You are on page 1of 9

Neuroethics (2008) 1:149157

DOI 10.1007/s12152-008-9016-6

REVIEW PAPER

The Cognitive Neuroscience of Psychopathy


and Implications for Judgments of Responsibility
R. J. R. Blair

Received: 11 April 2008 / Accepted: 16 April 2008 / Published online: 14 June 2008
# Springer Science + Business Media B.V. 2008

Abstract Psychopathy is a developmental disorder Introduction


associated with specific forms of emotional dysfunc-
tion and an increased risk for both frustration-based This paper is based on a talk delivered to the American
reactive aggression and goal-directed instrumental Philosophy Association in Baltimore 2007. The paper
antisocial behavior. While the full behavioral mani- was part of a special Session Arranged by the APA
festation of the disorder is under considerable social Committee on Philosophy and Law entitled: Are
influence, the basis of this disorder appears to be Psychopaths Responsible? The goal of this paper is to
genetic. At the neural level, individuals with briefly lay out the current understanding regarding the
psychopathy show atypical responding within the cognitive neuroscience of psychopathy before using
amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). these data to consider what can be said regarding issues
Moreover, the roles of the amygdala in stimulus- of responsibility.
reinforcement learning and responding to emotional
expressions and vmPFC in the representation of
reinforcement expectancies are compromised. The The Disorder of Psychopathy
implications of these functional impairments for
responsibility are discussed. Psychopathy can be considered to involve two
components: emotional dysfunction and antisocial
Keywords Psychopathy . Emotion . Amygdala . behavior [36, 41, 44]. The emotional dysfunction
Responsibility reflects reduced guilt, empathy and attachment to
significant others. The antisocial behavior component
reflects a predisposition to antisocial behavior from an
early age. Psychopathy is typically indexed in adults
with the revised psychopathy checklist [41, 42] and in
children and adolescents, through the antisocial process
screening device [37] or the Psychopathy Checklist-
R. J. R. Blair (*) Youth version [35]. The disorder is developmental in
Mood & Anxiety Program, National Institute of Mental that it emerges before the age of 10 years and persists
Health, National Institutes of Health,
deep into adulthood [45, 58].
15k North Drive,
Bethesda, MD 20892, USA The disorder is not equivalent to the Diagnostic
e-mail: blairj@intra.nimh.nih.gov and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder (DSM)-IV

DO09016; No of Pages
150 R.J.R. Blair

diagnoses of conduct disorder or antisocial personality traumatic stress disorder [76, 82] and childhood
disorder or their International Classification of bipolar disorder [54]. Psychopathy is a remarkable
Diseases (ICD)-10 counterparts. These diagnoses con- condition in that it is associated with increased risk
centrate on the antisocial behavior and do not include for both instrumental and reactive aggression [24, 38].
any extensive investigation of the presence or absence This is important to note because when considering
of the form of emotion dysfunction seen in psychopathy responsibility it is necessary to make a distinction
[14]. Individuals with psychopathy appear to represent between instrumental and reactive antisocial behavior.
only a fraction of those who would receive these
psychiatric diagnoses and thus it is important to note
that data obtained with populations defined by these The Basis of the Disorder
psychiatric criteria will not necessarily appear identical
to that obtained from a group of individuals with There have been suggestions that psychopathy might
psychopathy. be due to early physical/ sexual abuse or neglect [74].
There are also notable differences between individuals These factors certainly do increase the risk of
with acquired sociopathy following lesions of ventrome- antisocial behavior, particularly frustration and threat
dial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) despite earlier claims based reactive aggression (see below) [29]. However,
based on the increased risk for aggressions shown by importantly, the impact of extreme stressors and
some of these patients cf. [26]. Psychopathy is associated neglect on the development of the mammalian brain
with a series of core functional impairments. These are is now relatively clear. Work shows that these factors
not seen following lesions of orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). increase emotional responsiveness to threatening
For example, while individuals with psychopathy show stimuli [18, 71]. Psychopathy, however, is a disorder
impairment in aversive conditioning [6], patients with marked by reduced rather than increased emotional
OFC lesions do not [4]. Similarly, while individuals responsiveness (see below).
with psychopathy [12] show impaired fear recognition Current data suggests that there is a genetic basis to
in facial expressions, patients with vmPFC lesions show this disorder [16, 80]. In one of the largest of relevant
a more generalized impairment for all negative studies involving around 3,500 twin pairs, callous
emotional expressions [47]. unemotional traits were shown to be strongly heritable
(67% heritability) at 7 years [80]. However, currently we
have no understanding of psychopathy at the molecular
genetic level although it is important to note that recent
Instrumental and Reactive Aggression work has identified a variety genetic polymorphisms
that influence the responsiveness of those neural
Numerous investigators have previously made a systems, the amygdala and vmPFC, implicated in
distinction between instrumental a.k.a. instrumental psychopathy [43, 62, 70]. For example, several studies
or premeditated; cf. [78] aggression and reactive a.k.a. have reported that individuals who are long/long
affective, impulsive or defensive; cf. [78] aggression homozygotes for the serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR)
[3, 5, 25, 56]. Instrumental aggression is purposeful gene show significantly reduced amygdala responding
and goal directed (e.g., to obtain the victims to emotional expressions relative to those who have the
possessions). It need not be accompanied by an short form polymorphism of the gene [43]. It is possible
emotional state, such as anger, and can be considered that there is an array of genes whose polymorphisms
cold [78]. In contrast, reactive aggression is affect the functional integrity of the amygdala and
triggered by a frustrating or threatening event and medial frontal cortex. The basic genetic risk for
involves unplanned, enraged attacks on the object psychopathy may emerge if an individual possesses a
perceived to be the source of the threat/frustration. sufficient number of polymorphisms predisposing the
This aggression type is often accompanied by anger individual to reduced emotional and amygdala
and can be considered hot. Importantly, it is responsiveness.
initiated without regard for any potential goal. Importantly, while psychopathy may be under
Many psychiatric disorders are associated with an considerable genetic influence, social factors do still
increased risk for reactive aggression; e.g., post influence its development. For example, socioeco-
The cognitive neuroscience of psychopathy and implications 151

nomic status is associated with the emergence of the anterior cingulate cortex [51], inferior frontal cortex
full syndrome; it is significantly less likely to appear [39], parietal cortex [61], posterior cingulate cortex
in individuals of higher social status [77]. Social [61], insula [6, 61], and caudate [33]. However, there
disadvantage provides motive for goal directed has been a lack of consistently with only one or two
instrumental antisocial behavior and increases the risk studies reporting atypical activity in these regions. It
of frustration and consequent reactive aggression. is likely that this thus either reflects stochastic
influences or reduced input from core regions in the
particular task studied. One additional region that
should be considered however is superior temporal
Dysfunctional Neural Systems in Psychopathy cortex (STC). Atypical activity in this region was seen
in most of the above studies [6, 33, 51, 61]. Of course,
On the basis of the neuropsychological literature, two STC has considerable reciprocal connections with the
core neural regions appear dysfunctional in psychopathy: amygdala [1] and thus the atypical activity in this region
the amygdala and vmPFC. The core functional in individuals with psychopathic tendencies may be a
impairments seen in psychopathy (e.g., deficits in consequence of the amygdala dysfunction. Importantly,
aversive conditioning, the augmentation of the startle the suggestion of STC dysfunction in psychopathy cf.
reflex by visual threat primes and fearful expression [50], unlike the evidence for amygdala and vmPFC
recognition) are all seen following lesions of the dysfunction, remains currently unsubstantiated by
amygdala (for a review of this literature, see [8]. neuropsychological findings (and therefore we cannot
Psychopathy is also associated with problems in reversal be sure that they do not simply reflect reduced input
learning and on tasks such as the Iowa gambling task from core regions).
[20, 64]. These impairments are seen following lesions
of vmPFC [4, 49].
The neuroimaging literature on psychopathy has The Functional Contributions of the Amygdala
proven highly consistent with the neuropsychological and vmPFC
data in that it too has suggested a pathophysiology that
includes the amygdala and vmPFC. Thus, functional The amygdala is crucial for stimulus-reinforcement
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have learning [27, 31]. This learning allows representations
found that adults with psychopathy show reduced of conditioned stimuli within temporal cortex to be
activation of both the amygdala and rostral anterior linked to emotional responses mediated by the
cingulate cortex/vmPFC in response to emotional amygdala and other structures. In humans and other
words in the context of emotional memory paradigms primates, fearfulness and potentially sadness serve as
[51] and during aversive conditioning [6]. Moreover, reinforcers, stimuli associated with expression are
work with sub-clinical populations has found that avoided, and this type of stimulus-reinforcement
individuals with psychopathic traits show reduced learning relies on the amygdala too [46, 63]. In short,
amygdala and vmPFC responses to emotional expres- the amygdala allows the individual to learn the
sions [39] and less amygdala and vmPFC differentia- goodness and badness of objects and actions whether
tion in responding when making cooperation relative to the goodness and badness to be associated with these
defection choices in a prisoners dilemma paradigm objects involves non-social rewards and punishments
[72]. Recent work with adolescents with psychopathic (e.g., desirable foods/money or pain/loud noises) or
tendencies has shown reduced amygdala responses to social rewards and punishments (e.g., fearful or happy
fearful expressions and reduced connectivity between facial expressions).
the amygdala and vmPFC [61]. In addition, a study The amygdala sends outputs forward to vmPFC.
examining reversal learning reported that adolescents VmPFC has been implicated through both animal [75]
with psychopathic tendencies failed to show the and human fMRI work [52] in stimulus-outcome
reduction in vmPFC activity that is associated with processing. Importantly, vmPFC can represent the
an unexpected punished response [33]. reinforcement outcomes associated with multiple
The neuro-imaging literature on psychopathy has objects [68]. In line with this, recent fMRI work has
also implicated other structures, specifically dorsal reported that activity in vmPFC is better predicted by
152 R.J.R. Blair

total available reinforcement (e.g., reinforcement [61]. In short, individuals with psychopathy show
associated with two different objects) rather than only impairment in the form of emotional learning, stimu-
reinforcement associated with the chosen object [15]. lus-reinforcement learning, that is necessary for learn-
This is not to suggest that vmPFC codes total ing the goodness and badness associated with
available reinforcement. Instead, the suggestion is representations of objects and actions. Moreover, they
that what is being seen is activity in separable show impairment in responding to those social
populations of neurons representing reinforcement reinforcers, fearful and sadness expressions, which
associated with the different objects [68]. This is seen are so important for moral socialization. In short, it
as a cumulative signal when indexed by the imprecise would be expected that their ability to be socialized
signal that is the blood oxygenation level dependent would be compromised and the literature suggests that
(BOLD) response. this is indeed the case [67, 81]. Disrupted moral
VmPFC itself does not appear to choose between socialization may lead to an increased probability that
the objects associated with different reinforcement the individual will use antisocial methods to achieve
expectancies. Parameters that are associated with their goals as they are less likely to represent the
decision difficulty (e.g., the number of objects to aversive consequences of antisocial actions (the
choose between or the difference in the reinforcement distress to victims).
expectancies associated with the two objects) do not The amygdala is thought to output expectancies of
influence activity in vmPFC [15, 60]. They do, reinforcement outcomes to vmPFC which then repre-
however, influence activity in other regions, in sents this information to allow successful decision
particular dorsal anterior cingulate cortex [15, 60]. making [75]. Due to the amygdala dysfunction, it is
Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex has been implicated likely that individuals receive impoverished reinforce-
in the resolution of response conflicts [17, 23]; i.e., it ment expectancy information within vmPFC. Indeed,
allows the resolution of difficult decisions. In short, the reduced amygdala and vmPFC differentiation in
vmPFC appears to be critical for the representation of individuals with psychopathy when making coopera-
reinforcement outcome expectancies associated with tion relative to defection choices in a prisoners
objects, information that is then used by other systems dilemma paradigm [72] and the reduced connectivity
to affect decision making. between the amygdala and vmPFC in adolescents
with psychopathic tendencies when responding to
facial expressions is strongly suggestive of this [61].
The Functional Impairment In Psychopathy It is thought that the decision making impairment seen
in psychopathy [13, 64, 65] relates to this impairment
As noted above, psychopathy is associated with in the receipt and representation of reinforcement
atypical responding within the amygdala, vmPFC expectancy information [9].
and, potentially, superior temporal gyrus (STG) (this It is worth noting that there may also be structurally
latter structure will not be discussed here as a reduced connectivity between the amygdala and
functional account concerning why dysfunction in vmPFC in psychopathy. Investigations employing
STG should relate to psychopathy has yet to be Diffusion Tensor Imaging which attempts to index
provided). It appears clear that individuals with psy- the structural integrity of fiber bundles passing
chopathy show impairment in stimulus-reinforcement through white matter are actively investigating this
learning [34, 57); impairment that is reflected also in issue. Moreover, either as an indirect result of this
reduced amygdala and vmPFC activity to conditioned reduced input from the amygdala or as a direct
stimuli during aversive conditioning paradigms [6]. consequence of the likely genetic contribution, it
Individuals with psychopathy also show impairment in appears that the functional integrity of vmPFC is
the processing of the fearful and sad expressions of disrupted in psychopathy.
others. This is seen in reduced arousal responses In reversal learning paradigms, the individual
(indexed by skin conductance) to distress cues initially learns to make a response to gain a reward
[2, 10], impaired recognition of fearful and, to a lesser [20]. After a number of trials, the reinforcement
extent, sad expressions [11, 30, 59] and reduced BOLD contingency changes such that the previously correct
responses within the amygdala to fearful expressions response no longer results in reward, and a new
The cognitive neuroscience of psychopathy and implications 153

response must be learned to achieve the reward. responsibility are out of the scope of this work and,
Reversal learning is reliant on the functional integrity as such, I offer up personal speculations in the hope
of vmPFC [28, 32, 48, 73]. However, the animal that they are of interest to those whose goal it is to
literature, at least, strongly suggests that the amygdala consider such issues.
is not necessary for reversal learning. In other words, One major factor when considering responsibility
the observation of impaired reversal learning in is the individuals intention/ whether the action is
individuals with psychopathy [19, 20, 64], suggests planned. The individual who purposely drives their
the existence of vmPFC dysfunction independent of, car at another individual is considered far more
and additional to, the amygdala dysfunction. responsible and blameworthy than the individual who
The functional integrity of vmPFC may be critical accidentally runs another over. As noted earlier,
for reversal learning because vmPFC is necessary for psychopathy is associated with an increased risk for both
the detection of contingency change due to its role in instrumental and reactive aggression. By definition,
the responding to prediction errors [21]. On the basis instrumental aggression is intentional; the individual is
of this account, vmPFC dysfunction in psychopathy engaged in a planned actionusing the antisocial
would result in reduced detection of the contingency behavior to achieve their goals. In contrast, reactive
change and thus a reduced probability of changing aggression is not. Instead, it is triggered by a frustrating
behavior. Alternatively, or additionally, vmPFC may event and involves unplanned, enraged attacks on the
be critical for reversal learning because of its role in the object perceived to be the source of the threat/frustration.
representation of reinforcements associated with objects In the previous section, the role of vmPFC dysfunction in
[40]. If this is disrupted, there will be weaker tracking the increased risk for reactive aggression seen in
of the changing reinforcement values associated with psychopathy was discussed. In short, part of the
the two objects (i.e., that the good object is now bad syndrome of psychopathy does likely increase the risk
while the bad object has become good) and thus of behavior that could be considered to occur in a state of
impaired decision making; there will be an increased diminished responsibility.
tendency to stay with the object that was originally One of the problems with the instrumental-reactive
good. aggression dichotomy is the difficulty in characteriz-
Importantly, the impairment in reversal learning ing individual antisocial acts [22]. For example, it
and the related difficulties with operant extinction as appears that an individual shooting a romantic partner
indexed by the one pack card playing task; [66] as caught in the arms of a lover is regarded as less
well as the more general impairment in decision responsible for their actions than if the antisocial act
making impairment will all lead to an individual at but not the revelation had occurred. The action is
greater risk of frustration. Frustration occurs when an clearly instrumental in that the nature of the action,
action is taken to gain a particular outcome and that the use of a gun, is not an automatic but rather a goal
outcome is not forthcoming [5]. It has been suggested directed action. However, the action is not cold but
that the increased risk for reactive aggression seen in rather charged with considerable affect. It is possible
psychopathy is due to this increased risk of experiencing that the knowledge of this affect leads to people
frustration [9]. judging the individual as less responsible for their
actions.
Psychopathy is associated with reductions in some
forms of emotion; e.g., guilt, fearfulness and sadness
Implications for Judgments of Responsibility [69, 79]. However, it does not appear that all
emotional responding is diminished. Indeed, the
In this last section of the paper, I will briefly consider increased risk for reactive aggression [24, 38] would
potential implications of the above cognitive neuro- suggest that the propensity to anger is not diminished
science data for judgments of responsibility related to but rather may be increased. In short, the pathology
psychopathy. It should be noted that the goal of my associated with psychopathy, in particular the vmPFC
work is to understand the nature of the emotional dysfunction, may lead to an increase risk for anger
deficit associated with this disorder so that it might based antisocial behavior that could be considered to
come to be effectively treated. Considerations of occur in a state of diminished responsibility.
154 R.J.R. Blair

So far we have considered reactively aggressive and perhaps also superior temporal cortex. In partic-
actions and instrumental antisocial behavior that is ular, it appears that the role of the amygdala in
reactively charged, but what about more general stimulus-reinforcement learning and responding to
instrumental antisocial actions? In recent work, Levy specific forms of facial expression, in particular the
has argued that by failing the moral/conventional fearful and sad expressions of other individuals, is
distinction task [7], individuals with psychopathy compromised. In addition, the role of vmPFC in the
show a fundamental inability to categorize moral representation of reinforcement expectancies is com-
harms and as such their moral responsibility for their promised. Together these functional impairments
actions is reduced [55]. Certainly, as argued above, disrupt appropriate moral socialization and impair
individuals with psychopathy are impaired in the decision making. They also put the individual at
ability to learn the badness of actions and objects and increased risk for frustration based reactive aggression.
their subsequent decision making is consequently As such, from some perspectives [55], it can be argued
dysfunctional. Whether an instrumental action occurs that the functional impairments seen in psychopathy
is determined by decision making; the individual diminish responsibility.
weighs up the reward that may be accrued as a
consequence of the action against any potential costs
Acknowledgments This research was supported by the
associated with the action [53]. The amygdala and Intramural Research Program of the NIH: NIMH.
vmPFC dysfunction described above will seriously
impair the decision making in psychopathy and thus,
following Levys arguments, it can be considered that
the instrumental antisocial actions engaged in by
References
individuals with psychopathy occur in a state of
diminished responsibility.
1. Amaral, D.G., J.L. Price, A. Pitkanen, and S.T. Carmichael.
1992. Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid
complex. In The amygdala: neurobiological aspects of
Conclusions emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction, ed. J.P. Aggleton,
166. New York: Wiley.
2. Aniskiewicz, A.S. 1979. Autonomic components of vicar-
Psychopathy is a developmental disorder associated ious conditioning and psychopathy. Journal of Clinical
with specific forms of emotional dysfunction and an Psychology 35: 6067.
increased risk for antisocial behavior. The increased 3. Barratt, E.S., M.S. Stanford, L. Dowdy, M.J. Liebman, and
T.A. Kent. 1999. Impulsive and premeditated aggression: a
risk is for both frustration-based reactive aggression
factor analysis of self-reported acts. Psychiatry Research
and goal-directed instrumental antisocial behavior. 862: 163173.
Psychopathy is not equivalent to the DSM diagnoses 4. Bechara, A., H. Damasio, A.R. Damasio, and G.P. Lee.
of Conduct Disorder and Antisocial Personality 1999. Different contributions of the human amygdala and
ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. Journal
Disorder, nor is it similar in nature to acquired
of Neuroscience 19: 54735481.
sociopathy following lesions of vmPFC. 5. Berkowitz, L. 1993. Aggression: its causes, consequences,
The full manifestation of psychopathy is likely to and control. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
be under considerable social influence. At the very 6. Birbaumer, N., R. Veit, M. Lotze, M. Erb, C. Hermann, W.
Grodd, et al. 2005. Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy:
least, increased socioeconomic status (SES) and IQ
a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of
are likely to mitigate the increased risk for antisocial General Psychiatry 627: 799805.
behavior resulting from the disorder; the individual 7. Blair, R.J.R. 1995. A cognitive developmental approach to
with higher SES/IQ will have an increased range of morality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition 57: 129.
8. Blair, R.J.R. 2006. The emergence of psychopathy: implica-
activities that may achieve their goals beyond
tions for the neuropsychological approach to developmental
antisocial behavior. However, current data suggests disorders. Cognition 101: 414442.
that the basis of this disorder is genetic although the 9. Blair, J. 2007. Dysfunctions of medial and lateral orbito-
details of this contribution, which genes are involved, frontal cortex in psychopathy. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences 1121(1): 461479.
remain unknown.
10. Blair, R.J.R., L. Jones, F. Clark, and M. Smith. 1997. The
At the neural level, psychopathy appears to be psychopathic individual: a lack of responsiveness to
related to dysfunction within the amygdala, vmPFC distress cues? Psychophysiology 34: 192198.
The cognitive neuroscience of psychopathy and implications 155

11. Blair, R.J.R., E. Colledge, L. Murray, and D.G. Mitchell. 28. Dias, R., T.W. Robbins, and A.C. Roberts. 1996. Dissociation
2001. A selective impairment in the processing of sad in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature
and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic 380: 6972.
tendencies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 29(6): 29. Dodge, K.A., G.S. Pettit, J.E. Bates, and E. Valente. 1995.
491498. Social information-processing patterns partially mediate the
12. Blair, R.J.R., D.G.V. Mitchell, E. Colledge, R.A. Leonard, effect of early physical abuse on later conduct problems.
J.H. Shine, L.K. Murray, et al. 2004a. Reduced sensitivity Journal of Abnormal Psychology 104: 632643.
to others fearful expressions in psychopathic individuals. 30. Dolan, M., and R. Fullam. 2006. Face affect recognition
Personality & Individual Differences 37: 11111121. deficits in personality-disordered offenders: association
13. Blair, R.J.R., D.G.V. Mitchell, A. Leonard, S. Budhani, K. with psychopathy. Psychological Medicine 36(11): 1563
S. Peschardt, and C. Newman. 2004b. Passive avoidance 1569.
learning in individuals with psychopathy: modulation by 31. Everitt, B.J., R.N. Cardinal, J.A. Parkinson, and T.W.
reward but not by punishment. Personality and Individual Robbins. 2003. Appetitive behavior: impact of amygdala-
Differences 37: 11791192. dependent mechanisms of emotional learning. Annual New
14. Blair, R.J.R., D.G.V. Mitchell, and K.S. Blair. 2005. The York Academy of Sciences 985: 233250.
psychopath: emotion and the brain. Oxford: Blackwell. 32. Fellows, L.K., and M.J. Farah. 2003. Ventromedial frontal
15. Blair, K.S., A.A. Marsh, J. Morton, M. Vythilingham, M. cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: evidence from
Jones, K. Mondillo, et al. 2006. Choosing the lesser of two a reversal learning paradigm. Brain 126: 18301837.
evils, the better of two goods: specifying the roles of 33. Finger, E.C., A.A. Marsh, D.G. Mitchell, M.E. Reid,
ventromedial prefrontal cortex and dorsal anterior cingulate C. Sims, S. Budhani, D.S. Kosson, G. Chen, K.E. Towbin,
cortex in object choice. Journal of Neuroscience 26(44): E. Leibenluft, D.S. Pine, and J.R. Blair. 2008. Abnormal
1137911386. ventromedial prefrontal cortex function in children with
16. Blonigen, D.M., B.M. Hicks, R.F. Krueger, C.J. Patrick, psychopathic traits during reversal learning. Archives of
and W.G. Iacono. 2005. Psychopathic personality traits: General Psychiatry 65(5): 586594.
heritability and genetic overlap with internalizing and 34. Flor, H., N. Birbaumer, C. Hermann, S. Ziegler, and C.J.
externalizing psychopathology. Psychological Medicine Patrick. 2002. Aversive Pavlovian conditioning in psycho-
35: 637648. paths: peripheral and central correlates. Psychophysiology
17. Botvinick, M.M., J.D. Cohen, and C.S. Carter. 2004. 39: 505518.
Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an 35. Forth, A. E., D.S. Kosson, and R.D. Hare. 2003. The
update. Trends in Cognitive Science 8(12): 539546. psychopathy checklist: youth version. Toronto, Ontario,
18. Bremner, J.D., and E. Vermetten. 2001. Stress and Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
development: behavioral and biological consequences. 36. Frick, P.J. 1995. Callous-unemotional traits and conduct
Development and Psychopathology 13: 473489. problems: a two-factor model of psychopathy in children.
19. Budhani, S., and R.J. Blair. 2005. Response reversal and Issues in Criminological and Legal Psychology 24: 4751.
children with psychopathic tendencies: success is a func- 37. Frick, P.J., and R.D. Hare. 2001. The antisocial process
tion of salience of contingency change. Journal of Child screening device. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.
Psychology and Psychiatry 46(9): 972981. 38. Frick, P.J., T.R. Stickle, D.M. Dandreaux, J.M. Farell, and
20. Budhani, S., R.A. Richell, and R.J. Blair. 2006. Impaired E.R. Kimonis. 2005. Callous-unemotional traits in predicting
reversal but intact acquisition: probabilistic response the severity and stability of conduct problems and delinquen-
reversal deficits in adult individuals with psychopathy. cy. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 33: 471487.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology 115(3): 552558. 39. Gordon, H.L., A.A. Baird, and A. End. 2004. Functional
21. Budhani, S., A.A. Marsh, D.S. Pine, and R.J. Blair. 2007. differences among those high and low on a trait measure of
Neural correlates of response reversal: considering acqui- psychopathy. Biological Psychiatry 567: 516521.
sition. Neuroimage 34(4): 17541765. 40. Hampton, A.N., P. Bossaerts, and J.P. ODoherty. 2006.
22. Bushman, B.J., and C.A. Anderson. 2001. Is it time to pull The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in abstract
the plug on the hostile versus instrumental aggression state-based inference during decision making in humans.
dichotomy? Psychological Review 108(1): 273279. Journal of Neuroscience 2632: 83608367.
23. Cohen, J.D., M. Botvinick, and C.S. Carter. 2000. Anterior 41. Hare, R.D. 1991. The hare psychopathy checklist-revised.
cingulate and prefrontal cortex: whos in control. Nature Toronto, Ontario: Multi-Health Systems.
Neuroscience 3(5): 421423. 42. Hare, R.D. 2003. Hare psychopathy checklist-revised PCL-
24. Cornell, D.G., J. Warren, G. Hawk, E. Stafford, G. Oram, R, 2nd Edn. Toronto: Multi Health Systems.
and D. Pine. 1996. Psychopathy in instrumental and 43. Hariri, A.R., V.S. Mattay, A. Tessitore, B. Kolachana, F.
reactive violent offenders. Journal of Consulting and Fera, D. Goldman, et al. 2002. Serotonin transporter
Clinical Psychology 64: 783790. genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala.
25. Crick, N.R., and K.A. Dodge. 1996. Social information- Science 297(5580): 400403.
processing mechanisms on reactive and proactive aggres- 44. Harpur, T.J., A.R. Hakstian, and R.D. Hare. 1988. The
sion. Child Development 67(3): 9931002. factor structure of the psychopathy checklist. Journal of
26. Damasio, A.R. 1994. Descartes error: emotion, rationality Consulting and Clinical Psychology 56: 741747.
and the human brain. New York: Putnam (Grosset Books). 45. Harpur, T.J., and R.D. Hare. 1994. Assessment of psychopathy
27. Davis, M., and P.J. Whalen. 2001. The amygdala: vigilance as a function of age. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 103:
and emotion. Molecular Psychiatry 6(1): 1334. 604609.
156 R.J.R. Blair

46. Hooker, C.I., L.T. Germine, R.T. Knight, and M. DEsposito. response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents
2006. Amygdala response to facial expressions reflects with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior
emotional learning. Journal of Neuroscience 26(35): 8915 disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 165(6): 712720.
8922. 62. Meyer-Lindenberg, A., J.W. Buckholtz, B.A.R.H. Kolachana,
47. Hornak, J., J. Bramham, E.T. Rolls, R.G.O. Morris, J. L. Pezawas, G. Blasi, et al. 2006. Neural mechanisms of genetic
Doherty, et al. 2003. Changes in emotion after circum- risk for impulsivity and violence in humans. Proceedings of the
scribed surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
cortices. Brain 126: 16911712. 103(16): 62696274.
48. Izquierdo, A., and E.A. Murray. 2005. Opposing effects of 63. Mineka, S., and R. Zinbarg. 2006. A contemporary learning
amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex lesions on the theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: its not
extinction of instrumental responding in macaque monkeys. what you thought it was. American Psychologist 61(1): 1026.
European Journal of Neuroscience 22(9): 23412346. 64. Mitchell, D.G.V., E. Colledge, A. Leonard, and R.J.R.
49. Izquierdo, A., R.K. Suda, and E.A. Murray. 2004. Bilateral Blair. 2002. Risky decisions and response reversal: Is there
orbital prefrontal cortex lesions in rhesus monkeys disrupt evidence of orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in psychopathic
choices guided by both reward value and reward contingency. individuals? Neuropsychologia 40: 20132022.
Journal of Neuroscience 24(34): 75407548. 65. Newman, J.P., and D.S. Kosson. 1986. Passive avoidance
50. Kiehl, K.A. 2006. A cognitive neuroscience perspective on learning in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders.
psychopathy: evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 95: 252256.
Psychiatry Research 142: 107128. 66. Newman, J.P., C.M. Patterson, and D.S. Kosson. 1987.
51. Kiehl, K.A., A.M. Smith, R.D. Hare, A. Mendrek, B.B. Response perseveration in psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal
Forster, J. Brink, et al. 2001. Limbic abnormalities in Psychology 96: 145148.
affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by 67. Oxford, M., T.A. Cavell, and J.N. Hughes. 2003.
functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry Callous-unemotional traits moderate the relation between
50: 677684. ineffective parenting and child externalizing problems: a partial
52. Knutson, B., and J.C. Cooper. 2005. Functional magnetic replication and extension. Journal of Clinical Child and
resonance imaging of reward prediction. Current Opinion Adolescent Psychology 32: 577585.
in Neurology 18(4): 411417. 68. Padoa-Schioppa, C., and J.A. Assad. 2006. Neurons in the
53. Knutson, B., S. Rick, G.E. Wimmer, D. Prelec, and G. orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature 441
Loewenstein. 2007. Neural predictors of purchases. Neuron (7090): 223226.
53: 147157. 69. Patrick, C.J. 1994. Emotion and psychopathy: startling new
54. Leibenluft, E., R.J. Blair, D.S. Charney, and D.S. Pine. insights. Psychophysiology 31: 319330.
2003. Irritability in pediatric mania and other childhood 70. Pezawas, L., A. Meyer-Lindenberg, E.M. Drabant, B.A.
psychopathology. Annual New York Academy of Sciences Verchinski, K.E. Munoz, B.S. Kolachana, et al. 2005. 5-
1008: 201218. HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala
55. Levy, N. 2007. The responsibility of the psychopath interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression.
revisited. Philosophy, Psychiatry and Psychology 14(2): Nature Neuroscience 8(6): 828834.
129138. 71. Rilling, J.K., J.T. Winslow, D. OBrien, D.A. Gutman, J.M.
56. Linnoila, M., M. Virkkunen, M. Scheinin, A. Nuutila, R. Hoffman, and C.D. Kilts. 2001. Neural correlates of maternal
Rimon, and F.K. Goodwin. 1983. Low cerebrospinal fluid separation in rhesus monkeys. Biological Psychiatry 49(2):
5-hydroxy indoleacetic acid concentration differentiates 146157.
impulsive from nonimpulsive violent behavior. Life Scien- 72. Rilling, J.K., A.L. Glenn, M.R. Jairam, G. Pagnoni, D.R.
ces 33: 26092614. Goldsmith, H.A. Elfenbein, et al. 2007. Neural correlates of
57. Lykken, D.T. 1957. A study of anxiety in the sociopathic social cooperation and non-cooperation as a function of
personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology psychopathy. Biological Psychiatry 61(11): 12601271.
55: 610. 73. Rolls, E.T., J. Hornak, D. Wade, and J. McGrath. 1994.
58. Lynam, D.R., A. Caspi, T.E. Moffitt, R. Loeber, and M. Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emo-
Stouthamer-Loeber. 2007. Longitudinal evidence that psy- tional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. Journal
chopathy scores in early adolescence predict adult psychop- of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 57: 15181524.
athy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 116(1): 155165. 74. Rutter, M. 2005. Commentary: what is the meaning and
59. Marsh, A.A., and R.J. Blair. 2007. Deficits in facial affect utility of the psychopathy concept? Journal of Abnormal
recognition among antisocial populations: a meta-analysis. Child Psychology 33(4): 499503.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32(3): 454465. 75. Schoenbaum, G., and M. Roesch. 2005. Orbitofrontal
60. Marsh, A.A., K.S. Blair, M. Vythilingam, S. Busis, and R.J. cortex, associative learning, and expectancies. Neuron 47
Blair. 2007. Response options and expectations of reward in (5): 633636.
decision-making: the differential roles of dorsal and rostral 76. Silva, J.A., D.V. Derecho, G.B. Leong, R. Weinstock, and
anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroimage 35(2): 979988. M.M. Ferrari. 2001. A classification of psychological
61. Marsh, A.A., E.C. Finger, D.G.V. Mitchell, M.E. Reid, C. factors leading to violent behavior in posttraumatic stress
Sims, D.S. Kosson, et al. (2008). Reduced amygdala disorder. Journal of Forensic Sciences 46(2): 309316.
The cognitive neuroscience of psychopathy and implications 157

77. Silverthorn, P., and P.J. Frick. 1999. Developmental path- 7-year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
ways to antisocial behavior: the delayed-onset pathway in 46: 592597.
girls. Development and Psychopathology 11(1): 101126. 81. Wootton, J.M., P.J. Frick, K.K. Shelton, and P. Silverthorn.
78. Steiner, H., K. Saxena, and K. Chang. 2003. Psychophar- 1997. Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct
macologic strategies for the treatment of aggression in problems: the moderating role of callous-unemotional
juveniles. CNS Spectrum 8: 298308. traits. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 65:
79. Verona, E., C.J. Patrick, and T.E. Joiner. 2001. Psychopathy, 292300.
antisocial personality, and suicide risk. Journal of Abnormal 82. Zoccolillo, M. 1992. Co-occurrence of conduct disorder
Psychology 110(3): 462470. and its adult outcomes with depressive and anxiety
80. Viding, E., R.J.R. Blair, T.E. Moffitt, and R. Plomin. 2005. disorders: a review. Journal of the American Academy of
Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 31(3): 547556.

You might also like