You are on page 1of 15

AN EXAMINATION OF THE EVIDENCE-BASED

LITERACY RESEARCH IN DEAF EDUCATION

HH ABILITY TO RKAD AND WRITE for a Variety of purposes is essential


to success in school and in contemporary society. The purpose of this
investigation was to conduct an exhaustive review of the literature and
a meta-analysis of literacy research in the field of deaf education. Com-
puter and manual searches of 40 years of peer-reviewed journal articles
were conducted. A total of 964 articles related to literacy and deafness
were identified and examined; 22 articles met the criteria for inclusion
in the review. Results indicate that (a) no two studies examined the
same dimension of literacy; (b) there was a paucity of well-designed
group studies; (c) there were no systematic replication of studies; (d)
there is limited data to establish evidenced-based practices. Conse-
quently, increasing the quantity and improving the quality of research
in the field is recommended.
JOHN L. LUCKNER, ANN M .
SEBALD, JOHN COONEY, Read and Writeor Fail experiences throughout one's lifetime
JOHN YOUNG III, AND literacy skills arc essential to sucxess (Chall, 1996). Without well-developed
SHERYL GOODWIN MUIR in today's technological society Ever>'- literacy skills, students cannot partici-
day examples of these skills include ac- pate fully in classroom learning. They
LUCKNER IS DIRECTOR OF THE NATIONAL cessing the Internet and sending and are at much greater risk ftir school
CENTER ON LOW-INCIDENCE DISABILITIES, receiving e-mail; reading instructional failure and lifelong problems with
UNIVERSITY OF NORTHERN COLORADO, manuals for the workplace, comput- employment, social adjustment, and
GREELEY. SEBALD IS PROGRAM COORDINATOR ers, or cars; following directions at personal autonomy (Moats, 2000).
FOR THE NATIONAL CENTER ON LOW-INCIDENCE work, for travel, or for taking medica- Consequently, individuals who strug-
DISABILITIES, COONEY IS A PROFESSOR OF tions; and reading the newspaper or gle to read and write are much more
EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY IN THE SCHOOL OF enjoying a magazine or book. Literacy likely than literate people to drop out
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES, UNIVERSITY OF is also the key to functioning effec- of school, go to prison, or struggle to
NORTHERN COLORADO. YOUNG IS A DOCTORAL tively in schocjl. For most individuals, fmd and keep meaningful, satisfying
CANDIDATE IN THE SCHOOL OF EDUCATIONAL reading proficiency begins in early work (Cramer & Ellis, 1996).
RESEARCH, LEADERSHIP, AND TECHNOLOGY, childhood, advances with formal read- Literacy skills are also vital at a na-
UNIVERSITY OF NORTHERN COLORADO. MUIR IS ing instruction in school, and contin- tional level. Countries that are success-
A CONSULTANT WITH THE COLORADO ues to increase as the result of quality ful at instilling strong literacy skills in
DEWRTMENT OF EDUCATION. educational, social, and recreational their citizens are in a better position to

VOLUME ISO, No. 5, 2005/2006 AMERICAN ANNAi.s O F T H E D E A F


EVIDENCE-BASED LITERACY RESEARCH IN DEAF EDUCATION

meet the economic challenges of op- procedures to obtain valid knowledge, come successful writers (Cambra,
erating in a global information-based which includes research that is evalu- 199-4; Schirmer, Bailey, & Fitzgerald,
economy. Simultaneously, citizens ated using experimental or quasi- 1999). However, for the majority of
with strong literacy skills are better experimental designs, preferably students wht) are deaf or hard of hear-
prepareti to adtlres.s today's complex with random assignment" (Slavin, ing, learning to read anti write is a tor-
educational, social, economic, and 2002. p. 15). The problem for admin- tuously slow and frustrating process.
political issues. Finally, a population istrators, educators, and families who Research and tibservation suggest a
with strong literacy skills enables a work or live with students who are variety of reasons why students who
countr}' to better meet the complex deaf or hard of hearing is the paucity arc deaf or hard of hearing striiggle to
social challenges it faces. For exam- of scientifically based literacy research become fluent readers antl writers.
ple, strong literacy skills are linked to available for establishing research- Five problems arc often cited: ob-
better health outcomes for individu- based methods of instruction. Unlike structed access to the phontiiogical
als (Berkman et al., 2004). Simultane- the other areas of special education, code, limited fluency at the tm.set of
ously, a highly literate population will which atidress conditions often re- ftirmal schooling, inadequate literacy
he better able tt) participate in tlcter- ferred to as high-incidence disabilities experiences in early chiklhooti, tle-
mining how best to allocate scarce na- (e.g., learning disabilities), and unlike layed acquisition t>f vocabulary, and
tional resources among competing general education, the field of deaf prtjblunis with lower-level skills.
priorities, such as education, health, education does not have a large body
transportation, the environment, de- of empirically based experinienta! re- Obstructed Access to the
fense, and social programs. search to draw upon to establish re- Phonological Code
search-based methods for ensuring Hearing children learn to map the spo-
The imi^ortance of literacy has
that every student with a hearing loss ken language they already know ti) the
been highlighted by the National
becomes a literate adult. Compound- printed words on a page. For English,
Reading Panel (NRP), which was con-
ing the challenge of meeting the liter- as for most languages, chat ma[)ping is
vened in 1997 in response to a con-
acy goals of NCLB are the ongoing based on st)untl. Once chiklren under-
gressional directive to review the
difficulties experienced by students stand the underlying principles of
scientific literature and to determine
who are deaf or hard of hearing in the print-sound mapping, once they "crack
the most effective ways to teach chil-
efF(")rt to develop reading skills. Na- the code," they use their knowledge of
dren and youth to read. The NRP is-
tional research on students who are their spoken language to facilitate the
sued its report, and a summarizing
deaf or hard of hearing {e.g., Allen, reading prt)cess (Goidin-Meadow &
document titled Put Reading First:
19H6; Center for Asse.ssment and De- Mayberiy, 2001). Children who are
The Research Biti'Ming Blocks for
mt)graphic Studies, 1991; Traxler, deaf or hard of hearing dt) nt)t have
'leaching Children to Read was devel-
2000) indicates that the average stu- easy access tt^ the phont)lt)gical ctKle.
oped and disseminated (National In-
dent with a hearing loss graduates Additionally, natural sign languages
stitute for Literacy, 2001). These
from high school with reading com- such as American Sign Uinguage (ASL)
findings were then incorporatcti into
prehension skills at about the fourth- have their own vocabularies, mor-
the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001
gratle level. Approximately 20% of phologies, and syntaxes, which do not
(Pub. I.. No. 107-110). NCLB empha-
students with hearing loss (some parallel thtise t)f sptiken or printed
sizes the development of literacy as
2,000 annually) leave school with a English (Marschark & Harris, 1996).
well as school systems' accountability
reading level at or below .second grade
for student outcomes. School person-
(Dew, 1999).
nel are retjuirL'ti to demonstrate that Limited Fluency at the Onset
all students are reading at or above of Formal Schooling
grade level by the end of the third Obstacles to Fluency for Many children who arc deaf t)r hard of
grade, and that they continue to make Readers and Writers With hearing begin formal schooling with
atiequate yearly progress in subse- Hearing Loss little fluency in either a spt)ken t)r a
quent years. signed language, or an awareness of
It is important to note that there are
The NRP and NCLB support read- some students who are deaf or hard print anti literacy concepts (Marschark
ing instruction based on "scientifically of hearing who read at grade level & Harris, 1996). Reading and writing
based research," which is defined as (Erickson, 1987; Geers & Moog, 1989); are considered secondary forms of ex-
"rigorous, systematic, and objective furthermore, there are some who be- pression, highly dependent on a pri-

E 150, No. 5. 2005/2006 AMERICAN ANNAI^J OV THE DEAK


maiy language .system such as speech vicious circle is created: Impoverished groups, replication of results through
or sign as a foundation for develop- vocabularies limit reading compre- multiple studies, an ability to general-
ment. Unlike their hearing peers, who hension, and poor reatling strategies ize results, rigorous standards (espe-
leurn to read and write in a language and skills limit students" ability to ac- cially by means of peer review), and
they already know, tnany students who tjuire atietiuate vticabulary knowledge the convergence of results between
are deaf or hard of hearing learn to from context (deVilliers & Pomerantz, studies (National Center for Education
read and write while simultaneously 1992). Evaluation and Regional Assistance,
learning their first language. 2003). Where strong 'scientifically
Prohlems With based research" dcx'S not exist, it has
Inadequate literacy Lower-Level Skills been suggested that researchers pro-
Experiences in To be effective readers, individuals duce syntheses of research summariz-
Early Childhood need to be aetive, self-regulated, and ing the evidence pertaining to the
Children who have stimulating literacy armed with a variety of strategies to effectiveness of educational interven-
expetiences from birth onward have help them undef.stantl what they are tions and approaches (Valentine &
an edge when it C(.)mes to building vo- reading (Snow, 2002). Regrettably, Cooper, 2004). A common method for
cabulatT, untlerstanding the goals of many students who are deaf or hatxl of integrating a body of literature is meta-
reading, and developing an awareness hearing continue to struggle with analysis. For example, the NRP sug-
of print and literacy concepts (lyon, lower-level skills, such as word recogni- gested, "First, where po.ssible, there
2001). In comparison to their hearing tion, .syntactic parsing, and vocabulary sht>uld be meta-analyses of existing ex-
peers, children who are deaf or hard of comprehen.sion. As a result, they do perimental or quasi-experimental re-
hearing do not have books read to not develop the independent reading search in topic areas not addressed by
them as often (Paul, 1998). an activity strategies they need to understanti the NRP" (National Institute of Child
that has been determined to be an es- many narrative or ex[')(.)sitory texts, Health and Human Development,
sential component in literacy develop- such as self-questioning, activating 2000, p. 19).
ment {Adams, 1991). In many cases, prior knowledge, summarizing the Meta-anaiysis is a statistical proce-
children who are deaf or hard of hear- main idea, ctinstructing re[5resenta- dure used to identify trends in the
ing do not have books read to them be- tional iniages, predicting what text will statistical results of a set of existing
cause the adults in their lives do not follow, drawing inferences, monitc:)ring studies concerning the same research
feel comfortable signing, have a limited for misunderstanding, and re-reading problem (Glass, 1976; Rosenthal,
sign vocabulary, experience difficulty difficult passages of text (Andrews & 19^8). Through such a procedure, ef-
finding a comfortable way to seat the Mason, 1991; Strassman, 1992). fects, which are difficult or impossible
child and hold the book to accomplish to discern in the original studies when
satisfectoty visual contact, or do not re-
Meta-Analysis: Meeting the .sample sizes are too small, can be
ceive positive feedback from the child
Need for "Scientifically made visible, as the meta-analysis is
(Paul, 1998: Stewart & Kluwin, 2001).
Based Research" equivalent to a single study with the
In today's service- and kntnvledge- combined size of all original studies.
Delayed Acquisition driven economy, in which high levels Meta-analytic reviews go beyond
of Vocabulary of literacy and numeracy are required narrative reviews in the sense that
Vocabulary is critical to reatling com- of almost everyone to achieve a got)d they are systematic and explicit, and
prehension. The larger the reader's vo- standard of living, there is a demand employ quantitative methods of analy-
cabulary, the easier it is to make sense for rigorous, sustained scientific re- sis (Rosnow & Ro.senthal, 1996). Be-
of the text (Baumann & Kame'enui. search in education (National Research cause of these features, meta-analytic
1991). Research suggests that students Council, 2002). Simultaneously as [ire- reviews are considered to provide
who are deaf or hard of hearing experi- viously noted. NCI.B and many federal more thorough, comprehensive, and
ence delays in building their level of grant programs cail on educators to precise sunimative evaluations that
vocabulaiy knowledge, have smaller use "scientifically based research" to entail greater objectivity than narra-
lexicons, acquire new words at slower guide their decisions about which tive reviews. Moreover, meta-analysis
rates, and have a narrower range of teaching approaches to use. "Scien- is consistent with American Psycho-
contexts that result in word learning tifically based research" includes ex- logical Association (2001) guidelines
{U'derberg & Spencer, 2001). Thus, a perimental control (or comparison) that call for the use of effect sizes.

VOI.UMK 150, No. 5. 20OS/20O6 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THR DRAF


EVIDENCE-BASED LITERACY RESEARCH IN DEAF EDUCATION

which allow for an evaluation of the our analysis bad to meet five selec- according to its outcome domain. In
practical significance of differences. tion criteria: addition, the effect size for eacb de-
Consequently, the purpose of the re- pendent variable was calculated. The
search for the [ireseiit study was to 1. The study had to have been effect size is a quantitative expre.ssion
conduct an exiiau.stive review of the lit- published in a peer-reviewed of tbe magnitude of difference be-
erature and a meta-analysis of literacy journal between 1963 and 2003. tween tbe scores of tbe experimental
research in the fie]d of deaf education. Only peer-reviewed studies were and control groups. Specifically, it is
considered. Unpublished manu- the difference between two means
Method scripts (e.g., dissertations) were (e.g.. treatment minus control) di\'itletl
General Study Search Process excluded, by the poiited .stanclarti tleviation of
We used a three-step literature search 2. Study participants had to have the two ct)nditions (Thalbeimer &
strategy to identify pertinent studies. been identified as students who Cook, 2002). While statistical tests of
First, computer searches in ERIC, were deaf or hard of hearing. significance tell researchers the proba-
PsythlNFO, the William S. Gray data- 3. Tbe study sample had to have bility of tbe nu]l hypothesis, effect-si/e
base, and the Kraus CLirriculum data- consisted of children and youth measurements tel] tliem tbe si/e of the
base were conducted. The search between ages 3 and 21 years. experimental effect and a]low them to
terms were deaf, deafness, hard of 4. Studies had to have provided compare the magnitude of experimen-
hearing, hearing, impaired, literacy, the necessary statistical infor- tal treatments from one experiment to
reading, and ivriting. Specifically, the mation for the estimation of another (Tbalbeimer & Cook, 2002).
terms deaf deafness, bard of bear- effect sizes (e.g., means, stan- Effect sizes bave the same meaning
ing, and hearing impaired were each dard deviations, group sizes, /' across studies, even though studies use
individually cross-referenced with lit- values, / values, r values). different measures and the scores have
eracy, reading, and wriling. Second. different score distributions (Glass,
5. Studies had to have incorporated
the reference list from every identi- 1977). Effect size can be used to i-evicw
a control group.
fied study was reviewed. Third, man- a set of quantitative re.search studies on
ual searches for articles related to a particular problem or it can be u.sed
A total of 964 articles were re-
literacy and hearing loss in alt issues of as an aid to interpreting the results of a
viewed. Of these, 516 were excluded
the American Annals of tbe Deaf single study (Wilkinson, 1999).
because they were position papers,
Valla Ret'ieiv, and foumal of Deaf practitioner articles, literature reviews, Generally speaking, the effect size
Studies and Deaf Education between curriculum development descriptions, statistic is bel|")ful in judging the practi-
1963 and 2003 were conducted. or program descriptions. Another 425 cal significance of a research study. An
were excluded because they were effect size of 1.0 indicates that the
Specific Study studies that lacked a control group, treatment group mean was 1 standard
Inclusion Criteria studies of teachers or families, quaiita- deviation higher tban the control
For the purposes of the present tive studies, or studies that included group mean. Thus, the average par-
study, literacy was defined as the abil- individuals who were either yt)unger ticipant in the experimental group
ity to read and write. We relied heavily than 3 years oi' older than 21 years. performed at a level that was higher
on materials made available by the Three team memhers reviewed each than that of approximately 84% of all
Wiiat Works Clearinghouse to guide of the remaining studies to ensure that participants in the control group. An
us. Specifically, we iisetl the Stud\' De- each included a description of the in- effect size <:>f 0 indicates that tbe treat-
sign and Implementati()n Assessment tervention, a control group, and tiata ment and control group means were
De\'ice (Valentine & Co(.)per, 2004) as a related to literacy as a dependent vari- identical, which indicates that tbe
model for the development of our own able, and that eacb study sample was treatment had no effect. An effect size
study team's design and implementa- statistically independent from tbose in of 0,2 is considered small; an effect
tion assessment device. One study other studies. Because 2 of the studies size of 0,5 is moderate; an effect size of
team member screened each article to used the same sample and control 0,8 or greater is large (Cohen, 1992),
identify which were research studies group, we were forced to eliminate 1, In calculating effect size estimates
reporting literacy data on students This process left us with 22 studies to for the present study, we weighted tbe
wbo were deaf or hard of hearing. review. average scores by sample size accord-
Each of the studies included in Each studv was reviewed and coded ing to procedures recommended by

150, Nn. ^, 2nnV2OO6 AMERICAN ANNAI^ OF THF DEAF


Hedges antl Olkin (1985). Weighting the age range and gender composition listed in standard bibliographic for-
was conducted because ofthe general of the .sample (as available), and a re- mat.) Examination of the information
tendency of treatment effects to be in- search summary. In addition, for the provided in Table 1 reveals two impor-
versely related to sample size. The for- studies with a positive effect size, sug- tant factors:
mula used to determine effect sizes is gestitms for how the results of the
provided in Appendix A. study may apply to educational prac- 1. No two studies examined the
tice are provided. Several studies are same dimension of literacy (e.g..
Results listed more than once because multi- reading comprehension, vocab-
'lable 1 provides a summary of the re- ple assessments (dependent variables) ulary, worti recognition, writing).
viewed studies, lndutled are the au- were used to examine the effectiveness 2. No replications of previously
thor(s) of the study and the date of ofthe intervention (independent vari- conducted studies were under-
publication, the weighted effect size, able). (In 'lable 2, the studies are also taken.

Table 1
Summary of the Characteristics of the Reviewed Studies

Age range N
(years. (with gender Potential
Weighted uriless composition applications to
effect otherwise where educational
Study size indicated) indicated)' Research summary practice
Swanson (1982) 18 Naming was used for integrati
(4 females, and retrieval ot visual informatidri!
14 males)
MacGregor& Thomas (1988) 3.152 7.9-13.1 45 Use of a computer-mediated text Explicit vocabulary
system that included text passages, instruction that
an electronic dictionary that provided includes the definition,
definitions for unfamiliar words, a a context sentence,
sentence with the unfamiliar word and the use of
used in context, and a game to computer games to
practice key vocabulary improved provide practice with
vocabulary knowledge. key vocabulary
Anken&Hoimes{19/ 379 ,12.11~14.30.. - 10. The use of "adapted etas; iighjinterest literature
Improved word meaning.
MacGregor& Thomas (1988) 1.969 7.9-13.1 45 Use of a computer-mediated text Explicit vocabulary
system that included text passages, instruction and the
an electronic dictionary that provided use of computer
definitions for unfamiliar words, and games to provide
a game to practice key vocabulary practice with key
improved vocabulary knowledge. vocabulary
matsus Armour (1987) 1.829 High a:hool 6 Direct instruction in gramrTiatte^"^ Complementary"
principles of American Sign Language instruction in sign and
and translation to written English translation into written
improved spontaneous writing skills. English
At-Hilawani (2003) 1.767 Third graders 30 Use of the key word teaching strategy The teacher
(17 females. improved comprehension and vocabulary discusses the story,
13 males) teaches students to
select key words,
discuss events, and
summarize passages.
Schneiderman (1995) .2-14.0 20 Use of communication games to teach Sociat-interactive
English-language skills within the context approach to
of meaningful social interactions promoting lan
impraved writing skills. development
Al-Hilawani (2003) 1.352 Third graders 30 Use ot a modified reciprocal teaching The teacher discusses
(17 females, approach improved comprehension the story and teaches
13 males) and vocabulary. students to summarize.
question, clarify,
and predict.

(continued)

VOLUME 150, No. 5, 2005/2006 AMKKICAN ANNAI.S O F THh Vl-AV


EVIDENCE-BASED LITERACY RESEARCH IN DEAF EDUCATION

Age range N
(years, (with gender Potential
Weighted unless composition applications to
effect otherwise where educational
Study size indicated) indicated)^ Research summary practice
Anken& Hahnes(19' le of "adopted classics" improved High-interest literatii
paragraph meaning.
Gillespie&Twardosz(1997) 1.094 4-10 9 Evening group storybook reading to Reading stories to
(4 females, children at a residential program had a students
5 males) positive effect on independent reading
and interest in books.
BoydSVader(1972) Watching videos wttH'captfons'improved "Usedf raptiohs
comprehension of visual information.
Calvert (1981) 0.987 6.10-8.11 16 Intensified instruction that included a low Intensified and
student/teacher ratio and competency- competency-based
based instruction improved the ability fo insfrucfion
recognize correct English syntax.
Akamatsu & Armour (1987) 0.966 High school Direct instruction In grammatical Compiementary
principles of American Sign Language instruction in sign
and translation to written English and translation into
improved writfen English grammar. written English
Craig, Carr, & Latham (1964) 0.923 Second graders 20 Use of the natural ianguage approach, Analytical grammar
which entails the use of phrases, systems of instruction
narrative language, and controlled are less effective
presentation and reinforcement of new than interaction in
vocabulary, improved whtten ianguage meaningful situations.
better than analytical grammar
approaches (e.g.. the Fitzgerald Key) did
Walker. Munro, & Richards (1998)0.890 9-18 30 Inferential strategy training (e.g., Explicit inferential
(15 females, cause-and-effect relationships, strategy instruction
15 males) predicting outcomes) improved
reading comprehension.
MacGregor& Thomas (1988) 0.861 7.9-13.1 45 Use of a computer-mediated text system Explicit vocabulary
that included text passages, an electronic instruction and
dictionary that provided definitions for vocabulary practice
unfamiliar words, and a game to practice
key vocabulary improved vocabulary
knowledge.
Wander,Wilton,T6whsend,& 0.743"' Average='7'.6 7(3females, A word processing program was used fo Use of a word
Thomson (1995) 4 males) improve speiiing accuracy. processing program
Akamatsu & Armour (1987) 0.725 High school 6 The process of having students transcribe Multiple exposures to
2-to-3-minute videotapes of a person content through sign
signing a slory in English word order, and written English
revise their summaries, and respond to
questions about the stories increased
comprehension
(Andrews & Mason (1986) 0.693 5-8 23 Reading simple sforybooks (7-8 pages) Reading to students,
with a picture and 2 or 3 words per page discussion of stories,
and corresponding manual signs for each rehearsal of reading
word, along with 50 drill cards that had words and stories
printed words on one side and a
corresponding manual sign on the
other side, improved pre-reading
print knowledge.
Dale (1979) 0.628 Average =10.3 5 The effect of education in a general Support to the general
education setting with intensive support education teacher and
from a trained teacher of the deaf supplemental work on
improved word recognition skills. conversation skills,
reading practice, and
daily interaction with
the family using
home-school
notebooks

Voi.l'Ml-: ISO. No. 5, 20()S/2006 AMERICAN ANNAI.S OF THE DEAF


Age range H
(years, (with gender Potential
Weighted unless composition applications to
effect otherwise where educational
Study size indicated) indicated)^ Research summary practice
Calvert (1 Direct instruction in sight words and key Direct instructiotii
words in stories, instruction in sight words, explicfl
morphologic anaiysis arxJ use of context instruction in key
clues to figure out word meaning, and words, morphologic
intensified reading instruction improved instruction, instruction
vocabulary. in using context clues,
,and intensified
sading instruction
Explicit reading comprehension strategy Explicit reading
training (e,g,, locating details, story comprehension
grammar) improved reading strategy instruction
comprehension.
Direct instruction in referent words and Direct instruction
phrases in sentences (e.g,, pronouns, in referent words
adverbs, conjunctions), abng with an and phrases in
intensified reading program and direct sentences, and
vocabulary instruction, improved the intensified reading
ability to identify correct phrases or instruction
words when presented with
incomplete ser^tences.
Calvert (1981) 0.388 6,10-8.11 16 Intensified reading instruction improved Intensified reading
performance in answering questions instruction
about short reading passages.
Intensified reading instruction improved Intensified readi
reading comprehension. instruction
Braden, Shaw, & Grecko (1991) 0.294 7.3-11.6 48 Use of computer-assisted instruction Use of computers for
practice activities improved reading. reading practice
activities
24 Use of thematic organizers had a Pre-reading activities
(10 females, beneficial effect on comprehension. need to engage
14 males) students in thinking
about the topic and
provide direction for
applying this
knowledge to the
actual reading.
Intensified reading instruction improved Intensified reading
knowledge of syntax. instruction
(ntensific Jotensifled
spelling. istruction
Braden, Booth, Shaw, Leach 0.123 Average ^14,5 33 Use of telecommunication conversations Use of e-mail, chat
&MacDonald(1989) improved language skills. rooms, and the
Internet
Birch aStuckiess (19 121 Average =10.3 52 Use of programmed language improved Systematic synt;
; (23 females, understanding of comparative adjectives instruction
(e,g., "colder than ..."),
Birch SStuckless (1963) 0,105 Average = 10,3 52 Use of programmed language improved Systematic syntax
(23 females, understanding of predicate nominatives instruction
29 males) (e.g., "A dog is an animal").
:nsor&Koller(1997) 0,101 Average = i o ? y ^ ^ 20 Repeated readingof the same passage Use of rejaeated
(7 females, for 15 minutes for 3 days improved reading ,,
13 males) word recognition.
Braden el al. (1989) 0,098 Average ^ 14,5 33 Use of teiecommunication conversations Use of e mail, chat
improved reading skills. rooms, and the
Internet
Ensor& Roller (1997) 091 Average = 16,9 20 Repeated reading ot the same passage Use of repeated
(7 females, for 15 minutes tor 3 days improved reading
13 males) reading comprehension.

(continued)

UMI- ISO, No, 5, 2005/2006 AMERICAN ANNAI>; OF THF DEAF


EVIDENCE-BASED LITERACY RESEARCH IN DEAF EDUCATION

Age range N
(years. (with gender Potential
Weighted unless composition applications to
effect otherwise where educational
study size indicated) indicated)" Research summary practice
-|
Mander et al. (1995) J
WKIP' 7 (3 females.
4 males)
Use of a word processing program
improved clarity of wording In writing.
Use of word ,JBB
processing program'S|

SchirmerS Winter (1993) 0.052 10,11-16.00 24 Students used a textual schema for Use of well formed
(10 females, comprehension processing wtiile reading stories and novels
14 males) narrative text.
Braden etal.(1989) 0.000 Average = 14.5 33 Use of telecommunication conversations
had no differential effect on reading. ....-.j.,.,.
Robbins & Hatcher (1981) 0,000 9-12 36 Word training had no differential effect
(18 females, on sentence comprehension.
18 males)
= 8.4 14
& Aarnoutse(2001) speech-onty training was used, as well as
when training included speech paired
with sign.
Birch & Stuckless (1963) -0,263 Average = 10,3 52 Use of programmed language did not
(23 females. positively affect students' ability to
29 males) develop sentences.
Mander etai, (1995) --0,280 Average = 7.6 7 Use of a word processing program did not
,. .,,,., ,,. (3females, positively affect students' grammar skills.

Calvert (1981) -0.288 6.10-8.11 16 Intensified reading instruction did not


positively affect students' ability to imitate
grammatically correct sentences.
Birch & Stuckless (1963) -0.319 Average = 10.3 52 Use of programmed language did not
(23 females. s' ability to u
29 males)
Caivert(1981) -0,326 6.10-8,11 16 Intensified reading instruction did not
positively affect students' grammatical
language skills.
Braverman & Hertzog (1980) -0.326 8-20 187 Caption rate (60. 90,120 words per iiiinute)
(92 females. did not affect reading comprehension.

Mander et al. (1995) -0,359 Average = 7.6 7 Use of a word processing program did not
(3 females, positively aftect students' accuracy in
4 males) punctuation use,
Calvert ( f ^ ) ' " -0,414" "6.10-8:-t1 16 intensified reading instruction did not positively'
affect students' ability to use prompt
grammatically correct sentences.
Birch & Stuckless (1963) -0,526 Average = 10.3 52 Use of programmed language did not
(23 females, positively affect students' ability to use
29 males) predicate adjectives
Mander et at. (1995) '^^KI^O.B74 Average = 7.6 7 Use of a wonj processing program did not
(3 females, positively affect students' organizational
writing skills.
All samples included both female and male participants

Voi,i;Mn 150, No, 5, 2n0S./2006 AMERICAN ANNAI.S OF THE DEAE


Table 2
Intervention Studies Reference List

Akamatsu, C.T, & Armour, V. A. (1987). Developing written literacy in Dale, D. (1979). Integration on an individual basis. Special Educa-
deaf children through analyzing sign language. American Annals tion: Forward Trends. 6(2), 22-24.
oftheDeai, r32(1), 46-51. Ensor, A. D. I., & Koder, J, R. (1997). The effect of the method of re-
Al-Hilawanj, Y. A. (2003). Ciinical examination of three methods of peated readings on the reading rate and word recognition ac-
teaching reading comprehension lo deaf and hard-of-hearing stu- curacy of deaf adolescents. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
dents; From research to classroom applications. Journal of Deaf Education. 2{2). 61-70.
Studies and Deaf Education. 8(2), 146-156. Gillespie, C.W., STwardosz, S. (1997). A group storybook-reading in-
Andrews, J. R, & Mason, J. M- (1986). How do deaf children learn about tervention with children at a residential school for the deaf. Amer-
pre-reading? American Annals of the Deaf. 131{3], 210-217. ican Annals of the Deaf, 142{A), 320-332.
Anken, J. R., & Holmes, D. W. (1977). Use of adapted "classics" in a MacGregor, S. K., & Thomas, L. B. (1988). A computer-mediated text
reading program for deaf students. American Annals of the Deaf. system to develop communication skills for hearing-impaired stu-
122{^),8-\4. dents. American Annals of the Deaf. 133{4). 280-284.
Birch, J. W.. & Stuckless, E. R. (1963). Programming instruction in Mander, R., Wilfon, K. M., Townsend, M, A. R., & Thomson, P. (1995).
written language for deaf children. American Annals of the Deaf. Personal computers and process writing: A written language inter-
108{3}. 3^7-336. vention for deaf children. British Journal of Educational Psychol-
Boyd, J., & Vader, E. A- (1972). Captioned television tor the deaf. ogy. 65(4), 44^-453.
American Annals of the Deaf. n7(1), 34-37. Robbins. N- L., & Hatcher, C. W. (1981). The effects of syntax on the
Braden, J., Booth, K., Shaw, S., Leach, J., & MacDonald, B. (1989).The reading comprehension of hearing-impaired children. Volta Review,
effects of microcomputer telecommunication on hearing-impaired 83(2). 105-115.
children's literacy and language. Votta Review. 9/(3), 143-150. Schirmer. B. R., & Winter, C. R. (1993). Use of cognitive schema by
Braden, J. P., Shaw. S, R-. & Grecko, L. (1991). An evaluation of a children who are deaf for comprehending narrative text. Reading
computer-assisted instructional program for elementary hearing- Improvement. 30, 26-34.
impaired students. Volta Review. 93(6). 247-252. Schneiderman, E. (1995). The effectiveness of an interactive instruc-
Braverman, B. B.. & Hertzog, M. (1980)-The effects of caption rate and tional context: Principles from the parent-child interaction litera-
language level on comprehension of a captioned video presenta- ture. American Annals of the Deaf. 140(^). 8-15.
tion. American Annals of the Deaf. 125(7). 943-948- Swanson, L. (1982). Verbal short-term memory encoding of learning
Calvert, D- R. (1981). EPIC (Experimental Project in Instructional disabled, deaf, and normal readers. Learning Disability Quarterly.
Concentration): Report of a study of the influence of intensifying 5(1), 21-28.
instruction for elementary-school-age deaf children (Central Insti- Walker, L.. Munro, J., & Richards, F. W. (1998).Teaching inferential
tute for the Deaf, St. Louis, Missouri). American Annals of the reading strategies through pictures. Volta Review. 100(2). 105-
Deaf. 126{8). 865-984. 120.
Craig, W. N., Carr, T, & Latham, I. J. (1964). Comparison of two Wauters, L. N.. Knoors, H. E T, Vervloed. M. P J,, & Aarnoutse, A. J.
methods of teaching written language to deaf students. Ameri- (2001). Sign facilitation in word recognition. Journal of Special
can Annals of the Deaf. 109(2). 248-256. Education. 35(1), 31-40.

As such, we were unable to e.stablish instrviction in the grammatical direct teaching of sight words
di.stinct categtjrie.s or apply meta- principles of ASL and how to and teaching of morphological
analytic techniques with any group of translate ASI. into written English rules
studies. Accordingly, our initial syn- teacher discussion of stories, and
theses of the re.scarch i.s limited to use instructitm in reading compre- Discussion
of the information presented in Tahle hension strategies The purpose of the present study was
1 to identify promising elements of a interaction to conduct a meta-analysis of research
reading [program for students who arc reading to young students related to literacy and students who
deaf or hard of hearing. Results from use of captions are deaf or hard of hearing. We exam-
the studies with large effect sizes sug- intensified instruction ined the literature published t)n this
gest the importance of use of word processing topic from 1963 to 2003. A variety of
use of simple stories and word limitations need to be noted. First,
rehearsal recognition practice with young despite an exhaustive review of the
explicit vocabulary instruction readers literature, we were able to locate only
and practice with short passages use of the general education 22 studies that met our inclusion cri-
high-interest literature curriculum teria. Clearly, there is a rn^cd for more

VOLUME 150, No. S. 2005/2006 AMKRICAN A^NNM^S OF THF. DKAF


EVIDENCE-BASED LITERACY RESEARCH IN DEAF EDUCATION

experimental studies in the area of lit- of research designs that lead to the sys- leagues (2005), which is .similar tt) the
eracy development of .students who tematic accumulation of knowledge. system tlevek)pe(.i by the OxfortI Cen-
are deaf or hard of hearing. Second, Intei^ventions that have anecdotal tre for Evidence-Based Medicine
while eveiy research .study was re- evidence of effectiveness shouki be (2001), suggests that the majority of
viewed by three inclividual.s to deter- exaniinetl hy means of alternative re- the literature in the heltl of tleaf ettu-
mine if it met the inclusion criteria, it seaivh designs (e.g., quasi-ex|;)eriniental cation would he considered Level 4,
is possible tbat a relevant stutly wa.s designs, single-subjeet time series the lowest level among four prt^posed
excluded. Third, many of the studies design.s). Also, studies need to be levels t)f evitlence:
inclucied in the review provitietl insuf- replicated scj that tieaf education re-
ficient information about the charac- searchers can strengthen the evi- Level 1; meta-analysis inckitling
teristics of the participants. Often, dence for drawing causal inferences well-tlesigned randomized con-
only a general age range was pro- about interventions. trol studies
vicied. gender breakdown was not As a follow-up to the pre.scnt .study, Level 2: ct)ntrollecl stutlies witht)ut
.supplieti, information about tlegi"ee of we are in the process of reviewing the randt)mizatit)n antl quasi-experi-
hearing loss was omitted, and no infor- other types of literacy research (i.e., mental tiesigns
mation about ethnicity was given. Fu- studies that did not include a control Level 3: well designed nonexperi-
ture researchers should gather and group) that were not included in this mental studies (i.e., correla-
report these important details. Fourth, .stutly to summarize the topics antl re- tional and case studies)
we used a stringent criterion ofcjuality sults. We plan to witlen our lens using Level 4; expert committee reports,
for our initial review of the literature. the guitielines for evaluating the tjual- consensus conferences, anci the
As previously noted, we used the rec- ity t)f evidence suggested for hridging experience of respectetl profes-
ommendations of the What Works the gap between research and prac- sitinals
Clearinghouse in selecting empirical tice for different methodologies (e.g.
studies yiekiing research-based evi- Brantlinger. Jimenez. Kllngner, Pu- Similar findings were reptjrted by
dence (Valentine & Cooper, 2004). gach. & Richartlson, 2005; Chatterji, Rasterbrooks (2005), who reviewed the
Consequently, we chose to exclude 2004; Gersten, et al., 2005; Homer literature in the area of literacy anti tleaf
dissertations, professional presenta- et al., 2005; Thompson, Diamond, education and noted, "Research in the
tions, and ERIC documents, as well as McWilliam. P Snyder, & S. W Snyder, area of literacy, although imprtmng in
descriptive and qualitative studies. the last 5 or 10 years, is rife with specu-
However, we tlo not encktrse a single lation, pseudo-emplrically based for the
Our review ofthe literature and the
re.search method, but believe, rather, mt)st part, deferential to a belief sys-
attempteti meta-analysis indicate that
that various research designs are tem, and characterized hy many holes
prtimttting the literacy skills of stu-
needed for different research ques- in the knt)wledge ha.se."
tlents who are deaf or bard of hearing
tions and purposes. We also believe
is a highly valued etiucational objec- Also, the majority of interventions
that in some instances when a grt)up
tive. Yet tiur review of 40 years of liter- that are CLirrently useti with stutlents
design is apj^roprlate, it may be unac-
ature suggests that the Held t)f tleaf who are deaf or hard t)f hearing (e.g.,
ceptable to deny services to control
educatlt)n tkjes not have what the the language experience apprtjach,
group participants.
U.S. Department t}f Education (2003, hilingual apprt)aches, the writing
Our review ofthe literature revealed pp. 10-11) refers to as "strong evi- process, tlialt)gue journals, trade
that conducting research on the effi- dence of effectiveness" or even "possi- hot>ks V.S. hasal reatlers. predictable
cacy of interventions that pronn)te lit- ble evidence t)f effectiveness" about hooks, teaching sight words, teaching
eracy among stutlents who are deaf or any specific educational intei"vention figurative language, the use of story
harti of hearing presents unique chal- for promoting the literacy develop- retelling), have a paucity of well-ct)n-
lenges. The low-incidence nature of ment of students wht) are deaf or hard ducted research to support their use.
the population as well as the difficulty of hearing. Rather, it appears that Comparable fintlings were alstj re-
posed by random assij^nment to for[n most practices have heen cieterminetl |X)rteti h\' Easterbrooks (2005):
treatment and control grou|is are of- by practitioners and respected profes-
ten-cited problems. However, re- sionals in the field. Use t)f tbe frame-
We canntit [joint to many
searchers in the field of deaf education work to classify evitlence-based
materials, strategies, or interventions
to think creatively about the use practices pi'oposed b\' Otlom antl col-
:tntl declare there Is experimental

H ISO, No. 5, 2005/2006 A.MERK:AN ANNALS or THE-: DKAF


proof of their effectiveness. In addi- program reported by the NRP were also children's toys; directions for travel
tion, many of" the jiractices that are identified in 'Ihe National Agenda: or for work, for taking medications,
considered sacrcci cows in deaf edu- Moving Fonvard on Achieving Educa- for making up baby formulas, for
cation have little or no evidence to tional Fxjtiality for Deafand Hard of safety for voting, for messages via e-
support their efficacy Hearing Students as the "core compo- mail, the Internet, pager, and fax.
nents" of a reading program for stu- f-earning to read opens the door to a
Given the lack of empirical studies dents who are deaf or hard of hearing better, brighter future. (Shaywitz,
to support practice in the field of deaf {National Agenda. 200S, p. 22). 2003, p. 293)
education, it may be beneficial to use The NRP identified five essential ar-
the information gained from the stud- eas for effective reading instruction. A Not long ago, most students who
ies included in Table 1, in combination combination of the information pre- were deaf or hard of hearing were able
v/ith tbe general education literacy re- sented in Table 1 with the recommen- to graduate from high .school and im-
search, to develop a model compre- dations of the NRP suggests that a mediately begin earning a paycheck.
hensive literacy pr(.)gram for students reading program for students wht) are Most schools for the deaf had voca-
who are deaf or hard of hearing. The deaf or hard of hearing might include tional education programs that taught
model, as well as the specific compo- the following components: skills that matched the available jobs.
nents of the model, could be viewed Examples of these jobs include print-
as working hypotheses that ct)uld be 1. conversation: the use of speech ing, shoe repair, carjjentr); sewing,
researched and revised based on the or sign (or both) for informal barbering, welding, and automotive
results of validation studies. As sug- exchanges of views, ideas or repair. Unfortunately, jobs in manufac-
gested by Levin, O'Donnell, and Kra- information turing are tUminishing. As a result, em-
tochwill (2003), the initial hypothesis 2. alphabetic principle: the use of ployers arc seeking workers who are
would be Stage 1 of a 4-stage ap- letters and letter combinations computer literate, as well as skillful in
proach tt) examining the efficacy of to represent phonemes or signs reading, mathematics, and problem
the model and the components. Stage (or both) in a system of writing solving (Luckner, 2002). Individuals
2 would involve controlled classroom 3. vocabulary: the words people with poor reading skills are at a disad-
experiments. Stage 3 would entail the must know to communicate ef- vantage when competing for jobs and
integration of the knowledge gener- fectively are therefore less likely tt) be em-
ated from Stage 2 into the design of ployed (Erank, Karst, & Boles, 1989).
4. fiuency: the ability to read a text
randomized classroom trial studies or
quickly and accurately with ease The development of literacy skills
single-subject studies (Horner ct al.,
and expression is regarded as one of the highest prior-
2003). Stage 4 would determine the
5. comprehension: the prtK'css of ities in contem|)orar\' education. Yet lit-
factors that lead to ado|;)tion of the
constructing meaning from print eracy is one of the most complex skills
mtxlel in educational programs and
6. writing: communicating through students must master to ensure aca-
teacher preparation programs f(.)r stu-
the use of written symbols demic succe.ss as well as to function
cients who are deaf or bard of hearing
effectively in the workplace and in
(Odom etal., 2005).
Conclusion society Technology, and the science
While acknowledging that there At the end of the Agricultiiml Age, the behind it, permeates all as|5ects of peo-
are professionals (e.g.. Garan, 2002; ability to write your name meant that ple's lives, from how they work anti
Krashen, 2005) who question the you were literate. Eifty years later, as so- communicate to what they shop for
process as well as the results reported ciety transitioned to the Industrial Age, and how they pay the bills. The com-
by the NRP (National Institute of Child a sixth-grade education provided the plexity of today's world means that in-
Health and Human Development, same status. In H)day's global econ- dividuals need to have some level of
2000), it does provide stimuli for de- omy, emerging technologies have set proficiency in reading, mathematics,
veloping a multidimensional model of the criteria for literacy much higher: and science in order to understand
reading instruction for students who and participate fully in the economic
are deaf or hard of hearing (Easter- In our increasingly tedmica! society and .social realms. The crucial factor
brooks, 2005; Schirmer ik McGough, it's all about i^rint; instructional that iiromotes or hinders success in
2005). Simtiltaneously, the essential manuals ft)r the workplace, for com- today's society is the ability to access,
elements of a comprehensive reading puters, for cars, for putting together understand, and use different types of

VOLUME 150, No. 5, 2005/2006 AMERICAN ANNAI^S OF THK DFAF


EVIDENCE-BASED LITERACY RESEARCH IN DEAF EDUCATION

information, The job of educators Is Center for Assessment and Demographic Stud- ods for meta-analysis. San Diego, CA: Acad-
to hclj) students de\'cl()[i tlie atti- ie.s. (1991), Stanford Achieremen/ Test, emic Press.
I'igbtb edition- Hearing-mipaired norms Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J,, McGee. G..
tudes, knowledge, and skills that will hookk'l. Washington, DC: Gallautlet t'niver- Odom, S., & Wolery, M. (2005). Tbe use of
enable them to become proficient sity, Gallaudel Research Institute. single-subject research to identify evidence-
readers and writers. Improving the Chall, J. S. (1996). Stages of reading derelop- ba.scd practice in special edticatinn. Excep-
inent (2nd eti.) Fort Worth. I'X: Harcouri. lional Cbildren, 77(2), 165-179,
quality of research and bridging tbe Brace. Kiiishen, S. (2005). Is in-school free reading
gap between researcb and practice in Chatterji, M. (21)()^). Fvidenccon "what works": gfxidfor children'!' Why the National Read-
thefieldof deaf education is an essen- An argument for extendetl-term mixed- ing Panel report is (still) wrong. Retrieved
tial step if ettucators ofthe deaf hojie method (F.TMM) evaluation designs. Hdti- April 22, 2005. from http:''ww\v.sdkrashen
cational Re.searcher, .1^(9), 3-13. .coni/articles.'in-schoor\)20FVR/all.html
to improve educational and career Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer Psychologi- Lc'derberg, A. K., <S Spencer. P E, (2001). Vocab-
outcomes for individuals who are deaf cal liulletin, lU, 155-159. ulary development of deaf and hard of hear-
or hard of hearing. A.s n(.)tcd in The Cmmer, S. C, & Ellis, W (Eds.). (1996). Learn- ing children. In M. D. Clark, M. Marscbark, &
ing disabilities: Ufetong issues. Baltimore: M. Karchmer (Eds,). Context, cognition,
National Agenda: Moving forward Brookes. and deafness (jip. 88-112). Washington.
un Achieving Educational liqiiality DeVillicTS, R A,, ik Pomerant/. S. B. (19921, Hear- DC: Gallaudet University Press.
for DeaJAnd Hard Of Hearing Stu- ing impaired studenis learning neu words Levin. J. R., O'Donneil, A, M.. & Knitochwill, T.
dents. "Research is the foundation from wiiucn ciynicxt. A/>/>//(.-d Psycbo/i?igitis- R. (2003). Ediicalional/p.sycboiogical inter-
lics. /.-J, 409-431. vention re.search. ln w: Reynold & G. Miller
upon which tiuality ctiucational prac- Dew, D. (Ed.). (1999). Serving indiridmi/s (lids.). Handbook of psychology': Vol. 7. Fd-
tices for deaf and hard of hearing who are iotv-functioning deaf Report of iicationalpsycholog)' (p^. 557-581). Hobo-
students is based" (National Agenda, the Twenty-Fifth ln.Ktttute on Rehabilita- ken, N|: Wiley
2005, p. 37). tion Issues. Washington, DC: (ieorge Wash- Luckner, J, L. (2Q02), Facititaling (he Iransition
ington llnivcrsity of students who are deaf or bard <)f hear-
Easterbnioks, S, R, {2{)[)'^.yM\uM-\).Reiieir of lit- ing. Austin, 'VX: Pro-Ed.
eracy in literacy derelopnienl and instruc- Lyon, R. (2001). How do cbiUhvn learn to
References tion in sttuients who are deaf and hard of ready Retrieveil Se|)temlx::r 27, 2004, from
Adam.s, M. J. (1991). tieginning to read: Think- bearing. Retrieved February 11, 2(H)5, from www.readingroekets.org/anic!e.pbpi'ID= 101
ing and learning about print. http i/Avww.dcafod. net/De;ifedForu ms/Show Marschark, M., & Harris, M. (1996). Success and
MA; MIT Press. Post,aspx?P(>stID= 1964 failure in learning to read: The special case
.MIcii. T. (19H6). Patterns of academic a Hrickson, M. E. (1987). Deaf reatlers reading (?) of deaf cbildren. In C. Cornoldi &
incni among ht-arinj; impaired .students: beyi>nti the \\H:VA\. American .Annals of tbe J, Oakhiil (Eds.), Reading comprehettsion
197.4 and 1983. In A, Schildroih & M. K;rli- Deaf liJ. 19\~2'-)4. dijfic tilties: Processes and interi 'ention
mcr (F,d.s.), Deaf cbildren in America (pp. Frank, K,, Kiirst. R., Ik Boies, C. (1989). After (pp, 279-300). Mabwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
161-206), San Diego, CA: Little, Brown, graduation: The que.st for employment by Moats, L. C. (2000). Speech to print: Language
American Psychological A-ssociatioii. (2001). disabled college ^rsduaxcs. Jottmal of A{}- essentials for teachers. Baltimore: Brookes.
Publication niamtal of ihe American Psy- plied Rebahilitation Counseling, 20. 3-7. The national agenda: Moving fortvard on
cbologicai Assnciation (5ih cd.). Wasliinj;- Cki]-an. E. M. (2002). Resisting reading man- achieving edticational equality for deaf
ton, DC; Author. dates: How to trit4mph with the truth. and hard ()fhealing students. (2005). Austin,
Andrews.J, F., & Mason. J. M. (1991). Strategy Portsmouth, NH; Heinemann. TX; Author.
usage among deaf and hearing readers. FJC- Geers, A.. & Moog, j . (1989). Factors predictive National Center for Education Evaluaiion anti
ceptioimt Cbildren. 57(6), 536-545. ofthe (.levelo|)meni ol'Iiteracy in profoundly Regional Assistance, (200.^), Identifying and
Baumann, J. F, & K;imc'cnui, F.. J. (1991). Re- hearing-impairetl adolescents, Yalta Review. implementing educational practices .\tip-
search on viK^abuiary instruction: Ode to ';/(2), 69-86. ported hy rigoroiL'i evidence: A user-friendly
Voltaire. In J. Flood, D. Iapp, &]. R. Squire Gersten, R., Euchs, I.. S., Compion. D.. Coyne, M., guide. Washington, DC: tJ.S. IX'partment <f
(Eds.), llandh(Xik on research on teaching Greenw(MKl, C, ,S lnnocenti, M. S. (2005). Education, Institute of Kducatiun Sciences.
tbe FJiglish language arts (pp. 6()'^i-(i32). Quality indicatoi^s fot group experimental and National Institute for Literacy (2001). Put read-
New York: Macmillan. quasi-expeririienta! research in spei ial etiuca- ing first: The research building blocks for
Berkman, N. D., DeWalt, D, A.. Pignone, M. P. tion. Exceptional Children. 7/(2), l-(9-l64. teaching cbildren to read. Retrieved Febru-
Sheridan, S. L., Lt)hr, K. N., I.ux, I,., Sutton, S. Glass, G. V (1976). Frimao', secondary; and ary 15, 2004, from http:.'.''ww\v,nifl,gov/pnn-
F. Swinson, T, ik Bonito, A. J. (2()()4). Uter- nieta-analysis of research, llducational Re- nershipforreading:']iublication.s/PFRhooklet
acy and bealih otucomcs (Summary, Rvi- searcher. 5, 3-H. National Instituie of Child Health and Human
dence Report/lbchnoiogy Assessment No. Glass, G. Y (1977). Iniegratiiig findings: Tbe Development. (2000). Report of tbe Na-
87), Rockville. MD; Agency for Healthcare meta-analysis of research. Review of Re- tional Reading liinel 'leaching cbildren
Research and Quality search in Education, 5, 351-379. to read: An evidence-hased assessment of
lirantlinger, K.,Jimenez, R., Klingncr,.!,. Pugach. Glass, G. V. McGraw, G., & Smith, M. (191). the scientific research literature on read-
M., tk Richard.son, V (2005), Qualitative Meta-analysis in social researcb. Beverly ing and its iniplictttlons for reading in-
studies in special education, livce/i/iinu/i Hills, CA: Sage. struction (NIH Publication No. 00-4769).
Cbitdren. 77(2), 195-207. Goldin-Meadow, S., & .Maybern; R. I. (2<KI1), Washington, DC: l',S. Government Printing
Cambra, C. (1994). An instructional program ap- How do profctundly deaf children learn co Office,
proach to improve hearing-impaired adoles- lead? Learning Disahilities Research and National Research Council. (2002). .Scientific
cents' narratives: A pilot study, Votta Rerieti'. Practice. !6(4). 222-229. research in education. Washington, D(^:
). 237-246. I ledges. I.. V. ik Olkin. I. (19H5). Statistical meth- National Academv Press.

\\ii-: ISO, No. S, 2005/2006 AMERICAN ANNALS or niv. DEAF


No Child Left Behind Act of 2001. Pub. L No. Schirmer, B. R., & McGoiijjh. S, M. (2(K)5). 'Ic-ach- articles: A simplified methodology. Re-
107-lUl, S 12OI-U26, 115 Star. 425 (2001). ing reading t{i chiklrcn who are (.leaf; Vio the trieved January 9. 2005, from http://work-
Odom, S. L., Brantlinj^er, E.. Gersten, R.. conclusions of the National Reading Panel k'arning.com/effcct sizes.htm
Homer, R., Thompson, B.. & Harris, K. R, apply? Revietv of Hdu cat tonal Research, ThoiTipson, B., Diamond, K. E., McWiliiam. R..
(2005). Researcb in special education scien- 75(1), 83-117. Snyder, H, & Snyder, S. W (2004). Evaluating
tific methods and cvidence-ba.se(i practices. ShaTO'itz. S. (2003)- Otvrcomiiigdyslexia: A new the [juality of evidence from correlational
Ibcceplional CbiUtrtm. 7/(2), 137-148. and complete science-bused pi-ogrtini for research for evkiencc-ba,seii practice, ^v-
Oxford Centre lor Fvidcrne-Basecl Medicine. reading prubk-iiis at ttf/y k'lvl. Neu York: ceptional Children. 7/(2), 181-19-4.
(2001). Let'els of eridence and grades for Knopf, Traxler, C. B. (2000), The Stanford Achievement
recouimendcttions. RetriL'\e(.l Februan' 8, Skivin. R. F.. (2002). Kvidence-based education Test, ninth edition: Natk)nai norming and
2005, from liitp://\vww,ce!)ni.net/levels_<>t'_ policies: 'Ihin.sforniing educational practices performance standards for deaf and hard-of-
evidence.asp and rL'search. HdiicationatResearcher, ,^1(7), hearing .students. yoH/-na/ of Deaf Studies
Paul. P V (1998). Literacy and deafness: The de- 15-21. and Deaf Hducaliun. 5(4), 337-348.
velopment of reading, writing, and literate Snow, C. E. (imi). Readingff>y itmk'rstaiidmg: LIS, Department of Hducation. (2003). Identtfy-
thought. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 'Ihwttrd a research and detvlopincHt ;>ro- ing and iiuiilementing educational !>rac-
Rostnth;il, R. (]9"'8). Combinin); results of in- gram in reading C()))iprchetisi<>ii. Adinj^mn. tices sujijMjrled hy rigoiitus erideuce: .A
dependent .studies. Psychological Hiillclhi. VA: R:ind. user-friviidly guide. Washington, DC: In.-,ti-
85. 185-197. Sicwari. D, A., & Kkiwin, i: N. (2001). leaching tute of" E[kicatit)n Sciences.
Rosnow, R. 1,,, & Rosemhal, R. (i99(i). Comput- deaf and hard of hearing studenls: Content, Valentine, J, C, & Cooper, H. (2004). What
ing contrasts, effect sizes, and councernulls strategies, and curriculum. Boston: Allyn ik Vtb/A'.v Clearinghouse study desigri and im-
on other people'.s published data: General Bacon, filementatioii assessment derice (Versk)n
procedures for research consumers. /Vi-- Stra.ssmun, B. K. (1992). Deaf adolescents' 1,1). Washinglon, DC: U.S. Department of
chological Methcjds. 1. 331-340. metacognitive knowledge about school- Education.
Schirmer. B, R., Bailey. J., & Fitzgerald, S. M, ri;late{.l reaiiing, American Annals iif the Wilkinson. L (1999), Stati.stic:il metiiods in psy-
(1999). I'sinj! a written a.s.scs.sMieni rubric Daaf /J7(4). 326-330. cliolog\' journals: Guidelines and explana-
for writing development of children who art- Thalhcimer, W. & Cook. S. (2002), llvw to cal- tions (Task Eorcc on Statistical Inference).
deaf. Exceptional Children, 65(3), 3H3-397. cttlate effect sizes from ptiblished research American Psyclx}logist, 5-J(H). 594-604.

Voi,L!MF, ISO, No. 5. 20OS/20O6 ANNALS OF


EVIDENCE-BASED LITERACY RESEARCH IN DEAF EDUCATION

Appendix A

Formula Used to Determine Effect Sizes

wbere

V-w^ (,,- 1) + (77.,,,,^.-1)

and where
.T..p represents the mean for the experimental group
..x'.,--/ represents the mean tor the control group
S/..,, represents the pooletl .stantlani deviation between the two groups

The pooled standard deviation is used rather than each grou|")'s standard deviation in the
calculation because it provides a better estimate of effect size (i.e., an unbiased estimate).

GAIN SCORE (Glass, McGraw, & Smith, 1981)

where

.Vcviv/xw represents the post-mean for tbe experimental grcjup (i.e., mean score after tlie intervention
occurred)
x,:,|,.,,.. represents the pre-mean for the experimental group (i.e., mean score prior to tbe interventii>n
occurring)
.v,.-,r represents tbe post-mean for the control group (i.e., mean score after the intei^vention occurred)
x.,^,.., represents tbe pre-mean for the control group (i.e., mean score prior to tbe intervention occurring)
5c,,,>.,<.,/ & S,^,^..,! represent the pooled standard deviations between the pre- and post-measures for eacb
group (i.e., experimental and control); e.g., .S',.,,,.,,.,, is calculated liy pooling tbe 5,,,^,,^,. and tbe S^,^,,,,,

UNBIASED EFFECT SIZE, d' (i.e.,

d' = d(l -
4N-9\
wbere

/'-^^*-<:/for large .V

Tbis means that d' approaches d in distribution as tbe sample size gets larger, wbich would
indicate an unbiased estimation of r/. Tbis is done because d is an unknown parameter.

i-; n o . No. S, 2005,72006 AMERICAN ANNALS OF 'niE DEAF

You might also like