You are on page 1of 14

INT J LANG COMMUN DISORD, NOVEMBERDECEMBER 2013,

VOL. 48, NO. 6, 689702

Research Report
Investigation of practices to support the complex communication needs
of children with hearing impairment and cerebral palsy in a rural district of
Kenya: a case series
Karen Bunning, Joseph K. Gona, Susan Buell, Charles R. Newton and Sally Hartley
School of Allied Health Professions, Faculty of Medicine & Health, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
Centre for Geographic Medicine Research (Coast), Kenya Medical Research Institute, Kilifi, Kenya
Neuroscience Unit, Institute of Child Health, University College London, London, UK
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
(Received January 2013; accepted June 2013)

Abstract
Background: Rehabilitation services are scarce in low-income countries, where under-representation of some
specialist professions has led to the role extension of others. An example of this can be found in Kilifi in Kenya
where the role of speech and language therapy has been taken on by occupational therapists and teachers.
Aims: To investigate the communication practices used by these professional groups to support children with
complex communication needs in a rural part of Kenya and to explore the ways in which this might be seen to
facilitate or obstruct improved communication by asking the following questions: What are the critical features
of interactional discourse in practitionerchild dyads with caregiverchild dyads providing a natural comparison?
What communicative modalities and practice techniques are invoked? And how does this information relate to
extending professional roles?
Methods & Procedures: An in-depth, descriptive study of a case series was conducted in a school for deaf children
and the occupational therapy department of a district general hospital. A mixed methodology was used involving
naturalistic observation and applied linguistics analysis. A convenience sample was established comprising six
practitionerchild dyads assigned to partnership types: (A) three children with hearing impairment and their
teachers; and (B) three children with cerebral palsy and their occupational therapists. As a natural comparator, the
same three children in B were also observed with their mothers (partnership type C). Dyadic interaction was video
recorded on three occasions. The video data were sampled, transcribed into standard orthography and translated.
Codes were applied to determine turn structure, linguistic move types and communicative modalities. Sequential
analysis was conducted on the move types.
Outcomes & Results: Partnership type A dyads showed a fairly even turn distribution between teacher and child.
A common pattern was teacher-initiated Instruct and Model/Prompt, followed by child response in the form of an
Action. The most frequently used modality was Sound Production and Hands-on-Articulators, which corresponded
to articulation drill practice. Partnership type B dyads revealed a tendency towards adult domination of turns.
The majority of adult-initiated moves required no response from the child. The practice technique Hands-on-
Articulators involved manipulating the oral musculature of the child. Partnership type C dyads showed resonances
of type B dyads, although focused more on Motor-Action in relation to task performance.
Conclusions & Implications: The assignment of speech and language therapy duties to teachers and occupational
therapists has resulted in suboptimal practice for children with complex communication needs.

Keywords: communication, rehabilitation, developing country, interactional discourse.

Address correspondence to: Karen Bunning, School of Allied Health Professions, Faculty of Medicine & Health, University of East Anglia,
Norwich NR4 7TJ, UK; e-mail: k.bunning@uea.ac.uk
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders
ISSN 1368-2822 print/ISSN 1460-6984 online  C 2013 Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists

DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12042
690 Karen Bunning et al.

What this paper adds?


What is already known on the subject?
There is a scarcity of speech and language therapists in developing countries. Extending the roles of other, established
disciplines is a commonly applied strategy to circumvent gaps in multidisciplinary provision. However, there is
currently no published information on the nature and effectiveness of such practice.
What this paper adds?
This case series provides evidence of the way communication support activities were delivered to children as part
of a role extension by teachers in a special, residential school for the deaf and occupational therapists seeing
pre-school children at the district general hospital in a rural part of Kenya. Practice tackled surface-level features of
communication, i.e. speech production or else manipulation of the oral musculature for speech, with limited recourse
to language and symbolic development. The study concluded that the assignment of duties to circumvent gaps in the
multidisciplinary team runs the risk of suboptimal practice, and may lead to the consequential neglect of childrens
complex communication needs.

Introduction East African universities (Staley 2013). East Africa has an


The prevalence of communication disorder amongst the estimated population of 141.8 million (The World Bank
full range of impairments experienced by children in 2011). Based on a 15% prevalence rate (WHO 2011:
Sub-Saharan Africa has been reported at around 50% of 29), 21.27 million will have a disability, of which 50%
the disabled population, e.g. 49.5% in Uganda (Hartley are likely to have communication difficulties (Hartley
1998), over 50% in Zimbabwe (World Health Organi- 1998, WHO 1997, Mungala-Odera et al. 2006). This
sation (WHO, 1997), and 52.5% in Kenya (Mungala- indicates that 10.6 million may require speech and lan-
Odera et al. 2006). Associated with cognitive impair- guage therapy. There are about 35 speech and language
ment, autistic spectrum disorders and sensory or motor therapists covering this region (18 Makerere graduates,
impairments, the nature of communication need may VSO volunteers and independent practitioners), which
be complex and have a pervasive impact on the develop- gives an approximate ratio of one speech and language
ing child, variously affecting social inclusion and access therapist to 302 857 children and adults with commu-
to education (Karangwa et al. 2010), classroom partic- nication difficulties.
ipation (Ademokoya and Olujide 2007), and academic Poor availability of speech and language therapy
attainment (Olusanya et al. 2006). has given rise to two main approaches. The first is
Despite the reported prevalence and impact of child- community-based initiatives as promoted by the WHO
hood disorders in Sub-Saharan Africa, there is a scarcity through the Community Based Rehabilitation guide-
of rehabilitation specialists (Mont 2007). Discrepan- lines, which includes how to conduct a basic assess-
cies in rehabilitation personnel employed in countries ment and implement communication strategies to over-
of low-income status compared with middle-income come difficulties (WHO 2010: 1218). In Kenya,
ones have been reported. In high-income countries, other community-based initiatives have included work-
speech and language therapy is the acknowledged pro- ing with womens groups to mobilize local support for
fession responsible for addressing communication dif- disabled children and their families (Hartley et al. 2009).
ficulties. In low-income countries, however, there is The second approach involves the extension of existing
a dearth of appropriately qualified personnel (WHO practice to carry out duties associated with the missing
2011). In East Africa (Burundi, Kenya, Rwanda, Tanza- profession; however, there has been no published re-
nia and Uganda), speech and language therapy provision port of the effectiveness of this role reassignment strat-
has mainly been confined to Nairobi and Mombasa in egy or of the resultant practice. In Kilifi, occupational
Kenya, Moshi in Tanzania, and Kampala in Uganda therapists were one of two professions implicated by a
(Jochmann 2006), and supplied by a small, expatri- lack of speech and language therapy provision, where
ate workforce from the organization Voluntary Service their role extension required specific accommodation of
Overseas (VSO). A more recent development is the es- children with complex communication needs (Bunning
tablishment of a dedicated specialist training course at et al. 2007) in spite of reported workforce deficien-
Makerere University in Uganda. So far, two cohorts cies (International World Federation of Occupational
have graduated successfully (n = 18), with destination Therapy 2012). Occupational therapy is a client-centred
posts reported as Uganda, Tanzania, Rwanda and Kenya. health profession concerned with promoting health and
Three more regional programmes are also planned at well-being through occupation. It employs interventions
Practices to support communication needs in Kenya 691
for physical and psychiatric conditions using specific, Cultural factors form another layer of influence on
purposeful activity to prevent disability and promote interaction style. Oral communication may have partic-
independent function in all aspects of daily life (see ular importance in rural communities of Africa where
International World Federation of Occupational Ther- literacy levels are low (Hartley 1998). Tonsing et al.
apy). Occupational therapists in Africa have a key role (2005) observed that some cultures with a stronger oral
in addressing the needs of children with developmental tradition might find aided forms of communication, e.g.
delay, providing information to caregivers, psychological communication boards, less acceptable than unaided
support through counselling, and setting up stimulation forms, e.g. speech and sign. The importance of speech is
programmes to promote physical, cognitive and social demonstrated in Hartley et al.s (2009) study involving
development (Mwanjabe 2010). Sherry (2010) observes community groups, in which deafness was attributed to a
that in spite of an underlying person-centred philosophy, lack of speech, with hearing predicted to return once the
pressure to conform to a medical model characterized child had started speaking. Their study revealed a lack
by professional dominance may be present. of understanding and awareness of childhood disability
Teachers were the second profession with an ex- in the community. Speech has been included in the goal
tended remit for addressing communication needs. In- of normality expressed by caregivers (Hartley et al.
formation on pedagogical practice in Kenyas special 2009). The culture of mainstream education in Sub-
education sector is largely undocumented, although Saharan Africa reflects the high value of speech in the
Muuya (2002) reported a major focus on pupil con- domination of teacher-led recitation, where classroom
trol, containment and care. A larger body of research discourse has been characterized as teacher explanations,
has examined practice in mainstream education. The question and answer routines, and whole-group choral
dominance of transmissional forms of teaching that re- responses (Hardman and Abd-Kadir 2007).
strict opportunities for pupil participation in classroom The current study aimed to investigate the commu-
discourse has been reported (Abd-Kadir and Hardman nication practice used by occupational therapists and
2007, Ackers and Hardman 2001). Teacher-led recita- teachers of the deaf to support children with complex
tion involving rote and repetition has also been observed, communication needs in Kilifi District, Kenya, and to
with limited attention to pupil understanding (Ponte- explore the ways in which this might be seen to facil-
fract and Hardman 2005). The introduction of a na- itate or obstruct improved communication. Kilifi Dis-
tional, school-based, teacher development programme trict is approximately 60 km north of Mombasa and
at the primary education level in Kenya was found to is the second poorest district in Kenya, with most of
improve levels of teacherpupil interaction; however, the the rural population living as subsistence farmers. There
absence of teacher follow-up, i.e. evaluation of the childs were three main research questions: What are the critical
response, reported in previous research (Abd-Kadir and features of interactional discourse in practitionerchild
Hardman 2007, Pontefract and Hardman 2005) per- dyads with caregiverchild dyads providing a natural
sisted (Hardman et al. 2009). comparison? What communicative modalities and prac-
As well as the setting, the presenting condition of tice techniques are invoked? How does this information
the child may affect practice. An apparent lack of re- relate to extending professional roles?
sponsiveness in a child with a developmental condition
may lead to the adult adopting more directive and in-
Design, setting and sample
structional styles of communication. Pennington (2008)
observed that parental use of questions and action re- The study was a descriptive, in-depth investigation of the
quests with children with cerebral palsy helped to cir- interactional discourse occurring between children with
cumvent ambiguities of meaning by restricting the lin- complex communication needs and the adults support-
guistic context. A similar pattern has been identified in ing them. The case series employed a mixed method-
children with profound cognitive impairment and com- ology involving naturalistic observation and application
plex needs (Bunning et al. 2013). In a review of related of structuralfunctional linguistics.
studies on adultchild interaction, Marfo et al. (1998) A preliminary survey of rehabilitation for children
identified two parameters affecting communication: (1) with developmental conditions identified two settings
the directiveness or the extent to which the adult directs where dedicated communication support activities were
the actions of the child; and (2) the sensitivity to child provided (Bunning et al. 2007). The first setting was a
behaviour or the degree to which the adult is tuned-in residential school for the deaf, which housed what was
to the childs response repertoire. Intrusive directiveness referred to as a speech therapy department staffed by
combined with a lack of sensitivity seems to inhibit child teachers of the deaf. The school followed an oral ap-
responsivity; however, directiveness combined with sen- proach to communication, although not exclusively
sitive, contingent responses to child behaviour appears Kenyan Sign Language was also used in the class-
to facilitate development (Marfo et al. 1998). rooms and in peer interactions during free periods.
692 Karen Bunning et al.
Table 1. Practitioner characteristics

Professional Training receivedlast Experience


Practitioner Sex Languages Place of work qualification 5 years (years)
T1 Male Swahili; English; Kenyan Special School Diploma (Sp. Speech and languageemphasis 20
Sign Language for Deaf Needs Ed.) on phonetics and phonology;
audiometric testing; hearing aid
repair and maintenance
T2 Male Swahili; English; Kenyan Special School Diploma (Sp. As above 16
Sign Language for Deaf Needs Ed.)
OT1 Female Swahili; English; Giriama Kilifi District Diploma in OT Tricycle and designing; 19
Hospital community counselling;
HIV/AIDS peer education;
home care
OT2 Female Swahili; English; Giriama Kilifi District Diploma in OT General counselling; TB and 13
Hospital leprosy
Note: Teachers = T1/T2; and occupational therapists = OT1/OT2.

Children were drawn out of the classrooms for individ- The two occupational therapists had received no spe-
ual sessions of approximately 10-min duration in one of cific training in speech, language and communication,
the allocated rooms, twice weekly. In line with all educa- but had followed an interest in children presenting with
tional provision in Kenya, Spoken English was the pre- communication needs in their department. Both teach-
ferred language for the childrens education, which in- ers of the deaf had received training in audiology, hearing
cluded speech therapy. This was against a background aid management, Kenyan Sign Language, phonetics and
of the local languages used by families and communi- phonology.
ties being Giriama and Swahili. The second setting was
the occupational therapy department at Kilifi District
Hospital, where one day a week was dedicated to what Ethics
was termed speech work in the absence of a speech Ethical approval was given by the National Ethical Re-
and language therapy service. Their caseload comprised view Committee of Kenya and at the University of East
children aged 05 years with a variety of developmental Anglia in Norwich, UK. Informed consent was obtained
conditions. Children were usually seen individually in from all participants.
a small side room designated for this purpose. Ther-
apy was conducted in the language of the childs home,
which was usually Swahili or Giriama. In order to gain Methods
insights into natural communication taking place in
contrast to professional practice, it was decided to in- Data collection
clude the primary caregiver interacting with the child. Video recording was used to capture communication
A convenience sample was established comprising taking place in the nine communication dyads. For
three types of communication partnership: A, teachers partnership types A (teacherchild) and B (occupa-
of the deaf and children with hearing impairment; B, tional therapistchild), sessions designated specifically
occupational therapists and children with cerebral palsy; for communication support activities were targeted. For
and C, caregivers and children with cerebral palsy. The partnership type C (motherchild), video capture was
head of each of the two services nominated three chil- conducted during waiting time for an occupational ther-
dren who were currently in receipt of dedicated support. apy appointment. To accommodate individual variabil-
Departmental nomination and familiarity with the child ity across time, video capture for each dyad occurred on
were favoured over a more random sample because the three separate occasions in different weeks. By restricting
practitioners needed to feel confident about being ob- the temporal gaps between recording times, data stabil-
served in their work. The sample comprised six children, ity was considered more likely. The camera operators
three with hearing impairment and three with cerebral research status was known to persons present, but no
palsy; four service practitioners and three caregivers part was taken in the ongoing activity. The adult was
all mothers (a total of 13 participants). Table 1 displays asked to do what you usually do with the child and to
the characteristics of the adults. Table 2 presents sum- ignore the camera as far as possible. Filming followed
mary information on each child that was available at the usual amount of time allocated to the activity and
the time of recruitment. Three children were observed varied across partnerships accordingly. A typical speech
with teachers of the deaf. The remaining three children session in the school involved the teacher sitting with
were seen with the occupational therapists, and again the child, either at a desk or else on the floor. Using
with their mothers making nine partnerships in total. everyday objects or paper-based materials, the teachers
Practices to support communication needs in Kenya 693
Table 3. Distribution of turns across dyads in different

0 = No usable speech; 1 = a few words and sounds only; 2 = conversation about familiar things with help from the listenerfrequent failures in communication still occur; 3 = conversation about familiar things with little or no assistancesome
(see table 2)
Practitioner

failures in communication still occurs; 4 = discussion about new ideas as well as familiar thingsa few failures in communication still occur; and 5 = communication is adequate for purposeminimal discernible communication difficulties.
partnership types

OT1

OT2

OT2
T2

T2
T1
Partnership type A
T1 C1 T2 C2 T2 C3

M1: mother; < 20;00; only

M3: mother; age unknown;


Mean 27 27.7 30.7 26.3 24.7 23.7

M2: mother; > 20; five


Range 2333 2032 2637 2435 2030 2029
Caregiver
summary

Partnership type B

the one child


only child
OT1 C4 OT2 C5 OT2 C6

children
Mean 36 30.7 14.7 0.3 9 0.3
Range 2646 2043 1020 01 712 01
Partnership type C
difficultya
Areas of

3, 5, 6

M1 C4 M2 C5 M3 C6
1, 4

3
1

Mean 18.6 12.3 13.6 4 0 0


Range 1622 716 718 09 0 0
Table 2. Child (C4, C5 and C6) characteristics and mothers (M1/M2/M3)

Distraction testresponse at 110

Imitates lip language; symbolic


Attempts speech; short syllables
Communication and hearing

R = better ear90 db; basic

use of objects; vocal play

performed articulation drill work or sound discrimina-


db (drum); no speech
7090 db; speech delay

tion tasks with the child. In occupational therapy, the


Summary

child was positioned in supported seating during ther-


apy activities, e.g. a chair with a high back, a wedge
Notes: a 1 = hearing impairment; 2 = visual impairment; 3 = physical/motor difficulties; 4 = attention problems; and 5 = behaviour problems.
Vocalizes, cries

between the legs and a fixed tray to front, with the


gestures

mother in attendance. Activities mainly involved word


repetition or therapist wiping warm cloths and large ice
blocks (dimensions of 4 by 8 by 2 inchesthe sort to
be found in picnic cooler bags) around the childs oral
b
Rating

musculature. Interactions between the mother and child


12
2
0

1
2

usually involved carrying out gross motor exercises, e.g.


flexing and extending childs legs in bicycle fashion, or
Years in
school

fine motor tasks, e.g. placing wooden pegs in a board.


1
1

0
1

0
School for the Deaf

Data sampling
OT Department

The video footage was uploaded to a computer using


Service

Adobe Premier software. Competing demands on trans-


As Above
As above

As above
As above

lator availability affected project capacity to achieve full


transcripts of all recorded material. In order to reduce
the video footage into more manageable units, a sam-
pling framework was applied. Preliminary observations
Languages Spoken

Giriama; Swahili

Giriama; (a little
Chonyi; Swahili

(Bunning et al. 2007) revealed that the therapy appoint-


in Home

ments in both settings tended to be brief, around 1012


Swahili)

min in length, and focused typically on one main activ-


Swahili
Swahili

Swahili

ity, which was repeated throughout the session. Whilst


the study of rare phenomena often requires denser sam-
ples (Tomasello and Stahl 2004), the repetitive nature
Age (years;
months)
10;00
06;00

11;00

of therapy activities meant that it was not likely to be


4;06

3;00

1;06

the case for the current study. The first 2 min of the ses-
sion were excluded to minimize any potential effects of
camera reactivity. One-minute-long segments were sam-
Female

Female
Male
Male

Male

Male
Sex

pled at 2-min intervals in order to maintain the stability


of the interaction samples as far as possible (Heilmann
Child

et al. 2010). A 15-s lead-in was inserted at the start of


C1
C2

C3

C4

C5

C6

each segment to enable observer orientation. Data anal-


b

ysis was applied to the first three segments sampled from


694 Karen Bunning et al.
each session captured. This meant that each partnership proximations in Swahili or Giriama. Repeat coding was
yielded a total of nine video segments (three segments applied to a random sample representing 30% of the
from each of three video recorded sessions). data. Inter-coder agreement was estimated using the
Kappa statistic () and revealed Kappa values for lin-
guistic move types: = 0.79 and modes of communi-
Transcription and coding
cation: = 0.87 that were excellent according to Fleisss
The data were transcribed into standard orthography (1981) interpretation.
through repeated viewings of the video footage using
conventions adapted from Crystal (1992). A transcrip-
tion template was used, which was divided into three Data analysis
columns: the communication acts (vocal and non-vocal
aspects) of children, adults and contextual information, To investigate the distribution of turns and linguistic
respectively. Participant use of spoken language was move types in dyads, the mean and range for adult and
transcribed verbatim and translations inserted imme- child per dyad were calculated across the three sam-
diately afterwards in square brackets. Any unintelligible pled sessions. The strength of association between adult
items were represented by asterisks in brackets with a and child contributions and the recurrence of move type
question mark. Descriptions of other vocal behaviour, patterns were established through event-sequential anal-
e.g. high-pitched prolongation of vowel as in a scream, ysis within a time-lag framework. (Bakeman and Quera
and non-vocal acts, e.g. manual sign (standard and id- 2011, Bakeman and Gottman 1997, Pennington and
iosyncratic), body movement, handarm gesture, facial McConachie 1999). Firstly, the two- and three-event se-
expression, eye gaze, and manipulation of materials used quences were identified and marked on the transcripts.
in the session, were placed in brackets to make it distinct Then the raw frequencies for each two-event sequence
from spoken communication. were counted and entered into a prepared matrix, with
Turn boundaries were determined by pause length, the coding categories aligned vertically on the left for lag
usually of a 2-s gap and terminal contour behaviours, 0, also termed the given move, and horizontally for lag
e.g. pitch change, and non-vocal signals such as a break 1, termed the target move. A similar matrix was pre-
in eye gaze. A system-based approach to coding, guided pared for sequences from lag 1 to lag 2. When calculating
by the principles of structuralfunctional linguistics, was the total behaviours recorded, care was taken to avoid
used to determine the nature of the interaction. Based counting the event at lag 1 twice. Because of the few in-
on the work of Carletta et al. (1996), the framework cidences of transitions from lag 1 to lag 2, the next stage
consisted of two categories of dialogue move: initia- of the analysis focused only on those from lag 0 to lag
tion (I-) and response (R-). Additional codes were 1. The simple probability with which each given event
added to cater for some of the childrens early stage occurred in a dyad was calculated initially, followed by
communication behaviours, e.g. Self/Shared Expression the transitional or conditional probabilities for each tar-
(SSE); and to accommodate the teachinglearning as- get event being coded in relation to a given event. The
pects of therapy, e.g. the initiation move Model/Prompt probabilities were then converted to percentages.
(I-MP) and the response move Feedback (R-F). Based Communicative modalities were identified and
on preliminary observations completed (Bunning et al. marked on the transcripts. The frequency was calculated
2007), a second framework was devised comprising for each category and the mean scores were displayed
communicative modalities. For example, Spoken En- in a table.
glish/Swahili/Giriama were defined alongside other com-
munication forms, such as Kenyan Sign Language and
Gesture. Additional codes were included to capture the Results
use of certain practice techniques, e.g. Sound Production
Interactional discourse
and Hands-on-Articulators. Brief summaries of the cod-
ing frameworks are provided in Appendix A.The codes The distribution of turns varied across dyads and part-
were applied to the printed transcripts using the recom- nership types as shown in table 3. Partnership type A
mended annotations. Communications directed outside dyads showed a fairly balanced distribution of turns be-
the adultchild dyad were excluded from the analysis. tween teacher and child. A similarly even distribution
was seen in dyad OT1/C4; however, the other two dyads
in partnership type B revealed greater turn usage by
Variability
OT2 than the children (C5 and C6). In partnership
A second researcher was trained in the use of the cod- type C, the dyadic interaction between C4 and her
ing framework and to recognize any idiosyncratic be- mother (M1) showed resonances of the interaction be-
haviours emitted by the participants and any word ap- tween C4 and OT1. There was lower turn occupation in
Practices to support communication needs in Kenya 695
Table 4. Matrices of transitional probabilities for two-event sequences (expressed as a percentage) in partnership type A dyads where
the teacher (T) is at lag 0; the child (C) is at lag 1

Lag 1: C1
Response (R-)
sp A F Act R NR NRE SSE
Lag 0: T1
Initiation (I-)
I 40 76 15 9
ICE 10 22 78
MP 30 100
A 10 100
Q 10 25 25 25 25
SSE 0
Lag 1: C2
Response (R-)
sp A F Act R NR NRE SSE
Lag 0: T2
Initiation (I-)
I 15 19 81
ICE 3.5 34 66
MP 76 80 15 5
A 4.5 25 75
Q 1 100
SSE 0
Lag 1: C3
Response (R-)
sp A F Act R NR NRE SSE
Lag 0: T3
Initiation (I-)
I 20 78 6 16
ICE 5 25 75
MP 60 90 5 5
A 2.5 100
Q 2.5 70 10 20
SSE 0
Note: sp = simple probability expressed as a percentage for a given move per session.

dyads M1/C5 and M2/C6 compared with all the other given in the curved brackets. Where Swahili is used a
dyads, with M2/C6 occupying no turns at all. translation is provided immediately afterwards in square
Across all partnership types, the moves at lag 0 were brackets.
adult initiations and the moves at lag 1 were child re-
sponses. Moves at lag 2 were seen mainly in partnership
Partnership type A
type A, and on two occasions by OT1/C4 (partnership
type B). Tables 46 display the dyad matrices according The mean number of two-event sequences in partner-
to partnership type for the two-event sequences (lag 0 ship type A dyads was generally high, showing little vari-
to lag 1). The column sp displays the simple probabili- ation and with minimal presence of follow-up moves at
ties signifying the likelihood of the given move at lag lag 2. For dyad T1/C1, the mean number of two-event
0 preceding any target moves during a session. Each sequences was 25.3, of which six response moves at lag
cell of the matrix shows the transitional probability for 1 were followed up at lag 2. Dyads T2/C2 and T2/C3
each possible pairing of adult to child (lag 0 to lag 1) involved the same teacher. For dyad T2/C2, the mean
move types, which indicates the probability of the tar- number of two-event sequences was 25, of which 1.3
get event occurring at lag 1 after the given event at moves were followed up at lag 2. For dyad T2/C3, the
lag 0. Excerpts of dyadic dialogue are provided as exam- mean number of two-event sequences was 28, of which
ples. Non-vocal turns and contextual information are 4.6 moves at lag 1 were followed up at lag 2.
696 Karen Bunning et al.
Table 5. Matrices of transitional probabilities for two-event sequences (expressed as a percentage) in partnership type B dyads where
the occupational therapist (OT) is at lag 0; the child (C) is at lag 1

Lag 1: C4
Response (R-)
sp A F Act R NR NRE SSE
Lag 0: OT1
Initiation (I-)
I 20 7 30 3 56 4
ICE 10 6 6 82 6
MP 50 73 2 10 12 3
A 0
Q 20 29 13 25 8 25
SSE 0
Lag 1: C5
Response (R-)
sp A F Act R NR NRE SSE
Lag 0: OT2
Initiation (I-)
I 28 5 86 9
ICE 28 1
MP 9 14 57.5 28.5
A 14 82 18
Q 0
SSE 20 6 94
Lag 1: C6
Response (R-)
sp A F Act R NR NRE SSE
Lag 0: OT3
Initiation (I-)
I 10 100
ICE 40 100
MP 20 100
A 10 100
Q 10 100
SSE 10 100
Note: sp = simple probability expressed as a percentage for a given move per session.

As shown in table 4, the most common initiation In dyads T2/C2 and T2/C3, the given move I-MP
move in dyad T1/C1 was Instruct (I-I = 40%) and then had the highest probability of occurring in a session,
Model/Prompt (I-MP = 30%), which had the strongest with a transitional probability for the target move R-
likelihood of triggering a target response of Active (R- Act occurring at 80% and 90% of the time in each dyad.
Act) by C1. The first excerpt features T1 sitting behind Instruct moves (I-I) had a lower probability of occurrence
a desk with C1 seated to the side. They are looking at than for dyad T1/C1. In dyad T2/C2 there was a strong
each other during articulation drill work, which lasted association between the given Instruct move and No
on this occasion for 14 separate turns. Response Expected (NRE). In both dyads T2/C2 and
T2/C3, the given moves Alignment (I-A) and Query
1. T1 (turns head slightly away) ah (looks back at (I-Q), whilst having a lower chance of occurring in a
C1) ba session, were more likely to be associated with a target
2. C1 (makes vocal soundapproximating target of NRE.
with nasal quality) ba
3. T1 a-a-a (vowel sound is prolonged)
Partnership type B
4. C1 (makes vocal soundapproximating target)
a-a For dyad OT1/C4, the mean number of two-event se-
5. T1 ba-a-a quences was 42.7, of which less than 1 move at lag 1
Practices to support communication needs in Kenya 697
Table 6. Matrices showing transitional probabilities for two-event sequences (expressed as a percentage) in partnership type C dyads,
where the mother (M) is at lag 0; the child (C) is at lag 1

Lag 1: C4
Response (R-)
sp A F Act R NR NRE SSE
Lag 0: M1
Initiation (I-)
I 75 5 36 5 40 14
ICE 14 9 18 73
MP 0
A 3.5 33 33 33
Q 6 20 20 20 40
SSE 2 50 50
Lag 1: C5
Response (R-)
sp A F Act R NR NRE SSE
Lag 0: M2
Initiation (I-)
I 41 93 7
ICE 21 79 21
MP 25 18 12 70
A 5 25 50 25
Q 0
SSE 8 100
Note: sp = simple probability expressed as a percentage for a given move per session.

was followed up at lag 2 per session. For dyads OT2/C5 Expected (NRE). Whilst Query (I-Q) had a similar rate
and OT2/C6 the mean number of two-event sequences of occurrence, C4 was more likely to respond with a
was much lower, 25.3 and 14.6 respectively. No moves target move of Acknowledgement (R-A), Reply (R-R) or
at lag 1 were followed up at lag 2. Action (R-Act).
As indicated in table 5, dyad OT1/C4 had the high- In dyad OT2/C5, I-MP constituted a majority of
est occurrence of Model/Prompt (I-MP) as a given move given moves with R-Act as the most likely transitional
with a strong association to Action (R-Act) by C4. This is move at lag 1. No Response Expected (NRE) was a
shown in excerpt 2, which opens with OT1 holding up a highly probable target response to the given initia-
small toy and saying frog in Swahili, which C4 attempts tions of Instruct (I-I) and Inform/Comment/Explain (I-
to copy. The pattern shows OT1 modelling the spoken ICE). Excerpt 3 shows OT2 giving an explanation of
word and C4 responding with her own approximation, her actions. She addresses C5 in Swahili whilst sitting
which ran to 12 turns on this occasion. on the floor with her hands placed around the childs
jaw and mouth.
1. OT2 hii ni nini [what is this?] (points to detail
on toy frog) 1. OT2 mmmmmmmm (moves hands either
2. C4 (places hand on OT1s outstretched fore- side of C5s jaws and mouth, making for a
arm) nimee (looks towards OT1) repeated openclose movement)
3. OT1 ah? (looks up to C4) 2. OT2 fanya hivi useme vizuri sawa? [do this to
4. C4 (continues to make attempts at speech talk well, ok?]
sounds)
5. OT1 ulimi [tongue] Dyad OT2/C6 revealed an apparently one-sided di-
6. C4 (continues to make attempts at speech alogue where given event I-ICE was most likely to oc-
sounds) cur, followed by Model/Prompt (I-MP), but with a 100%
7. OT1 ulimi [tongue] probability of transition to No Response Expected (NRE).
8. C4 eh bimu (upper limb movement accom-
panies speech efforts)
Partnership type C
The use of Instruct (I-I) had a lower rate of occur- The mean number of two-event sequences recorded
rence with about half being associated with No Response for dyads in partnership type C were M1/C4 = 28.3;
698 Karen Bunning et al.
Table 7. Summary of communicative modalities and practice techniques across partnership type dyads: mean scores per session

Partnership type A Partnership type B Partnership type C


Modalities and practice
techniques T1 C1 T2 C2 T2 C3 OT1 C4 OT2 C5 OT2 C6 M1 C4 M2 C5 M3 C6
Swahili 7 1 29 0 18 7.7 22 0.3 17
Giriama 6
English 27 6 16 0.3 2
Kenyan Sign Language 1 1.3 0.7
Speech production 9 15 24 19 12 18 17 11 2 0.7 1.3
Oromotor practice 1.3 0.3 0.3 0 0.3
Gesture 21 11 4 11 3.3 8.3 9.3 2.3 4.3 1 5 4 4.7 0.7 6.7 0.5 1
Hands-on-Articulators 9.7 6 2.7 0.7 2.3 9.7 6.7 0.5
Sound input 2.3
Motor response 2 7.7 1 2 2.7 2 1.7 0.7 0.3 4.3 8.3 6 0.7

M2/C5 = 22.6 with no follow-up moves from lag 1 to used Swahili mainly. Speech Production acts were preva-
lag 2. No moves were recorded for dyad M3/C6, where lent in partnership type A dyads and in dyad OT1/C4
the mother engaged in extending and flexing the childs (partnership type B). Gesture was used mainly by the
limbs. teachers (T1 and T2) with a more balanced dyadic usage
As shown in table 6, there was a high probabil- in OT1/C4 (partnership type B.) The practice technique
ity of the mothers in dyads M1/C4 and M2/C5 using Hands-on-Articulators was used by all the practitioners.
the given Instruct (I-I) move. This was associated al- The mothers (partnership type C) spoke in Swahili
most evenly with No Response Expected (NRE) and Ac- with some support from Gesture. The small presence of
tion (R-Act) in dyad M1/C4, and mainly with NRE Gesture and Hands-on-Articulators in dyad M3/C6 did
as the target in dyad M2/C5. Model/Prompt (I-MP) not constitute interactional turns, but were representa-
as the next most likely given move, had a strong as- tive of small body behaviours.
sociation to NRE. The small presence of Query (I-Q)
as a given move was associated with Acknowledgement
(R-A), Action (R-Act) and Reply (R-R), although there Discussion
was a 40% probability of a No Response (NR). In dyad
Turns were distributed fairly evenly between the teach-
M2/C5 Self/Shared Expression (SSE) was a likely target
ers and pupils in Partnership A and dyad OT1/C4 in
move to the given I-MP particularly. NRE was majorly
Partnership B. The remaining dyads in Partnerships B
associated with the given moves of I-I, I-ICE and I-
and C revealed the dominant role of the adult. Initi-
MP. This is illustrated in excerpt 3 where C5 is standing
ating moves were used exclusively by the adults across
against a therapy roll, with his mother holding onto his
all the partnership types with the child as the respon-
shoulders whilst supporting the small of his back. The
dent. Follow-up moves at lag 2 in the form of Feedback
mother talks to her child as she adjusts his position.
and Reply were used mainly by the teachers in part-
nership type A, although not consistently. The teach-
1. M2 C5 haya shika chini [hold down] (adjusts ers variously favoured the Instruct and Model/Prompt
C5s clothes and also his position) moves, which were similarly used by the occupational
2. C5 (continues in position) therapist in dyad OT1/C4. In correspondence to the
3. M2 angalia chini. [look down] weka mikono adults in these dyads, the children mainly responded
chini [put hands down] (moves roll forward with Action moves. In OT2/C5 and OT2/C6, there
with child whilst holding onto the backs of was preferred use of Inform/Comment/Explain by the
his legs) occupational therapist with low levels of Action re-
4. C5 (continues in position) sponses being recorded for the children. Partnership
5. M2 haya angalia [okay look] (tips roll forwards) type C (caregiverchild) revealed higher Instruct and
6. C5 (continues in position) Inform/Comment/Explain moves by the adults, whereas
7. M2 shika chini. shika chini tena. [hold down. the children used more Self-Shared Expression. Com-
hold down again] (holds onto C5s legs) municative modalities and practice techniques revealed
reliance on speech generally amongst the adults across
all partnership types. Both Speech production and Hands-
Communicative modalities and practice techniques
on-Articulators featured in partnership types A and B.
Table 7 summarizes the communicative modality usage. The teachers execution of an instructional, speech
Partnership type A dyads employed English as the spoken training approach with the children produced pat-
medium during a session, whereas partnership type B terns that were resonant of the transmissional forms of
Practices to support communication needs in Kenya 699
teaching identified by Abd-Kadir and Hardman (2007) Pennington 2008, Pennington and McConachie 1999).
and Ackers and Hardman (2001). This may explain the This might explain how the recorded Acknowledgements
fairly even distribution of turns between adult and child emitted by C4 in response to Initiation-Query were not
playing out an initiation-response sequence. The inter- recognized by the occupational therapist (OT1). This
actional discourse in these dyads followed a recurring suggests inadequate knowledge and understanding of
sequence of Model/Prompt or Instruct to Action, similar communication development, in particular at the earli-
to the teacher-led recitation patterns found by Ponte- est stages, underpinned by a lack of specialist training
fract and Hardman (2005). The teachers limited use associated with the new responsibilities. In addition, the
of Feedback moves at lag 2, echoed findings reported given moves at lag 0, i.e. Inform/Comment/Explain or
by Hardman et al. (2009). It appears that the teaching Instruct were mainly rhetorical and required no response
frames of the classroom were imported into the therapy of the child, thereby indicating therapist control of the
session in Partnership type A. dyads. The similarities discourse, which affected a reduction in communication
seen in dyad OT1/C4 (partnership type B.) may have opportunities for the child similar to that observed by
been affected by the therapists personal experience and Abd-Kadir and Hardman (2007). Whether this was a bi-
cultural knowledge of the teachinglearning process as product of therapy that was about doing something to
summarized by Hardman and Abd-Kadir (2010). the child, e.g. manipulation of oral-musculature, which
A quite different theoretical orientation was in- was not conducive to two-way interaction, or a lack
dicated by the interactional discourse involving the of sensitivity to early, non speech communication be-
younger children (C5 and C6). Although the two-event haviours as described by Marfo et al. (1998) is debatable.
sequences were still adult-led, therapist initiation moves The similar control exerted by the mothers in part-
were linked to No Response Expected (NRE). This may nership type C raises the question of contextual influ-
be explained by the therapy activity of wiping ice blocks ences at work. Both partnership types C. and B. were
and warm cloths around the childs face, which effec- enacted in the hospital department where the mothers
tively cast the child in a passive role with the discourse were exposed to occupational therapy practice applied to
proceeding as a therapist-delivered commentary. The their children. What is notable is that the mothers relied
medical model in the hospital setting may have influ- more on Instruct moves and less on Model/Prompt, com-
enced the use of special techniques and equipment and pared with partnership type B dyads. This was borne
could be seen to support the dominance of the health out by the absence of the modalities/practice techniques
professional (Sherry 2010). No formal label was used of Speech Production, Oral-motor Practice and Hands-on-
by the occupational therapists to describe these activ- Articulators, although there was a greater concentration
ities; however, there appears to be parallels with two of Motor Responses from the child during physical exer-
particular intervention approaches. The first is a branch cises under the mothers instructions. However, Gesture
of proprioceptive neuromuscular facilitation, which is was used by both mother and child in dyads M1/C4
used by occupational therapists and physiotherapists to and M2/C5, which may reflect greater maternal sensi-
promote muscle flexibility. Called pseudo neuromuscu- tivity to a child who has difficulties in the area of speech.
lar facilitation, it was developed in the 1970s for the Alternatively, it could be that the mothers viewed ma-
treatment of acquired motor speech disorders (Langley nipulation and icing as a specialist activity requiring
and Darvill 1979) and involved applying ice to the neu- the particular expertise of the therapists. Of course it
ral pathways of the oral musculature. The second is a would most certainly have been the case that ice blocks
collection of exercises designed to increase strength, tone would not have been available at the homesteads.
and control of the oral musculature termed non-speech In addition to activity selection, the setting cul-
oral motor treatments (Powell 2008). Both approaches ture appears to have influenced the choice of language.
have been largely discredited due to a lack of evidence Speech was the dominant communication modality
supporting their efficacy (Ruscello 2008). used by adults across all the partnerships, consistent with
In addition to the underlying pedagogies of prac- its valued status in educational discourse (Abd-Kadir
tice, factors concerning the childs age, condition and and Hardman 2007) and the oral traditions associated
developmental stage may have influenced the discourse with African culture (Hartley et al. 1998). English as
structure. The dyads OT2/C5 and OT2/C6 (partner- the language of education also featured in the schools
ship type B.) featured younger children, for whom the therapy sessions (Pontefract and Hardman 2005). As the
early stage communication behaviours seen in typically school was residential the children were effectively de-
developing children were compromised by the primary nied development opportunities in their mother tongue.
condition of cerebral palsy. This affected their motor In addition, the school for the deaf followed an oral ap-
skills for verbal and non-verbal representations of mean- proach to communication, which likely governed the
ing (Pennington 2008). Thus the likelihood of the adults goal of speech production. In contrast, the language of
overlooking small or idiosyncratic, seemingly insignifi- the childs home was used in the occupational therapy
cant behaviours was a possibility (Bunning et al. 2013, department, which indirectly acknowledged the role
700 Karen Bunning et al.
of the home and the family in supporting the childs the observational methodology meant that practitioner
development. Typical practice with children with de- confidence outweighed such considerations. During the
velopmental disorders affecting communication would study period it was noted that one of the occupational
usually involve some combination of augmentative therapists changed her practice by ceasing the use
and alternative communication strategies (Pennington of icing and related techniques with the particular
2008); however, partnership types A and B were charac- participant, but was informally observed to continue
terized by speech sound production and manipulation their use at other times. Capacity-building workshops
of the oral musculature. carried out as part of the large scale project may have
The extent to which the practitioners were prepared influenced the therapist in question to try out new
for the additional responsibilities associated with the practices or to conceal usual practice due to raised
missing profession, i.e. speech and language therapy, awareness. Inclusion of motherchild interaction was
must be questioned. Training in phonetics, phonology originally planned as an informal comparator to the
and associated therapy techniques has likely influenced practice in partnership types A and B. Video capture
the teachers use of multi-sensory techniques for mod- during attendance of the occupational therapy depart-
elling sound production targets, which is resonant of ment was not ideal and likely influenced the choice of
practice described by Beazley et al. (2001). However, physical activities carried out. The homestead, whilst
the lack of feedback following the instructional two- less convenient, would have been a more natural setting
event sequences reflected the learning by rote reported for observing motherchild interactions; however, this
by Pontefract and Hardman (2005), with a lack of spe- was not possible due to resource limitations.
cific techniques to facilitate target accuracy. Continu-
ing professional development recorded for the occupa-
tional therapists seemed to be about raising awareness
Conclusions
via knowledge exchange on matters such as HIV and
AIDS, and skills training in areas relevant to the profes- In the absence of a speech and language therapy service
sion, with no specialist training recorded for the extra in this rural part of Kenya, there has been the assign-
duties associated with speech, language and communica- ment of duties to teachers of the deaf and occupational
tion needs. It is possible that the occupational therapists therapists. However, practice appears to be determined
practice, with its focus on oral-motor skills and mo- by the pedagogies associated with the original profes-
tor production of speech, stemmed from their original sional training. The teachers had received some pre-
diploma studies, where undoubtedly human anatomy qualification training in phonetics and phonology, al-
and physiology will have featured. The practice in both though practice was reminiscent of classroom discourse
partnership types A and B seem to tackle surface level in Kenya. The occupational therapists had not un-
processes affecting speech production, with apparently dertaken any specific training relevant to the support
limited attention to pre-requisite competencies, such as of communication development. They used techniques
symbolic development, or indeed communication by that demonstrated inadequate knowledge of communi-
means other than speech. Thus it may be that staff prac- cation. However, the mothers were present and therapy
tised what they had been trained for and not for the was conducted in the language of the home. Speech
additional responsibilities assumed. and sound production was emphasized with limited re-
course to language acquisition and the development of
concepts. The instrumental benefits of such practice
Limitations
and the capacity for generalization into everyday life are
This was a small-scale study of children with complex questionable.
communication needs associated with either hearing Whilst communication between the mothers and
impairment or cerebral palsy, interacting with adults in their children showed resonances of the communication
three types of communication partnership. The results, seen in occupational therapy sessions, their shared use
therefore, are not representative, but nevertheless of gesture and the spontaneous self-/shared expression
serve to illustrate some of the features of interactional by the children are possibly indicative of a more natural
discourse between teachers, occupational therapists and and participative interaction style. Development of sup-
mothers with such children. Nomination of the child port for children with complex communication needs
participants by the service heads was not ideal and and their families in low-income countries requires con-
introduced bias to the sample, as they may have chosen sideration of the range of augmentative and alternative
children more capable of producing speech. It was orig- communication strategies that might support a more
inally planned to recruit a purposive, stratified sample functional approach; the familys contribution as poten-
covering key age ranges and underlying conditions tial agents for change; and finally the training require-
associated with communication disorders; however, ments for effective expansion of professional roles.
Practices to support communication needs in Kenya 701
Acknowledgements ships to develop a culturally relevant intervention for children
with communication disabilities in Kenya. Disability and Re-
The authors would like to thank the Director of the Kenya Medical habilitation, 31, 490499.
Research Institute for permission to publish the data. Gratitude is HEILMANN, J., NOCKERTS, A. and MILLER, J. F., 2010, Language
expressed to the CP Charitable Trust for its generosity in funding the sampling: does length of the transcript matter? Language,
project. Thanks must also go to the Wellcome Trust at KEMRI and Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 393404.
the services across the health and educational rehabilitation sectors of International World Federation of Occupational Therapy, 2012,
Kilifi District. Professor Charles Newton is funded by the Wellcome Human Resources Project (available at: http://www.wfot.org/
Trust. Declaration of interest: The authors report no conflicts of ResourceCentre.aspx) (accessed on 12 November 2012).
interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing JOCHMANN, A., 2006, Speech and language treatment in East Africa.
of the paper ASHA Leader, 7 February (available at: http://www.asha.org/
Publications/leader/2006/060207/f060207b.htm#4) (ac-
cessed on 12 November 2012).
References KARANGWA, E., MILES, S. and LEWIS, I., 2010, Community level
responses to disability and education in Rwanda. International
ABD-KADIR, J. and HARDMAN, F., 2007, The discourse of whole Journal of Disability Rehabilitation and Education, 57, 267
class teaching: a comparative study of Kenyan and Nigerian 278.
primary English lessons. Language and Education, 21, 115. LANGLEY, J. and DARVILL, G., 1979, Procedures for Facilitating Im-
ACKERS, J. and HARDMAN, F., 2001, Classroom interaction in provements in Swallow, Mastication, Speech and Facial Expres-
Kenyan primary schools. Compare: Journal of Comparative sion Where These Have Been Impaired by Central or Peripheral
and International Education, 31, 245261. Nerve Damage (London: NHCSS).
ADEMOKOYA, J. A. and OLUJIDE, M. G., 2007, Typology and inter- MARFO, K., DEDRICK, C. and BARBOUR, N., 1998, Motherchild
ventions for some social problems affecting the learning of the interactions and the development of children with mental re-
hearing impaired child. International Journal of Rehabilitation tardation. In J. A. Burack, R. M. Hodapp and E. Zigler (eds),
Research, 30, 7579. Handbook of Mental Retardation and Development (Cam-
BAKEMAN, R. and GOTTMAN, J. M., 1997, Observing Interaction: bridge: Cambridge University Press), pp. 637668.
An Introduction to Sequential Analysis, 2nd edn (Cambridge: MONT, D., 2007, Measuring Disability Prevalence (Washington, DC:
Cambridge University Press). The World Bank, Disability & Development Team, Social
BAKEMAN, R. and QUERA, V., 2011, Sequential Analysis and Ob- Protection Unit).
servational Methods for the Behavioral Sciences (Cambridge: MUNGALA-ODERA, V., MEEHAN, R., NJUGUNA, P., MTURI, N., AL-
Cambridge University Press). COCK, K. J. and NEWTON, C. R., 2006, Prevalence and risk
BEAZLEY, S., FROST, R. and HALDEN, J., 2001, Working with deaf factors of neurological disability and impairment in children
children. In M. Kersner and J. A. Wright (eds), Speech and living in rural Kenya. International Journal of Epidemiology,
Language Therapy: The Decision-Making Process when Working 35, 683688.
with Children (London: David Fulton), pp. 163176. MUUYA, J., 2002, The aims of special education schools and units in
BUNNING, K., GEERE, J., CHIA, S., GONA, J. and HARTLEY, S., 2007, Kenya: a survey of headteachers. European Journal of Special
Report on Visits to Kilifi District Hospital and Wellcome-Funded Needs Education, 17, 229239.
Kenya Medical Research Institute (Norwich: University of East MWANJABE, T. T., 2010, Children with developmental delay. In V.
Anglia, School of Allied Health Professions). Alers and R. Crouch (eds), Occupational Therapy: An African
BUNNING, K., SMITH, C., KENNEDY, P. and GREENHAM, C., 2013, Perspective (Johannesburg: Sarah Shorten), pp. 190204.
Examination of the communication interface between stu- OLUSANYA, B. O., RUBEN, R. J. and PARVIN, G. A., 2006, Reduc-
dents with severe to profound and multiple intellectual dis- ing the burden of communication disorders in the develop-
ability and educational staff during structured teaching ses- ing world: an opportunity for the millennium development
sions. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 57, 3952. project. Journal of the American Medical Association, 296, 441
CARLETTA, J., ISARD, A., ISARD, S., KOWTKO, J., DOHERTY- 444.
SNEDDON, G. and ANDERSON, A., 1996, HCRC Dialogue PENNINGTON, L., 2008, Cerebral palsy and communication. Paedi-
Structure Coding Manual. HCRC TR-82 (Edinburgh: Uni- atrics and Child Health, 18, 405409.
versity of Edinburgh, Human Communication Research PENNINGTON, L. and MCCONACHIE, H., 1999, Motherchild in-
Centre). teraction revisited: communication with non-speaking phys-
CRYSTAL, D., 1992, Profiling Linguistic Ability (London: Whurr). ically disabled children. International Journal of Language and
FLEISS, J. L., 1981, Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions (New Communication Disorders, 34, 391416.
York, NY: Wiley). PONTEFRACT, C. and HARDMAN, F., 2005, The discourse of class-
HARDMAN, F. and ABD-KADIR, J., 2010, Classroom discourse; to- room interaction in Kenyan primary schools. Comparative
wards a dialogic pedagogy. In D. Wyse, R. Andrews and Education, 41, 87106.
J. Hoffman (eds), The International Handbook of English Lan- POWELL, T. W., 2008, The use of nonspeech oral motor treatments
guage and Literacy (London: Routledge), pp. 254264. for developmental speech sound production disorders: in-
HARDMAN, F., ABD-KADIR, J., AGG, C., MIGWI, J., NDAMBUKU, terventions and interactions. Language Speech and Hearing
J. and SMITH, F., 2009, Changing pedagogical practice in Services in Schools, 39, 374379.
Kenyan primary schools: the impact of school-based training. RUSCELLO, D. M., 2008, Non-speech oral motor treatment issues re-
Comparative Education, 45(1), 6586. lated to children with developmental speech sound disorders.
HARTLEY, S., 1998, Children with verbal communication disabilities Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 380391.
in Eastern Uganda. African Journal of Special Needs Education, SHERRY, K., 2010, Culture and cultural competence for occupational
1, 1118. therapists in Africa. In V. Alers and R. Crouch (eds), Occu-
HARTLEY, S., MURIRA, G., MWANGOMA, M., CARTER, J. and NEW- pational Therapy: An African Perspective (Johannesburg: Sarah
TON, R. J. C., 2009, Using community/researcher partner- Shorten), pp. 6077.
702 Karen Bunning et al.
STALEY, B., 2013, Speech language therapy in East Africa: develop- tative and alternative communication. In E. Alant and L.
ments in the region. S&L World, February (available at: http:// L. Lloyd (eds), Augmentative & Alternative Communica-
www.slworldbulletin.com) (accessed on 10 April 2013). tion & Severe Disabilities: Beyond Poverty (London: Whurr),
THE WORLD BANK, 2011, Population Total (available at: http://data. pp. 3067.
worldbank.org/indicator/SP.POP.TOTL) (accessed on 30 WORLD HEALTH ORGANISATION (WHO), 1997, Lets Communicate:
November 2012). A Handbook for People Working with Children with Commu-
TOMASELLO, M. and STAHL, D., 2004, Sampling childrens sponta- nication Difficulties (Geneva: WHO).
neous speech: how much is enough? Journal of Child Lan- WORLD HEALTH ORGANISATION (WHO), 2011, Rehabilitation. In
guage, 31, 101121. World Report on Disability (Geneva: WHO and The World
TONSING, K. M., ALANT, E. and LLOYD, L. L., 2005, Augmen- Bank), pp. 93123.

Appendix. Coding frameworks

Initiation move types


Instruct Commands or gives instruction, usually related to the current activity
Inform, comment or explain States information not solicited by another person; provides a commentary for their own actions; asks
self-directed, rhetorical question; explains or states a reason for a circumstance
Model or prompt Demonstrates a target response as a model for another person to copy; gives a prompt or a cue to other
person, which can take a vocal or a non-vocal form for completion by another person
Align Calls for other persons attention in the absence of clear instruction or gives a simple name call and
pause before the next move; attracts attention to detail
Query Asks other person a question that takes a yes/no answer; solicits information in an open-ended form;
solicits a response to a forced alternative; follows a proposition with a brief check on another persons
understanding/readiness/compliance
Response move types
Attend or acknowledge Aligns attention to an object or a person showing that something has been heard or seen, but it falls
short of a deliberate response like an action; use of back channels to indicate engagement
Feedback Evaluates a prior move by another person
Action Responds to an instruction given with or without a model, prompt or situational cue
Reply Replies as appropriate to various forms of query: gives a yes/no surface form meaning yes or no;
provides information solicited; completes the stem of a given phrase
No response Occurs when a turn change has been signalled in a previous turn, but no or little response is made.
There may be the intimation of confusion or insecurity in the persons gestures
Practice modalities and techniques
Speech Swahili Utterances spoken in Swahili
Speech Giriama Utterances spoken in Giriama
Speech English Utterances spoken in English
Kenyan Sign Language Hand shapes and movements derived from Kenyan Sign Language
Speechsound production Attempt at speechsound production
Oral production Attempt at oro-muscular production (lip closure, puffing cheeks, etc.)
Gesture Pointing, facial expression, handarm or body gestureincludes the use of materials for multisensory
feedback in speech production activities
Hands-on-Articulators Touch contact or approximation with oral-facial structures
Auditory input Sound input directed specifically around the ears and head
Motor response Execution of motor skills or action

You might also like