found no significant relation between achievement was not higher than that for
stated goals and productivity for short-ten- subjects low in need for achievement. How-
ure employees (less than 1-2 years), but a ever, an interaction occurred between work
significant relation between these measures load and need for achievement. Sales re-
for long-tenure employees (1-2 or more ported a positive linear relation between
years). Their explanation for this difference need for achievement and productivity in the
was that longer tenure employees have more underload condition and a curvilinear (in-
accurate perceptions of their chances of verted-U) relation between need for achieve-
reaching various levels of performance ment and productivity in the overload con-
and of performance-outcome contingencies. dition. Since high need achievers prefer goals
Nevertheless, it is not clear why it would of moderate difficulty, they presumably con-
take 1 or more years for these perceptions sidered the overload condition too challeng-
to become accurate. In sum, the evidence to ing for their liking.
date does not show much promise with re- In a laboratory experiment, Singh (1972)
spect to job tenure as a moderator. found that students with high need for
Age. In the study by Ivancevich and achievement set higher goals for themselves
McMahon (1977b) on technicians, age was over repeated trials of a mathematical cler-
not related to goal setting or performance. ical task than did low need achievers. Yukl
To our knowledge no other studies have in- and Latham (1978) obtained comparable
vestigated the moderating effects of age. results in their study involving typists. High
However, as previously noted, goal setting need achievers who were allowed to partic-
has been shown to be effective for children ipate in the goal-setting process set more
(e.g., Masters et al, 1977; Rosswork, 1977) difficult goals than did low-need-achieve-
as well as adults. ment typists, though they did not perform
Sex. No study has systematically ex- any better than low need achievers.
amined sex differences as a moderator of In the two experiments involving word
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 141

processing operators, Dossett et al. (1979) fort (toward quality) and attendance. No
found no moderating effects of need for obvious interpretation can be made of this
achievement on performance appraisal mea- finding.
sures or on performance on a selection test Self-esteem. In the study involving typ-
measuring mathematical ability. Goal dif- ists (Latham & Yukl, 1976), self-esteem did
ficulty was not examined in these studies not moderate the effects of participative and
because it was held constant across goal-set- assigned goal setting on performance. How-
ting conditions, Overall, the results again are ever, it was found that self-esteem and goal
inconsistent and unreliable, instrumentality interacted in their effects on
Need for independence. An earlier study performance (Yukl & Latham, 1978). In-
by French, Kay, and Meyer (1966) found strumentality was defined as "the extent to
that employees with a high need for inde- which desirable outcomes (e.g., job security,
pendence had greater goal acceptance when pay, promotion) are perceived to be contin-
participation in goal setting was increased gent upon goal attainment" (Yukl & La-
than when participation was reduced or not tham, 1978, p. 312). Specifically, when goal
changed. Goal acceptance was not affected instrumentality was low (goal attainment
by changes in participation for employees not perceived as linked to important out-
with a low need for independence. comes), typists with high self-esteem showed
The moderating effect of need for inde- greater performance improvement than in-
pendence has not been found by other re- dividuals with low self-esteem. There was no
searchers. For example, Searfoss and Mon- self-esteem effect when instrumentality was
czka (1973) found no moderating effect of high. When self-esteem was low, typists who
need for independence on the relationship perceived high goal instrumentality showed
between perceived participation on the part greater performance improvement than those
of managers in setting specific budgetary with low goal instrumentality; when self-es-
goals and subsequent motivation to achieve teem was high, there was no instrumentality
those goals. Similarly, in their study with effect. The integrating principle here may
typists, Latham and Yukl (1976) found that be that people with high self-esteem will
need for independence did not moderate the work hard without practical rewards (for
effects of either participative or assigned pride?), whereas people with low self-esteem
goal setting on performance. Dossett et al. will not.
(1979) also found no moderating effects of Carroll and Tosi (1970) found in a cor-
need for independence on the performance relational study that individuals with high
of word processing operators. self-assurance increased effort in the face of
Higher order need strength. Higher or- increasingly difficult goals, whereas those
der need strength is defined as the degree to with low self-assurance worked less hard as
which a person desires enriched work (va- goals became harder. It is likely that differ-
riety, autonomy, task identity, and feedback; ent self-perceptions regarding ability under-
see Hackman & Lawler, 1971). To our lie the self-assurance measure.
knowledge, only one study has examined this Dossett et al. (1979) found that word pro-
need as a possible moderator of goal setting. cessing operators with high self-esteem who
In the study by Ivancevich and McMahon were given performance feedback attained
(1977c) involving technicians, initial anal- their goals significantly more often than in-
yses revealed no consistent relationships be- dividuals with low self-esteem. These results
tween various goal attributes and perfor- are consistent with those of Schrauger and
mance measures. However, when higher Rosenberg (1970), who found that shifts in
order need strength was used as a moderator, performance following feedback depend on
goal clarity, feedback, and challenge were the self-esteem of the individual. Specifi-
related to effort (toward quantity and qual- cally, high self-esteem people improved their
ity) and attendance for technicians with high performance more than low self-esteem peo-
higher order need strength. Conversely, for ple following positive feedback; the per-
technicians with low higher order need formance of low self-esteem individuals
strength, goal acceptance was related to ef- decreased more than high self-esteem
142 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

individuals following negative feedback. variables included in the studies were not
Thus, high self-esteem individuals are influ- based on any clear theoretical rationale;
enced more by positives, whereas low self- thus, even when differences were found, they
esteem people are influenced more by neg- were hard to explain. Perhaps the most the-
atives. oretically plausible of the variables discussed
These results are congruent with Kor- earlier is that of need for achievement. Need
man's (1970) thesis, which asserts that for achievement theory (e.g., McClelland
individuals are motivated to behave in a & Winter, 1971) would predict, for example,
manner which is congruent with their self- that people high in need for achievement
concept. Thus, people respond more to feed- would (a) choose moderate goals; and (b)
back that agrees with their self-concept, work hardest when probabilities of success
whether it is positive or negative, than they were moderate, when task performance was
do to feedback that is inconsistent with their in their control, when there was performance
self-concept. feedback, and when intrinsic rather than
Internal versus external control. In the extrinsic rewards were emphasized. Al-
study of typists (Latham & Yukl, 1976), though there is some support for these pre-
belief in internal versus external control was dictions in the need for achievement litera-
found to have no moderating effect on per- ture, goal-setting studies have not been
formance. Dossett et al. (1979) also found designed to test them.
no moderating effects for locus of control on The results for self-esteem are also in-
job performance appraisal measures or on triguing. This variable seems worthy of fur-
test performance for word processors. How- ther study, since it is logical to expect that
ever, Latham and Yukl (1976) found that one's self-concept would affect the goals one
typists with participatively set goals who chooses. Self-esteem, of course, must be
were "internals" set more difficult goals than carefully separated from ability.
"externals." Third, there are difficulties with regard
to the measures used for assessing person-
Conclusions ality variables. For example, the personality
measures used were not consistent across
The only consistent thing about the studies studies. Steers (1975) used the Gough-Heil-
of individual differences in goal setting is brun Adjective Check List (Gough & Heil-
their inconsistency. A number of reasons for brun, 1965) to measure need for achieve-
this can be offered. ment, whereas Latham and Yukl (1976)
First, the studies were not specifically de- modified a questionnaire developed by Her-
signed to look for individual difference ef- mans (1970). Therefore it cannot be deter-
fects. The very fact that most studies as- mined whether the different results obtained
signed goals to the subjects means that any in these two studies were due to differences
individual differences that did exist were in the measures or in the population. Fur-
probably masked by the demand character- ther, the reliability and validity of person-
istics of the design. When goals are assigned, ality measures are often inadequate or not
subjects typically respond to situational de- reported. In addition, some personality mea-
mands rather than act in accordance with sures were administered after the experi-
their own styles and preferences. The best mental manipulations had taken place. This
design for revealing individual differences procedure can result in a confounding of re-
would be one in which there is free (or a sponses to the personality measures with the
considerable amount of) goal choice rather experimental treatment.
than assigned goals. Note that the person- Fourth, there may be confounding of in-
ality variables in the goal-setting studies re- dividual differences in some studies. To draw
viewed previously were most likely to emerge firm conclusions regarding an individual dif-
in the participative conditions (where the ference variable, it must be independent of
subject has some input into the decision) or other individual difference variables of in-
in the self-set goal conditions. terest. Researchers often do not report the
Second, most of the individual difference intercorrelations of individual differences,
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 143

yet they draw conclusions on various indi- contrary to expectations. However, this dif-
vidual difference variables obtained from the ference disappeared by the end of the ex-
same sample. periment. Frost and Mahoney (1976), Lon-
Fifth, many studies report that an indi- don & Oldham (1976), Mento et al. (1980,
vidual difference variable correlates with two studies), Oldham (1975), and Yukl &
performance for people who score high on Latham (1978) found no relationship be-
that variable but not for those who score low. tween measures of goal acceptance and per-
However, generally no test of significance formance. Organ (1977) found that goal
between the two correlations is reported. To acceptance correlated with performance
establish a moderating effect, a test of sig- within some of his assigned goal subgroups,
nificant differences between correlation but the pattern of correlations was uninter-
coefficients should be made (Zedeck, 1971). pretable theoretically.
Future research must overcome these dif- There are several possible reasons for
ficulties before any clear conclusions can be these negative results. First, the measures of
drawn regarding the role of individual dif- goal acceptance (which consisted typically
ferences in goal setting. of direct, face-valid questions such as, "How
committed are you to attaining the goal?")
Goal Acceptance, Commitment, may not have been valid. Some evidence that
and Choice the measures of goal acceptance may be at
fault was obtained in a study by Hannan
Goal acceptance and commitment are (1975) in the credit application evaluation
similar though distinguishable concepts. Goal task noted earlier. He measured goal accep-
commitment implies a determination to try tance not by a rating scale but by the degree
for a goal (or to keep trying for a goal), but of difference between the subject's external
the source of the goal is not specified. It (i.e., assigned or participatively agreed upon)
could be an assigned goal or a participatively goal and his or her personal goal (as deter-
set goal or a goal that one set on one's own. mined from a questionnaire given after ex-
Goal acceptance implies that one has agreed ternal goals were set). Hannan found that
to commit oneself to a goal assigned or sug- participation did lead to greater goal accep-
gested by another person. Both acceptance tance (though it had no main effect on per-
and commitment presumably can exist in formance) than assigning goals and that the
varying degrees. Since most studies have effects of participation became progressively
used assigned goals, the two concepts can stronger as the difficulty of the external goal
often be used interchangeably. increased. The goal acceptance measure was
Most recent studies of goal setting have related to one measure of performance. Han-
used goals as an independent variable. How- nan also found that personal goals predicted
ever, since it is assumed that assigned goals performance better than assigned goals, as
must be accepted before they will affect task did Mento et al. (1980). These findings sug-
performance, it is also relevant to examine gest that indirect measures of goal accep-
the determinants of goal commitment or ac- tance may be more valid than direct mea-
ceptance. Generally, attempts to measure sures.
degree of goal commitment in a manner that Second, in most of the studies where ac-
will differentiate between experimental ceptance was measured, nearly all subjects
treatments and/or relate to task perfor- showed complete or substantial goal com-
mance have failed. None of the experimental mitment; thus the range of scores was quite
conditions in the studies by Latham and limited. Small differences on the scales typ-
Saari (1979a, 1979b), Latham et al. (1978), ically used may not reflect genuine differ-
Yukl and Latham (1978), or Dossett et al.'s ences in psychological states,
Study 1 (1979) affected self-report measures Third, due to limitations in introspective
of goal acceptance. Dossett et al.'s (1979) ability, most (untrained) subjects may not
Study 2 found an initial difference, with as- be able to discriminate small differences in
signed goals showing greater acceptance psychological commitment (see Nisbett &
than participatively set goals, a prediction Wilson, 1977; but see also Lieberman, 1979,
144 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

for a more sanguine view of the usefulness gories, which are the main components of
of introspection). Recall that in the studies expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964).
by Latham et al. (1978) and Pritchard and
Curtis (1973) described earlier, there ap- Expectations of Success
peared to be significant commitment effects
Other things being equal, individuals are
for monetary incentives based on actual per- more likely to accept or choose a given goal
formance, but these were not reflected in the
when they have high rather than low expec-
direct goal commitment questions. tations of reaching it (Mento et al., 1980).
The solution to the last two problems may Such expectations evidently stem from self-
be to modify the design of the typical goal- perceptions about ability on the task in ques-
setting experiment. Designs that encourage
tion (Mento et al., 1980). Presumably these
a wide range of goal commitment, such as perceptions are inferences from past perfor-
those with a choice of various possible goals, mance. Past performance has consistently
with commitment to each being measured
been found to predict future goals (Cum-
after choice, may reduce the introspective mings et al., 1971; Lopes, 1976; Wilsted
burden and increase the variance of the an- & Hand, 1974; Ash worth & Mobley, Note
swers on the commitment scale. Within-sub-
2). Individuals are more likely to become
ject designs, which involve assigning differ- more confident and to set higher goals after
ent goals (under different conditions) to the
success and to become less confident and to
same subjects at different times, might also set lower goals after failure (Lewin, 1958),
make the commitment responses more ac-
although failure may lead to higher goals in
curate by providing a clearer frame of ref- pressure situations (Forward & Zander,
erence for the subject. In addition, when a
subject is less than fully committed to a 1971; Zander, Forward, & Albert, 1969) or
even due to self-induced pressure (Hilgard,
given goal, it is important to determine what
other goals he or she is committed to. For 1958). Generalized self-confidence may also
affect goal acceptance and choice.
example, a subject who is not fully commit-
ted to a moderately difficult goal could be
trying for a harder goal, an easier goal, or Values
no specific goal. Each alternative choice When the perceived value of attaining or
would have different implications for per- trying for a goal is higher, the goal is more
formance. likely to be accepted than when the perceived
Different degrees of goal commitment value is low (Mento et al., 1980). The valued
might be induced by varying types or degrees outcomes involved may range from intrinsic
of social influence (e.g., approval, disap- rewards like the pleasure of achievement to
proval). Such influences undoubtedly have extrinsic rewards following performance,
profound effects on goal choice and com- such as money, recognition, and promotion.
mitment among certain individuals, but a Instrumentality in expectancy theory is the
detailed discussion of the socialpsycholog- belief that goal acceptance or goal attain-
ical literature is outside the scope of this re- ment will lead to value attainment. Theo-
view. retically, goal choice and goal acceptance
Goal acceptance or commitment can be should be predictable from the expectancies,
considered a form of choice, (i.e., the choice values, and instrumentalities the subject
between accepting or rejecting a goal that holds with regard to the various choices
was assigned or set participatively). In this (Dachler & Mobley, 1973).
sense these studies tie in with the more tra- This is clearly a maximization-of-satisfac-
ditional studies of what is called "level of tion model, which is not without its critics
aspiration," which allowed subjects to freely (e.g., Locke, 1975). Nevertheless, treating
choose their own goals after each of a series expectancy theory concepts as factors that
of trials on a task (e.g., see Frank, 1941; predict an individual's goal choices does sug-
Hilgard, 1958). The factors that affect goal gest a way of integrating the expectancy and
acceptance and goal choice are basically the goal-setting literatures (Dachler & Mobley,
same. They fit easily into two major cate- 1973; Mento et al., 1980).
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 145

Although external factors such as rewards Furthermore, these effects are found just as
and pressures presumably affect the individ- reliably in field settings as in the laboratory.
ual through their effects on expectancies, 2. There are at least four mechanisms by
instrumentalities, and values, it is worth em- which goals affect task performance: by di-
phasizing pressures because they have played recting attention and action, mobilizing en-
a major role in most of the goal-setting stud- ergy expenditure or effort, prolonging effort
ies. For example, the typical laboratory over time (persistence), and motivating the
goal-setting study simply involves asking the individual to develop relevant strategies for
subject to try to reach a certain goal. The goal attainment. The latter two mechanisms
subject typically complies because of the are most in need of further study.
demand characteristics of the experiment 3. Goals are most likely to affect perfor-
(probably reducible to beliefs regarding the mance under the following conditions:
value of extra credit and the desire to help Range and type of goals. Individuals
the experimenter). with specific and hard or challenging goals
Similarly, in field settings subjects are typ- outperform individuals with specific easy
ically asked to try for goals by their super- goals, do-best goals, or no assigned goals.
visor. The supervisor, of course, is in a po- People with specific moderate goals show
sition to reward or punish the employee; performance levels between those of people
furthermore, employees know they are being with easy and hard goals but may not per-
paid to do what the organization asks them form better than individuals with do-best
to do. Ronan, Latham, and Kinne (1973) goals. A common problem with easy-goal
found that goal setting among woods work- subjects is that their goals are so easy that
ers was only effective when the supervisor once they are reached, they set new, higher
stayed on the job with the employees. The goals to have something to do, which means
mere presence of the supervisor could be that they are no longer genuine easy-goal
considered a form of pressure in this context. subjects. Perhaps easy-goal subjects should
In the studies by Forward and Zander be told not to try to exceed their goals or not
(1971) and Zander et al. (1969), competitive to set new goals when the easy goals are
or community pressures led to setting goals reached.
that were unrealistically high. The wider the range of goal difficulty, the
Although pressure is something that social more likely goal setting is to affect perfor-
scientists generally have been against, Hall mance (cf., Frost & Mahoney, 1976, with
and Lawler (1971) argued that if used ap- Locke et al., 1978). It is probable that longer
propriately (e.g., by combining it with re- time spans will progressively increase the
sponsibility), it can facilitate both high com- difference between subjects with hard goals
mitment and high performance. Pressure, of and those without hard goals.
course, also can be self-imposed as in the One aspect of goal setting that has not
case of the Type A personality who appears received much attention to date is the use-
to be a compulsive goal achiever (Friedman fulness of setting intermediate goals or
& Rosenman, 1974). subgoals as an aid to attaining longer term
or end goals. Locke and Bryan (1967) found
that on a 2-hour addition task, setting 15-
Summary, Conclusions, and Directions for minute subgoals led to slightly poorer per-
Future Research formance than setting just end goals. Ban-
dura and Simon (1977), however, found that
Based on the findings to date, the follow- setting weekly goals for weight loss only led
ing conclusions about goal setting seem war- to weight loss when daily goals (or multiple
ranted: goals within days) were set as well. There
1. The beneficial effect of goal setting on is probably an optimal time span for the set-
task performance is one of the most robust ting of goals depending on both the individ-
and replicable findings in the psychological ual and the task situation. Subgoals could
literature. Ninety percent of the studies conceivably facilitate performance by oper-
showed positive or partially positive effects. ating as a feedback device; they might also
146 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

serve to maintain effort over long time spans. raised by Ilgen et al. (1979), such as timing,
On the negative side, they might limit per- frequency, source, interpretation, and so on.
formance if the subgoals were treated as Monetary rewards. Money may be an
performance ceilings. More studies are effective method of improving performance
clearly needed on this topic. in relation to a given goal (presumably
Goal specificity. Goals seem to regulate through increased commitment), but the
performance most predictably when they are amounts involved must be large rather than
expressed in specific quantitative terms (or small (e.g., $3 rather than 3 in a typical
as specific intentions to take a certain action, laboratory experiment).
such as quitting a job) rather than as vague Further research on money and goal set-
intentions to "try hard" or as subjective es- ting could be tied into Deci's work on in-
timates of task or goal difficulty. trinsic and extrinsic motivation. Deci and
Ability. Individuals must have the ability Porac (1978) suggested that money rewards
to attain or at least approach their goals. that encourage the attainment of compe-
(In complex tasks they must choose appro- tence on a task (reaching a challenging
priate strategies, as noted previously.) Ex- goal?) may enhance rather than decrease
erting more effort will not improve task per- interest in the task.
formance if improvement is totally beyond Participation and supportiveness. There
the individual's capacity. Goal-setting stud- is no consistent evidence that participation
ies should carefully control for ability (such in setting goals leads to greater goal com-
as by a work sample pretest) to isolate the mitment or better task performance than
variance in performance due to goals from assigned goals when goal level is controlled,
that due to ability. If ability is not controlled, though it sometimes leads to setting higher
it becomes error variance when testing for goals than the supervisor would have as-
a motivation effect. The most practical way signed. One study found that participation
to set goals may be to base them on each facilitated the acceptance of hard goals
individual's ability on the task in question (Hannan, 1975).
as measured by a preexperimental work Supportiveness may be more important
sample. This usually insures ready goal ac- than participation, although this concept
ceptance and makes it easy to control for needs to be defined more clearly. Latham
ability when comparing different goals. and Saari (1979b) defined it as friendliness,
Knowledge of results (feedback). Knowl- listening to subjects' opinions about the goal,
edge of performance in relation to the goal encouraging questions, and asking rather
appears to be necessary if goals are to im- than telling the subject what to do. More
prove performance, just as goals are neces- exploration of the nature and effects of sup-
sary if feedback is to improve performance. portiveness in goal setting is clearly war-
Feedback is probably most helpful as an ad- ranted.
junct to goal setting when the task is divided Individual differences. No reliable in-
into trials and feedback is provided after dividual difference factors (other than abil-
each one, although the ideal frequency is not ity) have emerged in the goal-setting liter-
known. Feedforward, telling the subjects ature, probably because most of the studies
how fast they will need to work on a future have used assigned goals. Thus, situational
trial as compared with their speed on an im- constraints have prevented personal styles
mediately preceding trial may be a partial and preferences from affecting performance.
substitute in some cases (e.g., see Mento et In free-choice situations individual person-
al., 1980, Study 1). Knowledge and feed- ality traits may play a more substantial role.
back, of course, may have purely cognitive Subjects high in need for achievement should
(learning) effects on performance (see Locke prefer to set moderate goals, whereas those
et al., 1968, for a discussion of this issue), low in this motive should be more likely to
but these are not the concern of this review. set easy or very hard goals. Individuals with
Clearly more research is needed on feed- high self-esteem should be more likely to
back, especially research based on the issues accept and try for challenging goals than
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 147

those with low self-esteem. However, it is not goal-setting effectiveness. Obviously, indi-
clear whether a generalized self-esteem mea- viduals must have some control over task
sure would show as great an effect as a more pace, quality, method, and so on for goal
task-specific measure of perceived compe- setting, or any other motivational technique,
tence. Mento et al. (1980; based on Motow- to affect performance. We do not agree with
idlo, 1976) found that self-perception of those who claim that goal setting might work
ability added unique variance to perfor- only on certain types of tasks. However, it
mance even when expectancy, valence, and will undoubtedly be the case that the four
goal level were controlled. mechanisms noted earlier are differentially
Goal acceptance and choice. A basic as- important in different tasks. For example,
sumption of goal setting research is that the where more effort leads to immediate re-
individual accepts (is actually trying for) the sults, goals may work as long as they lead
goal that was assigned or was set. Personal the subject to work harder. On the other
goals usually predict performance better hand, where the task is complex, hard goals
than related measures such as assigned (or may only improve performance if they lead
objective) goal difficulty or subjective goal to effective strategies.
difficulty. Direct measures of goal accep- Regarding the relation of goals to re-
tance have been found to be generally un- wards, an intriguing finding by Masters et
related to either experimental treatments or al. (1977) was that children who were told
task performance. For example, rewards to evaluate their performance after each trial
such as money may affect performance, with block while speaking into a tape recorder
goal difficulty controlled, even though goal (e.g., "I did very good [sic];" "I didn't do
acceptance questions do not indicate in- very good [j/c]") all reached assymptote on
creased commitment. Indirect measures, such the task regardless of their assigned goals.
as the difference between the personal and Self-reward ultimately vitiated what had
the assigned goal, show more promise. How- been highly significant goal effects. This
ever, better experimental designs (e.g., finding is clearly worthy of future study.
within-subject designs and designs allowing Competition in relation to goal setting also
free choice of goals) may show effects even requires further study. Both Latham and
using direct questions. Baldes (1975) and Komaki et al. (1978)
Goal choice and acceptance are influenced found that goal setting plus feedback led to
by numerous factors, including pressure, all spontaneous competition among subjects.
of which may work through influencing the White et al. (1977) found that telling sub-
individual's expectancies, values, and per- jects that their performance would be com-
ceived instrumentalities. Support on the part pared to that of others ("evaluation appre-
of higher management for goal-setting pro- hension," in their terminology) had a
grams in organizations seems critical for powerful effect on task performance inde-
their success, as is the case for most social pendent of a separate goal manipulation.
science interventions (e.g., see Hinrichs, However, spontaneous goal setting within
1978; Ivancevich, 1974; Woodward, Koss, the evaluation apprehension condition was
& Hatry, Note 6). In an organizational con- not measured. It is likely that competition
text support may include insuring or secur- could lead people to set higher goals than
ing the commitment of middle and lower they would otherwise (other people's perfor-
managers. It is likely that the degree of con- mances become the goals) and/or lead to
tinuing support for goal-setting programs greater goal commitment (Locke, 1968).
will determine the duration of their effects. Another issue that has not been investi-
The Latham and Baldes (1975) study with gated is whether hard goals combined with
truck drivers has continued to be successful high pressure might lead to a conflict situ-
for the past 7 years (reported in Latham ation and therefore high anxiety. It has been
& Locke, 1979, Figure 1, Footnote b). shown that anxiety disrupts performance on
Other issues. Not mentioned in the above complex tasks when it leads subjects to
discussion was how the type of task affects worry rather than concentrate on the task
148 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

(Wine, 1971). As noted earlier, conflicts formance targeting in local government.' An exam-
may also occur among different goals, al- ination of current usage, impacts, and implemen-
tation factors. Washington, D.C.: Urban Institute,
though this has not been studied. Conflicting 1978.
pressures in goal setting may vitiate the
usual goal-performance relationship (For-
ward & Zander, 1971). Nor has the issue References
of individual versus group goal setting re-
ceived much attention. (Group goals are dis-
cussed in Zander, 1971.) Adam, E. E. Behavior modification in quality control.
A final note is in order with respect to the Academy of Management Journal, 1975, 18, 662-
679.
practical significance of the technique of Andrews, F. M., & Farris, G. F. Time pressure and
goal setting. In a review of all available ex- performance of scientists and engineers: A five-year
perimental field studies of goal setting, Locke panel study. Organizational Behavior and Human
et al. (1980) found that the median improve- Performance, 1972, S, 185-200.
Annett, J. Feedback and human behaviour. Baltimore,
ment in performance (e.g., productivity, Md.: Penguin Books, 1969.
quality) that resulted from goal setting was At Emery Air Freight: Positive reinforcement boosts
16%. In one company the use of goal setting performance. Organizational Dynamics, 1973, 7(3),
on just one job saved a company $250,000 41-50.
(Latham & Baldes, 1975). Combined with Bandura, A. Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs,
N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
the use of monetary incentives, Locke et al. Bandura, A., & Simon, K. M. The role of proximal
(1980) found that goal setting improved per- intentions in self-regulation of refractory behavior.
formance by a median of more than 40% Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1977, ;, 177-193.
a finding of great practical significance. Bassett, G. A. A study of the effects of task goal and
schedule choice on work performance. Organizational
A model for the use of goal setting in field Behavior and Human Performance, 1979, 24, 202-
settings has been developed by Latham and 227.
Locke (1979). White and Locke (in press) Bavelas, J. B. Systems analysis of dyadic interaction:
have documented the high frequency with Prediction from individual parameters. Behavioral
which goals actually regulate productivity Science, 1978,23, 177-186.
Bavelas, J. B., & Lee, E. S. Effects of goal level on
in business settings. Locke (1978) has ar- performance: A trade-off of quantity and quality.
gued that goal setting is recognized explicitly Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1978, 32(4), 219-
or implicitly in virtually every theory of and 240.
approach to work motivation. Becker, L. J. Joint effect of feedback and goal setting
on performance: A field study of residential energy
conservation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1978,
63, 428-433.
Blumenfeld, W. S., & Leidy, T. R. Effectiveness of goal
Reference Notes setting as a management device: Research note. Psy-
chological Reports, 1969, 24, 752.
Bragg, J. E., & Andrews, I. R. Participative decision-
1. Shapira, Z. Goal difficulty and goal selling as de- making: An experimental study in a hospital. Journal
terminants of task motivation. Unpublished manu- of Applied Behavioral Science, 1973, 9, 727-735.
script, Hebrew University, 1977. Brass, D. J., & Oldham, G. R. Validating an in-basket
2. Ashworth, D. N., & Mobley, W. H. Relationships test using an alternative set of leadership scoring di-
among organizational entry performance goals, sub- mensions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1976, 61,
sequent goals, and performance in a military setting 652-657.
(Tech. Rep. TR-6). Columbia: Center for Manage- Burke, R. J., & Wilcox, D. S. Characteristics of effec-
ment and Organizational Research, University of tive employee performance review and development
South Carolina, July 1978. interviews. Personnel Psychology, 1969, 22, 291-305.
3. Dockstader, S. L. Performance standards and im- Campbell, D. J., & Ilgen, D. R. Additive effects of task
plicit goal setting: Field testing Locke's assumption. difficulty and goal setting on subsequent task perfor-
Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psy- mance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1976, 61,
chological Association, San Francisco, August 1977. 319-324.
4. Saari, L. M., & Latham, G. P. Hypotheses on rein- Carroll, S. J., Jr., & Tosi, H. L. Goal characteristics
forcing properties of incentives contingent upon per- and personality factors in a management-by-objec-
formance. Unpublished manuscript, University of tives program. Administrative Science Quarterly,
Washington, 1980. 1970, 75, 295-305.
5. Likert, R. Personal communication, August 1977. Christensen-Szalanski, J. J. J. A further examination
6. Woodward, J. P., Koss, M. P., & Hatry, H. P. Per- of the selection of problem-solving strategies: The ef-
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 149

fects of deadlines and analytic aptitudes. Organiza- performance. American Scientist, 1971, 59(1), 64-
tional Behavior and Human Performance, 1980, 25, 73.
107-122. Hamner, W. C, & Harnett, D. L. Goal-setting, per-
Chung, K. H., & Vickery, W. D. Relative effectiveness formance and satisfaction in an interdependent task.
and joint effects of three selected reinforcements in Organizational Behavior and Human Performance,
a repetitive task situation. Organizational Behavior 1974,72, 217-230.
and Human Performance, 1976, 16, 114-142. Hannan, R. L. The effects of participation in goal set-
Cummings, L. L., Schwab, D. P., & Rosen, M. Per- ting on goal acceptance and performance: A labo-
formance and knowledge of results as determinants ratory experiment. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
of goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1971, University of Maryland, 1975.
55, 526-530. Hermans, H. J. M. A questionnaire measure of achieve-
Dachler, H. P., & Mobley, W. H. Construct validation ment motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology,
of an instrumentality-expectancy-task-goal model of 1970, 54, 353-363.
work motivation: Some theoretical boundary condi- Hilgard, E. R. Success in relation to level of aspiration.
tions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1973, 58, 397- In C. L. Stacey & M. F. DeMartino (Eds.), Under-
418. (Monograph) standing human motivation. Cleveland, Ohio: How-
Deci, E. L., & Porac, J. Cognitive evaluation theory and ard Allen, 1958.
the study of human motivation. In M. R. Lepper & Hinrichs, J. R. Practical management for productivity.
D. Greene (Eds.), The hidden costs of reward. Hills- New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1978.
dale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1978. Ilgen, D. R., Fisher, C. D., & Taylor, M. S. Conse-
Dossett, D. L., Latham, G. P., & Mitchell, T. R. The quences of individual feedback on behavior in orga-
effects of assigned versus participatively set goals, nizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1979, 64,
KR, and individual differences when goal difficulty 349-371.
is held constant. Journal of Applied Psychology, Ivancevich, J. M. Changes in performance in a man-
1979, 64, 291-298. agement by objectives program. Administrative Sci-
Erez, M. Feedback: A necessary condition for the goal ence Quarterly, 1974, 19, 563-574.
setting-performance relationship. Journal of Applied Ivancevich, J. M. Effects of goal setting on performance
Psychology, 1977, 62, 624-627. and job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology,
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. Belief, attitude, intention, and 1976, 61, 605-612.
behavior: An introduction to theory and research, Ivancevich, J. M. Different goal setting treatments and
Reading, Mass: Addison-Wesley, 1975. their effects on performance and job satisfaction.
Forward, J., & Zander, A. Choice of unattainable group Academy of Management Journal, 1977, 20, 406-
goals and effects on performance. Organizational 419.
Behavior and Human Performance, 1971, 6, 184- Ivancevich, J. M., & McMahon, J. T. Black-white dif-
199. ferences in a goal-setting program. Organizational
Frank, J. D. Recent studies of the level of aspiration. Behavior and Human Performance, 1977, 20, 287-
Psychological Bulletin, 1941, 38, 218-226. 300. (a)
French, J. R. P., Kay, E., & Meyer, H. H. Participation Ivancevich, J. M., & McMahon, J. T. Education as a
and the appraisal system. Human Relations, 1966, moderator of goal setting effectiveness. Journal of
19, 3-20. Vocational Behavior, 1977, 11, 83-94. (b)
Friedman, M., & Rosenman, R. H. Type A behavior Ivancevich, J. M., & McMahon, J. T. A study of task-
and your heart. New York: Knopf, 1974. goal attributes, higher order need strength, and per-
Frost, P. J., & Mahoney, T. A. Goal setting and the formance. Academy of Management Journal, 1977,
task process: I. An interactive influence on individual 20, 552-563. (c)
performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Kahneman, D. Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs,
Performance, 1976, 17, 328-350. N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973.
Gough, H. G., & Heilbrun, A. B. The Adjective Check- Kaplan, R., & Rothkopf, E. Z. Instructional objectives
list manual. Palo Alto, Calif.: Consulting Psycholo- as directions to learners: Effect of passage length and
gists Press, 1965. amount of objective-relevant content. Journal of Ed-
Greller, M. M. Evaluation of feedback sources as a func- ucational Psychology, 1974, 66, 448-456.
tion of role and organizational level. Journal of Ap- Kim, J. S., & Hamner, W. C. Effect of performance
plied Psychology, 1980, 65, 24-27. feedback and goal setting on productivity and satis-
Hackman, J. R., & Lawler, E. E. Employee reactions faction in an organizational setting. Journal of Ap-
to job characteristics. Journal of Applied Psychology, plied Psychology, 1976, 61, 48-57.
1971, 55, 259-286. (Monograph) Kolb, D. A., & Boyatzis, R. E. Goal-setting and self-
Hall, D. T., & Foster, L. W. A psychological success directed behavior change. Human Relations, 1970,
cycle and goal setting: Goals, performance, and at- 23, 439-457.
titudes. Academy of Management Journal, 1977, 20, Komaki, J., Barwick, K. D., & Scott, L. R. A behavioral
282-290. approach to occupational safety: Pinpointing and rein-
Hall, D. T., & Hall, F. S. The relationship between forcing safe performance in a food manufacturing
goals, performance, success, self-image, and involve- plant. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1978, 64, 434-
ment under different organizational climates. Journal 445.
of Vocational Behavior, 1976, 9, 267-278. Korman, A. K. Toward a hypothesis of work behavior.
Hall, D. T., & Lawler, E. E. Job pressures and research Journal of Applied Psychology, 1970, 54, 31-41.
150 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

LaPorte, R. E., & Nath, R. Role of performance goals tivation. Academy of Management Review, 1978, 3,
in prose learning. Journal of Educational Psychol- 594-601.
ogy, 1976,65, 260-264. Locke, E. A. Latham versus Komaki: A tale of two
Latham, G. P., & Baldes, J. J. The "practical signifi- paradigms. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1980, 65,
cance" of Locke's theory of goal setting. Journal of 16-23.
Applied Psychology, 1975,<50, 122-124. Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. F. Cognitive aspects of psy-
Latham, G. P., & Kinne, S. B., III. Improving job per- chomotor performance: The effects of performance
formance through training in goal setting. Journal of goals on level of performance. Journal of Applied
Applied Psychology, 1914,59, 187-191. Psychology, 1966, 50, 286-291.
Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. Increasing productivity Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. F. Performance goals as de-
with decreasing time limits: A field replication of Par- terminants of level of performance and boredom.
kinson's law. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1975, Journal of Applied Psychology, 1967,57, 120-130.
60, 524-526. Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. F. Goal-setting as a deter-
Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. Goal-setting: A moti- minant of the effect of knowledge of score on perfor-
vational technique that works. Organizational Dy- mance. American Journal of Psychology, 1968, 81,
namics, 1979,5(2), 68-80. 398-406.
Latham, G. P., Mitchell, T. R., & Dossett, D. L. Im- Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. F. The directing function of
portance of participative goal setting and anticipated goals in task performance. Organizational Behavior
rewards on goal difficulty and job performance. Jour- and Human Performance, 1969, 4, 35-42. (a)
nal of Applied Psychology, 1978,65, 163-171. Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. F. Knowledge of score and
Latham, G. P., & Saari, L. M. The effects of holding goal level as determinants of work rate. Journal of
goal difficulty constant on assigned and participa- Applied Psychology, 1969, 53, 59-65. (b)
tively set goals. Academy of Management Journal, Locke, E. A., Bryan, J. F., & Kendall, L. M. Goals and
1979, 22, 163-168. (a) intentions as mediators of the effects of monetary in-
Latham, G. P., & Saari, L. M. Importance of supportive centives on behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology,
relationships in goal setting. Journal of Applied Psy- 1968, 52, 104-121.
chology, 1979, 64, 151-156. (b) Locke, E. A., Cartledge, N., & Knerr, C. S. Studies of
Latham, G. P., & Yukl, G. A. Assigned versus partic- the relationship between satisfaction, goal setting, and
ipative goal setting with educated and uneducated performance. Organizational Behavior and Human
woods workers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1975, Performance, 1970, 5, 135-158.
60, 299-302. (a) Locke, E. A., Cartledge, N., & Koeppel, J. Motivational
Latham, G. P., & Yukl, G. A. A review of research on effects of knowledge of results: A goal-setting phe-
the application of goal setting in organizations. Acad- nomenon? Psychological Bulletin, 1968, 70, 474-485.
emy of Management Journal, 1975, 18, 824-845. Locke, E. A., Feren, D. B., McCaleb, V. M., Shaw,
(b) K. N., & Denny, A. T. The relative effectiveness of
Latham, G. P., & Yukl, G. A. Effects of assigned and four methods of motivating employee performance.
participative goal setting on performance and job sat- In K. Duncan, M. Gruneberg, & D. Wallis (Eds.),
isfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1976, 61, Changes in working life. New York: Wiley, 1980.
166-171. Locke, E. A., Mento, A. J., & Katcher, B. L. The in-
Lewin, K. Psychology of success and failure. In C. L. teraction of ability and motivation in performance:
Stacey & M. F. DeMartino (Eds.), Understanding An exploration of the meaning of moderators. Per-
human motivation. Cleveland, Ohio: Howard Allen, sonnel Psychology, 1978, 31, 269-280.
1958. Locke, E. A., & Schweiger, D. M. Participation in de-
Lieberman, D. A. Behaviorism and the mind: A (lim- cision-making: One more look. In B. M. Staw (Ed.),
ited) call for a return to introspection. American Psy- Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 1). Green-
chologist, 1979, 34, 319-333. wich, Conn.: JAI Press, 1979.
Locke, E. A. Motivational effects of knowledge of re- London, M., & Oldham, G. R. Effects of varying goal
sults: Knowledge or goal setting? Journal of Applied types and incentive systems on performance and sat-
Psychology, 1967, 51, 324-329. isfaction. Academy of Management Journal, 1976,
Locke, E. A. Toward a theory of task motivation and 19, 537-546.
incentives. Organizational Behavior and Human Per- Lopes, L. L. Individual strategies in goal-setting. Or-
formance, 1968, 3, 157-189. ganizational Behavior and Human Performance,
Locke, E. A. Purpose without consciousness: A contra- 1976, 15, 268-277.
diction. Psychological Reports, 1969, 25, 991-1009. Masters, J. C., Furman, W., & Barden, R. C. Effects
Locke, E. A. Critical analysis of the concept of causality of achievement standards, tangible rewards, and self-
in behavioristic psychology. Psychological Reports, dispensed achievement evaluations on children's task
1912,31, 175-197. mastery. Child Development, 1977, 48, 217-224.
Locke, E. A. Personnel attitudes and motivation. Annual McClelland, D. C., & Winter, D. G. Motivating eco-
Review of Psychology, 1975, 26, 457-480. nomic achievement. New York: Free Press, 1971.
Locke, E. A. The myths of behavior mod in organiza- Meichenbaum, D. Cognitive-behavior modification. New
tions. Academy of Management Review, 1977, 2, York: Plenum Press, 1977.
543-553. Mento, A. J., Cartledge, N. D., & Locke, E. A. Mary-
Locke, E. A. The ubiquity of the technique of goal set- land vs. Michigan vs. Minnesota: Another look at the
ting in theories of and approaches to employee mo- relationship of expectancy and goal difficulty to task
GOAL SETTING AND TASK PERFORMANCE 151

performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Ronan, W. W., Latham, G. P., & Kinne, S. B., III.
Performance, 1980, 25, 419-440. Effects of goal setting and supervision on worker be-
Migliore, R. H. MBO: Blue collar to top executive. havior in an industrial situation. Journal of Applied
Washington, D.C.: Bureau of National Affairs, 1977. Psychology, 1973, 58, 302-307.
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. Plans Rosswork, S. G. Goal setting: The effects on an aca-
and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, 1960. demic task with varying magnitudes of incentive.
Miller, H. E., Katerberg, R., & Hulin, C. L. Evaluation Journal of Educational Psychology, 1977, 69, 710-
of the Mobley, Homer, and Hollingsworth model of 715.
employee turnover. Journal of Applied Psychology, Rothkopf, E. Z., & Billington, M. J. A two-factor model
1979, 64, 509-517. of the effect of goal-descriptive directions on learning
Mobley, W. H., Homer, S. O., & Hollingsworth, A. T. from text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1975,
An evaluation of precursors of hospital employee turn- 67, 692-704.
over. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1978, 63, 408- Rothkopf, E. Z., & Billington, M. J. Goal-guided learn-
414. ing from text: Inferring a descriptive processing model
Mobley, W. H., Hand, H. H., Baker, R. L., & Meglino, from inspection times and eye movements. Journal
B. M. Conceptual and empirical analysis of military of Educational Psychology, 1979, 71, 310-327.
recruit training attrition. Journal of Applied Psy- Rothkopf, E. Z., & Kaplan, R. Exploration of the effect
chology, 1919,64, 10-18. of density and specificity of instructional objectives
Mossholder, K. W. Effects of externally mediated goal on learning from text. Journal of Educational Psy-
setting on intrinsic motivation: A laboratory experi- chology, 1972, 63, 295-302.
ment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1980,65, 202- Ryan, T. A, Intentional behavior: An approach to hu-
210. man motivation. New York: Ronald Press, 1970.
Motowidlo, S. J. A laboratory study of the effects of Sales, S, M. Some effects of role overload and role un-
situational characteristics and individual differences derload. Organizational Behavior and Human Per-
on task success and motivation to perform a numer- formance, 1970, 5, 592-608.
ical task. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Univer- Schrauger, J. S., & Rosenberg, S. F. Self-esteem and
sity of Minnesota, 1976. the effects of success and failure feedback on perfor-
Motowidlo, S., Loehr, V., & Dunnette, M. D. A labo- mance. Journal of Personality, 1970, 38, 404-417.
ratory study of the effects of goal specificity on the Searfoss, D. G., & Monczka, R. M. Perceived partici-
relationship between probability of success and per- pation in the budget process and motivation to achieve
formance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1978, 63, the budget. Academy of Management Journal, 1973,
172-179. 16, 541-554.
Nemeroff, W. F., & Cosentino, J. Utilizing feedback Singh, J. P. Some personality moderators of the effects
and goal setting to increase performance appraisal of repeated success and failure on task-related vari-
interviewer skills of managers. Academy of Manage- ables. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University
ment Journal, 1979, 22, 566-576. of Akron, 1972.
Ness, R. G., & Patton, R. W. The effect of beliefs on Steers, R. M. Task-goal attributes, n achievement, and
maximum weight-lifting performance. Cognitive supervisory performance. Organizational Behavior
Therapy and Research, 1979, 3, 205-211. and Human Performance, 1975, 13, 392-403.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. Telling more than we Steers, R. M., & Porter, L. W. The role of task-goal
can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psy- attributes in employee performance. Psychological
chological Review, 1977, 84, 231-259. Bulletin, 1974, 81, 434-452.
Odiorne, G. S. MBO: A backward glance. Business Strang, H. R., Lawrence, E. C. & Fowler, P. C. Effects
Horizons, October 1978, pp. 14-24. of assigned goal level and knowledge of results on
Oldham, G. R. The impact of supervisory characteristics arithmetic computation: A laboratory study. Journal
on goal acceptance. Academy of Management Jour- of Applied Psychology, 1978, 63, 446-450.
nal, 1975, 18, 461-475. Taylor, F. W. The principles of scientific management.
Oldham, G. R. The motivational strategies used by su- New York: Norton, 1967. (Originally published,
pervisors: Relationships to effectiveness indicators. 1911.)
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, Terborg, J. R. The motivational components of goal set-
1976, 15, 66-86. ting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1976, 61, 613-
Organ, D. W. Intentional vs arousal effects of goal-set- 621.
ting. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor- Terborg, J. R., & Miller, H. E. Motivation, behavior,
mance, 1977, 18, 378-389. and performance: A closer examination of goal setting
Pritchard, R. D., & Curtis, M. I. The influence of goal and monetary incentives. Journal of Applied Psy-
setting and financial incentives on task performance. chology, 1978, 63, 29-39.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, Umstot, D. D., Bell, C. H., Jr., & Mitchell, T. R. Effects
1973, 10, 175-183. of job enrichment and task goals on satisfaction and
Rand, A. Introduction to objectivist epistemology. New productivity: Implications for job design. Journal of
York: The Objectivist, 1967. Applied Psychology, 1976, 61, 379-394.
Reynolds, R. E., Standiford, S. N., & Anderson, R. C. Vroom, V. Work and motivation. New York: Wiley,
Distribution of reading time when questions are asked 1964.
about a restricted category of text information. Jour- Wexley, K. N., & Nemeroff, W. F. Effectiveness of
nal of Educational Psychology, 1979, 77, 183-190. positive reinforcement and goal setting as methods of
152 LOCKE, SHAW, SAARI, AND LATHAM

management development. Journal of Applied Psy- ments. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor-
chology, 1975, 60, 446-450. mance, 1977, 20, 54-65.
White, F. M., & Locke, E. A. Perceived determinants Yukl, G. A., & Latham, G. P. Interrelationships among
of high and low productivity in three occupational employee participation, individual differences, goal
groups: A critical incident study. Journal of Man- difficulty, goal acceptance, goal instrumentality, and
agement Studies, in press. performance. Personnel Psychology, 1978, 31, 305-
White, S. E., Mitchell, T. R., & Bell, C. H., Jr. Goal 323.
setting, evaluation apprehension, and social cues as Zander, A. Motives and goals in groups. New York:
determinants of job performance and job satisfaction Academic Press, 1971.
in a simulated organization. Journal of Applied Psy- Zander, A., Forward, J., & Albert, R. Adaptation of
chology, 1977, 62, 665-673. board members to repeated failure or success by their
Wilsted, W. D., & Hand, H. H. Determinants of as- organization. Organizational Behavior and Human
piration levels in a simulated goal setting environment Performance, 1969, 4, 56-76.
of the firm. Academy of Management Journal, 1974, Zedeck, S. Problems with the use of "moderator" vari-
17, 172-177. ables. Psychological Bulletin, 1971, 76, 295-310.
Wine, J. Test anxiety and direction of attention. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 1971, 76, 92-104.
Yales, J. F., & Kulick, R. M. Effort control and judg- Received November 29, 1980

You might also like