You are on page 1of 34

Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research

1992, SR-1111112, 227-260

Diversity and Commonality in Music Performance: An


Analysis of Timing Microstructure in Schumann's
uTraumerei"*

Bruno H. Repp

This study attempts to characterize the temporal commonalities and differences among
distinguished pianists' interpretations of a well-known piece, Robert Schumann's
"Triiumerei." Intertone onset intervals (lOIs) were measured in 28 recorded performances.
These data were subjected to a variety of statistical analyses, including principal
components analysis of longer stretches of music and curve fitting to series of lOIs within
brief melodic gestures. Global timing patterns reflected the hierarchical grouping
structure of the composition, with pronounced ritardandi at the ends of major sections and
frequent expressive lengthening of accented tones within melodic gestures. Analysis of
local timing patterns, particularly of within-gesture ritardandi, revealed that they often
followed a parabolic timing function. The major variation in these patterns can be
modelled by families of parabolas with a single degree of freedom. The grouping structure,
which prescribes the location of major tempo changes, and the parabolic timing function,
which represents a natural manner of executing such changes, seem to be the two major
constraints under which pianists are operating. Within these constraints, there is room for
much individual variation, and there are always exceptions to the rules. The striking
individuality of two legendary pianists, Alfred Cortot and Vladimir Horowitz, is objectively
demonstrated here, as is the relative eccentricity of several other artists.

INTRODUCTION Young musicians compete for career opportunities


by entering competitions in which their perfor-
A. Diversity and commonality in music mances are compared and evaluated by juries (see
performance Cline, 1985; Horowitz, 1990). While ever new ren-
More than at any earlier period in musical ditions of the standard repertoire vie for the at-
history, the contemporary scene in serious music tention of record buyers and concert audiences, a
is dominated by the performer (see Lipman, 1990). spate of reissues of historical recordings on CDs is
Music consumers thrive on a limited repertoire of offering stiff competition. For some of the more
standard masterworks, primarily from the 19th popular works, the Schwann catalogue lists
century, that are offered again and again in dozens of performances; if deleted records, avail-
different performances, both in live concerts and able in libraries and private collections, are
on recordings. The great musical events of our counted, they may run into the hundreds. There
time are not the premieres of new compositions, have never been such ample opportunities to com-
but the appearances and reappearances of pare different performances of the same music.
superstar conductors and instrumentalists. Obviously, this remarkable diversity reflects not
only clever marketing and the promotion of su-
This research was made possible by the generosity of perstars, but also the ability of music lovers to dis-
Haskins Laboratories (Carol A. Fowler, President). Additional tinguish and appreciate different performances.
support came from NIH (BRSG) grant RR05596 to the
Laboratories. I am grateful to the Record Library, School of
The more sophisticated among these listeners de-
Music, Yale University, for providing most of the recordings tect qualities in the performances of individual
analyzed here, and to its manager, Karl Schrom, for his help. artists that lead them to search out these per-
Thanks also to Pat Shove for many stimulating discussions. formers' concerts and recordings. They may voice

227
228 Repp

OpInIOnS about the quality of particular perfor- That way, it is possible for different artists to
mances by these and other artists, describing meet the norm equally and yet be discriminably
them as "brilliant" or "noble" or "thoughtful." different. For example, two pianists may be
Some of the more gifted professional critics excel equally adept in expressing a particular musical
in characterizing different performances in terms structure in their performances, but one may
alternatingly scholarly and poetic. choose a slow tempo and large tempo changes,
While the attention of listeners and critics thus whereas the other may prefer a faster tempo and
is mainly drawn to the differences among perfor- smaller deviations from the rhythmic beat. The
mances, there are also strong commonalities, usu- former artist may then be characterized as
ally taken for granted and hence unnoticed. "romantic" or "exuberant," while the latter will
Though there is a large variety of acceptable per- evoke epithets such as "restrained" and "noble,"
formances of a given piece of music, there is an though both may receive equal acclaim from a
even larger variety of unacceptable performances, sensitive audience.
which rarely make their way into the concert halls The commonality-diversity distinction seems
or onto records. Unless they have the mark of in- obvious enough, but there is little tangible evi~
spired iconoclasm (as do some of the performances dence to substantiate it. Although volumes have
by the late Glenn Gould; see, e.g., Lipman, 1984), been written about different performers and their
they quickly succumb to the fierce competition of characteristics, these discussions rarely go beyond
the musical marketplace. Music teachers, how- generalities, and the vocabulary used (such as the
ever, have to deal with them every day and try adjectives quoted above) are not specifically linked
their best to mold immature and wayward stu- to particular performance properties; they also
dents into performers that can be listened to with may be used differently by different writers. There
pleasure. Though teachers may differ considerably is no consistent terminology, nor a well-developed
in their methods and goals, and are rarely very and generally accepted theory of performance de-
explicit about what these are, they are transmit- scription and evaluation, that would make possi-
ting the unwritten rules (though see Lussy, 1882) ble an objective characterization of performance
of a performance tradition that goes back to 19th commonalities and differences. Music criticism is
century central Europe, where most of the an art rather than a science, and the critic's im-
standard repertoire originated. Despite various pressions, accurate as they may be, are filtered
changes in performance practices during the last through an idiosyncratic web of personal experi-
200 years, most of them of a narrowly technical ences, expectations, preferences, and semantic as-
nature, there are generally accepted norms of mu- sociations. In fact, the belief is widespread that
sical performance, according to which the artist's performance differences cannot be characterized
actions are largely subordinated to the musical objectively.
structure. The artist's primary task is the expres- Such a negative conclusion, however, can only
sion of the musical structure, so it can be grasped be justified if objective performance analysis has
and appreciated by the listener, and make an im- been attempted and has failed. A total failure is
pression on him or her. (See Lussy, 1882; highly unlikely, however, for there are many
Riemann, 1884; Stein, 1962.) This is presumably physical properties of a musical performance that,
done by conventional means that are adapted to without any question, can be measured objec-
the hearers' perceptual and cognitive abilities. tively. Whether objective analysis can capture a
However, the particular doses in which these performance exhaustively may remain in doubt;
techniques are applied (unconsciously, for the the proper question is how much can be learned
most part) vary from artist to artist and account from it. Surely, even a partial characterization in
for individual differences in interpretation. terms of verifiable and replicable observations can
Thus there are two basic aspects of music per- make a valuable contribution to our understand-
formance: a normative aspect (i.e., commonality) ing of performance commonalities and differences, .
that represents what is expected from a competent which remain mostly a mystery to this day. For
performer and is largely shared by different example, most music lovers would agree that
artists, and an individual aspect (i.e., diversity) Artur Rubinstein and Vladimir Horowitz were two
that differentiates performers. The individual as- very different pianists, whose performances of
pect may be conceived of as deviations from a sin- similar repertoire (e.g., Chopin) are instantly dis-
gle ideal norm; more profitably, however, it may tinguishable. But what is it, really, that makes
be thought of as individual settings of free param- them so different and individual? And do they
eters in the definition of the normative behavior. have anything in common at all? It is all too easy
Diversitv and Commonalitv in Music Performance: An ,4na/vsis of Timmg Microstructure In Schumann's "Triiumcrc;P 229

to couch the answers to such questions in terms of however, several researchers have taken up the
"artistic personality," which do not enlighten us at topic again. Their work, with few exceptions, has
all about the nature of the differences and com- taken a case study approach; their focus was not
monalities. Objective performance analysis has a on individual differences but on the instantiation
contribution to make here. of certain principles in (hopefully, representative)
performance samples, mostly from pianists. Thus,
B. Objective performance analysis Shaffer (1980, 1981, 1984, 1989; Shaffer, Clarke,
The concept of objective performance analysis & Todd, 1985) examined the timing of piano
goes back to Carl Seashore (1936, 1938, 1947) and performances from the perspective of motor
his collaborators, who pioneered the use of programming and control (see also Povel, 1977).
acoustic analysis techniques to derive His erstwhile students, Eric Clarke and Neil
"performance scores" that show the exact Todd, went on to make significant contributions of
variations of pitch, timing, and intensity produced their own. Clarke (1982, 1985) conducted case
by an artist on some instrument. A considerable studies of piano performances of Satie's music and
amount of data was collected in Seashore's wrote at length on how musical structures are
laboratory, but their analyses remained expressed in timing variations (Clarke, 1983,
rudimentary and focused primarily on technical 1988). Todd (1985, 1989; Shaffer & Todd, 1987)
aspects such as pitch accuracy and vibrato in developed a computational model of timing at the
singing and string playing, and chord synchrony phrase level, which has been revised and extended
on the piano. Although some studies compared to dynamics in his most recent publications (Todd,
different performances of the same music, no 1992a, 1992b). Bengtsson and Gabrielsson (1980;
statistical characterization of commonalities and Gabrielsson, 1974; Gabrielsson, Bengtsson, &
differences was attempted, nor were the results Gabrielsson, 1983) conducted extensive studies of
interpreted with more than a passing reference to the performance of different rhythm patterns in
musical structure. These limitations reflect the relatively simple musical contexts, but with
behavioristic approach of American psychology at attention to individual differences. Gabrielsson
the time, as well as the unavailability of (1985, 1988) has written more generally about
psychological performance models and advanced timing in music performance. A provocative theory
statistical methods. Nevertheless, Seashore (1947, of "composer's pulse" in performance timing has
p. 77) was able to reach conclusions that reinforce been developed by Clynes (1983, 1987).
the premises of the present study: There are few studies in the literature that em-
ployed what one might consider a representative
... there is a common stock of principles which sample of different performances. Gabrielsson
competent artists tend to observe; ... We should (1987) conducted a detailed comparison of five pi-
not, of course, assume that there is only one way
anists' performances of the first eight measures of
of phrasing a given selection, but, even with such Mozart's Piano Sonata in A major, K. 331; the
freedom, two artists will reveal many common study included measurements of timing, intensi-
principles of artistic deviation. Furthermore,
ties, and articulation (i.e., tone durations). Palmer
insofar as there are consistent differences in their (1989) studied the same music as performed by six
phrasing, these differences may reveal elements
pianists in "musical" and "unmusical" styles, and
of musical individuality.
she analyzed the timing patterns, note asyn-
Contemporary with Seashore's work, similar chronies, and articulation. Palmer also recorded
research was going on in Germany. Hartmann eight pianists playing the first 16 measures of a
(1932) compared the timing patterns of two Brahms Intermezzo and studied timing patterns
famous pianists' performances of a Beethoven in relation to intended phrasing. She concluded
sonata movement and provided detailed numerical that "pianists share a common set of expressive
descriptions of the differences between them. The timing methods for translating musical intentions
small size of the sample, however, limits the into sounded performance" (p. 345). These studies
generality of his conclusions. Although one of the still used relatively small samples and obtained
two artists seemed quite eccentric, this impression only limited amounts of data from each performer.
cannot be substantiated without more extensive The largest amount of performance data was
comparisons. analyzed in Repp's (1990) study, which included
The three decades between roughly 1940 and 19 complete performances by famous pianists of a
1970 were barren years for the objective study of Beethoven sonata movement. The analysis was
music performance. In the last two decades, limited, however, in that it concerned primarily
230 Repp

timing patterns at the level of quarter-note beats. reflect an adult's recollection of childhood
Also, the focus of the study was a search for (Brendel, 1981; Chissell, 1987).
Clynes' (1983) elusive "Beethoven pulse" in the "Traumerei" occupies a central position in the
timing patterns. Following the example of "Kinderszenen" suite, not only by its location but
Bengtsson and Gabrielsson (1980), Repp applied by its duration and structural role. It serves as a
principal components (factor) analysis to these resting and turning point in the cycle, which
timing data, to determine how many independent shows so many intricate thematic connections that
timing patterns were instantiated in this sizeable it may be considered a set of free variations (Reti,
sample of expert performances. Two factors 1951; Traub, 1981). Its key signature, meter, and
emerged, the first representing primarily phrase- motivic content also single it out as the hub of the
final lengthening, while the second factor captured suite (Brendel, 1981; Traub, 1981). However, it is
other types of expressive timing variation. Musical also often performed by itself, both by classical
listeners' evaluations of the performances were artists (e.g., it was one of Horowitz's favorite
also obtained, and some relationships between the encores) and in numerous popular versions and
measured timing patterns and listeners' judg- arrangements. Indeed, "Traumerei" is perhaps the
ments were found. most popular Romantic piano piece. Its score is
The present study continued the general ap- shown in Figure 1.
proach taken by Repp (1990), but without the aim The melodic/rhythmic structure (or grouping
of testing Clynes's theory of composer's pulse. Its structure; see Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983) of
main purpose was to assemble a large sample of "Traumerei" is depicted schematically in Figure 2,
performances of a particular composition by out- which also introduces terminology to be used
standing artists, and to analyze the timing pat- throughout this paper. The layout of the figure
terns in detail, using various statistical methods. corresponds to that of the score in Figure 1, and
These methods, it was hoped, would make it pos- bars (measures) are numbered. The piece is
sible to separate commonalities from differences. composed of three 8-bar periods (A, B, A'), the first
The common patterns would reveal how most pi- of which is obligatorily repeated. Each period is
anists transmit musical structure and expression subdivided into two 4-bar phrases, which are
through timing variations, and it was of interest represented by staff systems in Figure 1 and by
to determine whether there would be a single large rectangular boxes in Figure 2. (Actually, the
common factor or several. The characterization of beginning and end of each phrase extend slightly
individual differences was also of interest, espe- beyond the four bars, overlapping with the
cially with respect to some of the legendary pi- preceding and following phrases, respectively.)
anists in the sample, who are known for their There are two phrase types, a and b, each of which
individuality. recurs three times with slight variations
The music chosen for this investigation was a (indicated by subscripts in Figure 2). Phrase al
well-known piano piece from the Romantic period, (period A) is repeated literally in period A'.
"Traumerei" by Robert Schumann. It was selected Phrases b2 and b3, which constitute period B, are
because it is a higWy expressive piece that permits structurally identical but differ in key, harmony,
much freedom in performance parameters, and . and some other details.
hence much room for individual. differences in The melodic events are divided horizontally
interpretation. Also, there are numerous record- (roughly, along the dimension of relative pitch)
ings available. among four registers or voices (S = soprano, A =
alto, T = tenor, B = bass). Vertically (along the
I. THE MUSIC dimension of metrical distance), the events within
"Traumerei" ("Reverie," "Dreaming") is the phrases are grouped into melodic gestures, which
seventh of the 13 short pieces that constitute are represented by filled boxes in Figure 2.
Robert Schumann's (1810-1856) "Kinderszenen" Characteristically, they extend across bar lines
("Scenes from Childhood"), op. 15. This little suite, (vertical solid and dashed lines in Figure 2). A
universally considered one of the masterpieces in melodic gesture (MG) is an expressive unit com-
its genre, was composed by Schumann in 1838 posed of at least two and rarely more than seven
when he was secretly engaged to Clara Wieck. The successive tones. It is defined here to begin with
pieces were selected from some 30 pieces the onset of its first tone and to end with the onset
composed for Clara around that time, and their of its last tone. This seems reasonable, for a pi-
titles may have been added as an afterthought. anist cannot influence the time course or dynam-
They are not intended for children but rather ics of the last tone once the key has been struck.
Oiversitll and Commonalitll in lvluS1C Performance: An Anaillsis of Timing Microstructure in ScJlllmann '5 "Tniumerel" 231

;::C
---- ~I h n-n ~~ c:
r--..l-r r ,
) ~1 ~I
tJ ~
:'-1
I [t
Fr' I Ll
1
Jl' ~I
-=1
.. l.--' ...... , 1 I [
~
(espr.)

~ ",.
('jJ rn rTil I
, '"
"it., I

., ,-
) m~
~ <>
~. liLLO/
~.1:
~
I I
I I "'< b.. _.~. I.
i. -
: - #
. ~

----.--: . I......!.-I
~
I
(esp'.)

~d ~~l r
[ill ---fiYL
-
....:: I - JJ IJll I.

)_.
I
.," ~
~ r
(J.' ~h~~~: 'L1....W
p. ",:~-,...
L...J rr
I'$. l;_
I

23
~

e ~= I
.
r. I I d
I

1'= I nl
I I f.'\
io o .

,":=!.
" I
~a
Figure 1. The piano score of "Traumerei," created with MusicProse software following the Clara Schumann (Breitkopf
& Harlel) edition (with some deviations in minor details due to software limitations). The layout of the score on the
page is intended to highlight the structure of the music.
232 Repp

p s p s p s o
Melodic Gestures: MG' ~~ MG3a ~~ MG3b S
O[] MG2 tzJ [23 MG4a ~!6d MG4b A
D MG2i sa MGSa ~~ MGSb T
[l]]] lJI] MG6 f ~ MG6a [ill MG6b B
Period Phrase 1 2 3 4
Type S
A
a,
o T
B

A 5
S
A
b,
o T
B

S
A
T
B

B S
A
T
B
20
S
A
a, T
B
AI
S
A
T
B

Figure 2. Schematic representation of the melodiclrhythmic structure of "Traumerei." (See text for explanation.)
Oiversit1/ and Commonalit1/ in Music Performance: An Analllsis of Timmg Microstructure in Schumann's "Triiumerei" 233

(In Figure 2, each MG box extends one eighth-note divided between the tenor and alto voices (bars 10
space beyond the metrical onset of the last tone.) and 14). In the following timing analyses, we will
Blank spaces represent time spans devoid of MGs; essentially be concerned only with the primary
they may contain single tones, sustained tones, or MGs, which represent the leading voice(s) in the
rests. Multivoiced chords having some gestural polyphonic quartet. As can be seen in Figure
quality are represented by vertical ellipses in 2, in phrases of Type a the primary MGs
Figure 2. Arrows indicate continuity of a MG (including the final MG6f in bar 24) are all in the
across a line break or with a subsequent melodic soprano, except for MG6a which is in the bass and
event. Thus, MG1 and MG6 continue from the end overlaps both MG5a and MG 1. In Type b phrases,
of one line to the beginning of the next, and MG3b during the second half, the primary MGs cascade
and MG5b in the soprano voice are closely linked. down through the four voices, overlapping each
When an arrow points into blank space, it points other.
to the onset of a single-tone event that coheres
with the MG. II. THE PERFORMANCES
MGs are divided into primary (p) and secondary The sample of performances analyzed here
(s) ones. The latter are usually shorter and includes 28 performances by 24 outstanding
accompany primary MGs; they are represented by pianists, selected according to ready availability
boxes filled in lighter shades. An exception to this (see Table 1). Two artists, Cortot and Horowitz,
classification is MG2i, a delayed imitation of MG2 are represented by three different recordings each.

Table 1. The artists and their recordings.

Code Artist Recording

ARG Martha Argerich (1941-) DG 410 653-2 (CD) [1983]


ARR Claudio Arrau (1903-1991) Philips 420-871-2 (CD) [1974]
ASH Vladimir Ashkenazy (1937-) London 421 290-2 (CD) [1987]
BRE Alfred Brendel (1931-) Philips 9500 964 [1980]
BUN Stanislav Bunina DG 427315-2 (CD) [1988]
CAP Sylvia Capova a Stradivari SMC-6020 (C) [T] [1987]
Cal Alfred Cortot (1877-1962) EMI3C 153-53793M [1935]
C02 Alfred Cortot EMI3C 153-53794M [1947]
C03 Alfred Cortot EMI3C 153-53795M [1953]
CUR Clifford Curzon (1907-1982) London LL-I009 [-1955]
DAV Fanny Davies (1861-1934) Pearl GEMM CD 9291 (CD) [1929]b
OEM Jorg Demus (1928-) MHS OR 400 [-1960s]
ESC Christoph Eschenbach (1940-) DG 2535 224 [1966]'
GIA Reine Gianoli (1915-1979) Ades 13.243-2 (CD) [1974]
Hal Vladimir Horowitz (1904-1989) RCA LD 7021 [T] [1947]
H02 Vladimir Horowitz Columbia MS 6411 [1963]
H03 Vladimir Horowitz Columbia MS 6765 [1965] (live) [T]
KAT Cyprien Katsaris (1951-) Telefunken 6.42479 AP [1980](live)[T]
KLI Walter Klien (1928-1991) Allegretto ACS 8023 (C) [T]C
KRU Andre Krust a MHS 1009 (orig. Erato) [-1960s]
KUB Antonin Kubalek (1935-) Dorian DOR-90116 (CD) [1988]
Mal Benno Moiseiwitsch (1890-1963) Decca DL 710,048 [-1950s]
NEY Elly Ney (1882-1968) Electrola WDLP 561 [-1935]
NOV Guiomar Novaes (1895-1979) Vox PL 11.l60 (orig. PL8540) [1954]
aRT Cristina Ortiz (1950-) MCA Classics MCAC-25234 (C) [1988]
SCH Artur Schnabel (1882-1951) PatM COLH 85 [1947]
SHE Howard Shelleya CHAN 8814 (CD) [1990]
ZAK Yakov Zak (1913-1976) Monitor MC 2039 [-1960]

abirthdate not known


boriginally Pearl CLA 1000
Cdate unknown
Abbreviations: CD =compact disc; C =cassette; < =date of liner notes (recording date probably the same or preceding year); - =
estimated date; T = "Traumerei" only
234 Repp

Most of the recordings (17) are on long-playing the artists, were noted (especially in performances
records, 3 are on cassettes, and 8 are on CDs by COl-3, DAV, MOl, and SHE) and measured,
(including a transfer of a 1929 recording by Fanny but the label from which the 101 was computed
Davies, a one-time student of Clara Schumann, was placed at the onset of the major melody tone
from a very scratchy original). Most of the (usually the one with the highest pitch and the
performances are of the complete "Kinderszenen," latest onset). The same convention was followed in
but five are of "Traumerei" only. Two of the latter the case of the written-out arpeggi in bars 2, 6,
are from live concerts; all others are studio and 18.
recordings. Table 1 includes actual or estimated B. Data representation
recording dates.
The 24 artists include some of the most The data were submitted to further analysis in
renowned pianists of this century as well as some the form of eighth-note lOIs. That is, intervals
less well-known artists. They can be grouped involving grace note onsets (in bars 2, 6, 8, 16, and
according to gender (6 female, 18 male), country of 18) were omitted from the data and were
origin (5 from Russia; 4 each from Austria and considered separately.l lOIs longer than a
France; 3 from England; 2 each from Germany nominal eighth-note were divided into eighth-note
and Brazil; one each from Czechoslovakia, intervals of equal duration. Thus, for example, the
Argentina, and Chile; one unknown), and half-note 101 in bar 2 was represented as four
approximate age at the time of recording (about equal eighth-note lOIs. A complete performance
equal numbers of young, middle-aged, and old). At thus yielded 254 eighth-note lOIs.
least 12 pianists are no longer alive. e. Measurement error
III. ANALYSIS METHODS The measurement procedure would have been
prohibitively time-consuming if maximum accu-
A. Measurement procedure racy had been aimed for (e.g., by displaying
All tone interonset intervals (lOIs) were hand- shorter waveform segments on the computer ter-
measured by the author using a waveform editing minal screen). A certain amount of speed-accuracy
program. Each performance was low-pass filtered tradeoff had to be taken into account. The magni-
at 4.9 kHz and digitized at a 10 kHz sampling tude of the resulting measurement error can be
rate. The digitized waveform was displayed on the estimated from three performances (BRE, C02,
screen of a computer terminal, 2 s at a time. The and the first half of CUR) that, for various rea-
screen resolution for that display was about 2 ms. sons, had to be remeasured. Two of them (BRE,
A cursor was placed at each tone onset, and a CUR) derived from good LPs with a moderate
permanent "label" was attached to that point in amount of surface noise, whereas C02 was of
the waveform file. The differences between the older vintage and had special problems related to
time points of successive labels yielded the lOIs, the pianist's tendency to play the left- and right-
which were noted down to the nearest millisecond. hand parts asynchronously. There were several
After some practice, it took about 2 hours to very large discrepancies (in excess of 80 ms)
measure one complete performance. between the two measurements of the C02
Two problems had to be coped with. One was performance, due to a conscious change in the
that the onset of a soft tone was often difficult to author's criteria for treating asynchronies; these
detect by eye, particularly in some of the older discrepancies were not considered true
recordings, which had much surface noise. (DAV measurement errors and were omitted from the
was the worst by far.) An auditory method was data presented below, though some smaller
used in that case: The cursor was moved back in inconsistencies of the same kind may still be
small steps, and the waveform segment up to the included.
cursor was played back until the onset of the tone The measurement error distributions are shown
in question could no longer be heard. This proce- in Table 2. The combined BRE+CUR data
dure was time-consuming but usually resulted in represent average accuracy, whereas the C02
rather accurate location of tone onsets (cf. data constitute a worst-case scenario; only DAV
Gabrielsson, 1987). The second problem concerned was even more difficult to measure. It can be seen
onset asynchronies among simultaneous tones in that over 90% of the measurement errors in the
different voices. Unintended asynchronies, which BRE+CUR data did not exceed 10 ms, which is
usually were too small to be detected by eye in the less than 2% of the average 101. The largest error
waveform, had to be ignored. Larger asynchronies, was under 35 ms, or about 6% ofthe.average 101,
which in most cases must have been intended by and the average measurement ,error was 4.3 mS,
DIversItv and Commonalltv In MusIc Performance: An Anail/SIS ofTImll1R MIcrostructure l1J Schumann's "TriiumereI" 235

less than 1%. In the C02 data set, about 90% of made, nor with pianists' gender, age at the time of
the errors ranged from 0 to 20 ms, and the largest recording, or country of origin.
error was under 50 ms. The average error was
10.4 ms, or about 2%. Measurements of the Table 3. Overall tempi (qpm) estimated from the first
virtually noiseless CD recordings presumably quartile of the fOf distribution.
were even more accurate than suggested by the
BRE+CUR data.
48 ESC
49
Table 2. Distributions of absolute measurement error
50 KAT, NEY
in two sets of remeasured data. 51
52 ZAK
BRE+CUR (N=412) C02 (N=245) 53 CAP
54 KLI
Range (ms) Percent Cum. Percent Cum. 55
56 CUR
0-5 77.4 77.4 24.5 24.5 57 BUN, NOY
6 - 10 13.8 91.3 40.0 64.5 58 DEM, KUB
11 - 15 2.7 93.9 18.8 83.3 59 ARR, KRU, MOl, SCH
16 - 20 1.7 95.6 5.7 89.0 60
21 - 25 2.2 97.8 3.7 92.7 61 H02
26 - 30 1.7 99.5 3.3 95.9 62 ARG
31 - 35 0.5 100.0 2.0 98.0 63 ASH
36 -40 1.2 99.2 64 H03
41 - 45 0.4 99.6 65 HOI, SHE
46 - 50 0.4 100.0 66 C03
67 BRE,ORT
68 GIA
69
IV. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 70
The strategy in presenting the results will be to 71
proceed from more global properties to more 72 COl
detailed aspects. 73
74
A. Overall tempo 75 C02
Perhaps the most obvious dimension along 76
which different performances vary is that of 77
overall tempo. The present set of performances is 78
no exception. Tentative estimates of global tempo 79 DAY
were obtained by computing the first quartiles
(the 25% point) of the individual eighth-note 101
distributions, multiplying these millisecond values B. Repeats
by 2, and dividing them into 60,000. The choice of The first step in the data analysis was an
the first quartile was motivated by the attempt to eliminate redundancy and reduce
consideration that expressive lengthening of lOIs random measurement error by averaging across
is both more frequent and more pronounced than repetitions of the same (or highly similar) musical
shortening, and also by the fact that it gave tempi material. Of course, such averaging is meaningful
for two pianists, BRE and DAV, that agreed only if there are no systematic differences in
closely, respectively, with Brendel's (1981) timing microstructure across repeats. The prime
statement of his preferred tempo and with Clara target for averaging was the first period, bars 1-8,
Schumann's (DAV's teacher's) recommended which was repeated literally, according to
tempo. (Repp, 1993b, will provide a more extended Schumann's instructions in the score. All but two
discussion.) Table 3 shows that the tempo range pianists (DAV, KRU) observed this repeat. 2
extended from 48 to 79 quarter-notes per minute. To compare the first and second repeats for all
Apart from the fact that the three fastest pianists, a grand average timing pattern was first
performances are all old recordings, there does not obtained by computing the geometric mean of
seem to be any systematic relationship between corresponding lOIs across all 28 performances.
tempo and the time at which the recording was (For DAV and KRU, the data of bars 1-8 were
236 Repp

simply duplicated for the missing second repeat.) which such a comparison was made (see, e.g.,
The geometric mean was preferred over the Palmer, 1989; Repp, 1990; Seashore, 1938). Thus,
arithmetic mean because it compensated for any it seemed justified to average across the two re-
tendency of slower performances to show more peats of bars 1-8 in all further analyses, except as
expressive variability, which would have noted.
dominated in an arithmetic average. In this grand There were other instances of identical or highly
average, the two repeats of bars 1-8 were found to similar musical material in the piece. Their timing
have extremely similar timing profiles, with only a patterns may be compared in Figure 3, which
slight tendency for the second repeat to be played plots the grand average eighth-note lOIs. A
slower. The correlation between these two logarithmic ordinate scale is used here to
averages was 0.987, which indicates that any accommodate the longest values; it also makes the
variations across repeats for individual pianists scale comparable to the percentage scale used by
were either random or idiosyncratic. The authors such as Gabrielsson (1987). The abscissa
correlations between the two repeats in individual represents metrical distance (or "score time") in
performances are shown in Table 4 (column 2); eighth-note steps.3 Longer notes appear as
most of them were quite high. "plateaus" of multiple eighth-notes; that way, all
The close similarity of timing patterns across re- lOIs are represented on the same proportional
peats has been noted in virtually every study in scale.

Table 4. Correlations of the timing profiles for repeated or similar sections (R = repeat; GM = geometric mean).

Bars

Artist 1-8/1R-8R 1-4117-20 9-12/13-16

ARG 0.781 0.861 0.630


ARR 0.933 0.938 0.850
ASH 0.922 0.951 0.927
BRE 0.953 0.888 0.880
BUN 0.510 0.633 0.632
CAP 0.935 0.922 0.846
COl 0.859 0.717 0.837
CO2 0.865 0.736 0.851
cm 0.750 0.741 0.571
CUR 0.937 0.911 0.921
DAY * 0.863 0.863
DEM 0.938 0.924 0.828
ESC 0.890 0.734 0.802
GIA 0.777 0.809 0.914
HOI 0.918 0.958 0.697
H02 0.811 0.861 0.822
H03 0.826 0.738 0.770
KAT 0.825 0.901 0.867
KLI 0.804 0.893 0.861
KRU * 0.875 0.940
KUB 0.951 0.913 0.888
MOl 0.805 0.855 0.493
NEY 0.920** 0.904 0.872
NOY 0.931 0.906 0.908
ORT 0.677 0.609 0.707
SCH 0.676 0.574 0.748
SHE 0.868 0.831 0.859
ZAK 0.839 0.875 0.909
GM 0.987 0.986 0.950
*no repeat
**bars 1-4 only (see Footnote 2)
DlVersitu and Commonalitu In Music Performance: An Analysis of Timing Microstructure in Schumann 5 "Triiumerei" 23;""

2500

.........
2000 -- a, (bars 04)

-e- a, (bars 17-20)


-- b 1 (bars 58)

-e- bz (bars 9-12)

-- --
Ul
E a2 (bars 21-24) b3 (bars 1316)
-.J
<t:
>
a:
w
f-
Z 1000
f-
W MG5b
MG3a
I I
~ 800 r-l
MG4b
o ITl
a: MG3b
w
z 600
f-

~-M~~~M~~~M~~~M~~
6~~~~NNN~~M~~~~~~

BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE

Figure 3. Grand average lOIs (geometric means across all 28 performances). The two panels show the timing profiles
for phrases of Type a and b, respectively. Primary melodic gestures (d. Figure 2) are indicated by brackets. The bar
numbers on the abscissa refer to bars 0-8 only; for the later phrases, an appropriate constant must be added.

The left-hand panel of Figure 3 compares the Figure 2). The timing profiles of phrases b2 (bars
timing profiles for the three Type a phrases. Bars 9-12) and b3 (bars'13-16) are especially close. Only
17-20 (phrase al) are musically identicalwith bars the (prescribed) ritardando in bar 16 is much
1-4 (see Figure 1), and it can be seen that their more pronounced than that in bar 12 (which is not
timing patterns are highly similar (r = 0.986), notated). If the final halves of bars 12 and 16 (the
though bars 17-20 tend to be played at a ritardandi) are excluded, correlations between
somewhat slower tempo. The individual these two timing patterns are again extremely
correlations for these four bars (Table 4, column 3) high (see Table 4, last column). Phrase bl (bars 5-
are comparable to the correlations between the 8) also shows a rather similar profile; differences
two repeats of bars 1-8. The timing pattern for occur precisely where its musical content deviates
bars 21-24 (phrase a2) is initially similar but from phrases b2 and b3 (especially in bar 6).
diverges soon, due to the fermata (long hold) in The correlations in Table 4 indicate which
bar 22 and the progressive ritardando (slowing of artists were highly consistent in their timing
tempo) towards the end of the piece. patterns, and which of them were less consistent.
The right-hand panel of Figure 3 compares the The consistent group is led by ASH, CUR, and
timing patterns of the three Type b phrases. Once NOV and includes many others; the less
again a striking similarity can be seen, due to the consistent group includes ARG, BUN, C03, MOl,
similarity or identity of the melodic structure (see ORT, and SCH.4
238 Repp

C. Melodidrhythmic structure and the 101, which seemed fairly stable across all phrases.
average timing pattern The quarter-note upbeats at the ends of bars 4
and 20, which coincided with MG6a, were
Let us examine now the average timing pattern relatively longer. The eight-note upbeats in bars 8
and its relation to the musical structure in greater and 12, which coincided with MG6b, were even
detail. Although the grand average timing profile longer. Finally, although the grace-note upbeat of
is not necessarily an optimal performance timing bar 16 is not shown explicitly in Figure 3, the 101
pattern, it captures. features that many individual accommodating it was lengthened enormously,
performances have in common, whereas it sup- due to the major ritardando at the end of period
presses random and idiosyncratic timing devia- B. (More information about the grace-note upbeat
tions that differ from performance to performance. is provided later on.)
In Figure 3 it can be seen that there are global The chord following MG1 enriches the harmonic
temporal trends within phrases, particularly those and rhythmic texture but is not really part of the
of Type b. Their timing profiles follow a concave melody; however, it may be thought of as a kind of
function on which various local peaks are echo of the tonic and thus could be linked with
superimposed. Bars 21-24 (phrase a2), too, seem MG 1. The chord bisects the inter-gesture interval
to follow such a curve, though it is grossly between MGl and MG2. Of the resulting two lOIs,
distorted by the fermata and the final grand the first (nominally shorter) one was consistently
ritardando. Bars 1-4 and 17-20 (phrase al) have a longer relative to the second (nominally longer)
flatter global timing shape. The curvilinear trends one, as if pianists tried to equalize the two. This
in Type b phrases are reminiscent of the parabolic may reflect final (and/or accent-related)
curves hypothesized by Todd (1985) as the basic lengthening of the tonic in MG1, which is
phrasal timing pattern. Todd's model 5 , however, absorbed by the 101 preceding the chord.
operates on the durations of time units equivalent MG2 is undoubtedly the most salient melodic
to whole bars, whereas the present timing profiles gesture of the piece. It comprises five eighth-notes
are across eighth-note units and thus provide that ascend in increasingly larger pitch steps to a
much finer resolution. The curves that "cradle" the final note which constitutes the apex of the four-
present timing patterns (i.e., the "bottom lines" of bar melodic arch that constitutes each phrase.
the Type b timing profiles) are, in fact, poorly fit Variants ofthe gesture occur in bars 1-2, 5-6, 9-10,
by quadratic functions. However, they do 13-14, 17-18, and 21-22. MG2 is unusual because
represent the general principle formalized by it culminates on the second beat of a bar, negating
Todd and observed by many others, that there is a entirely the customary accent on the first beat. As
slowing of the tempo at major structural can be seen in Figure 3, the average temporal
boundaries, in proportion to the importance of the pattern of the five lOIs is quite similar in all
boundaries. Thus the most extreme ritardando occurrences of the gesture: The first 101 is close to
(prescribed in the score) occurs at the end of the the proportional duration of the preceding 101, but
piece; a pronounced slowing down (also the next two lOIs are much shorter. The fourth
prescribed) occurs at the end of the second period 101 is longer again, similar to the first, and the
(bar 16); next come the end of the first period (bar fifth 101 is the longest; it is clearly visible as a
8) and the end of phrase bl (bar 12), which show . sharp peak in the timing profile. The duration of
about the same amount of ritardando (not explicit that fifth 101 varies with position in the piece: It is
in the score); the end of phrase al (bars 4 and 20) least extended in bars 10 and 14, longer in bars 2,
shows the smallest, but still quite noticeable 6, and 18, where it accommodates the two grace
slowing down. notes of the left-hand chord (cf. Figure 1), and
The piece begins with a brief melodic gesture longest in bar 22 where it leads to the climactic
(MG1) that ascends from the dominant to the fermata. In fact, this eighth-note 101 in bar 22
tonic, with accent on the latter (hence the tended to be lengthened relative to the fermata
intervening bar line, which indicates accent on the chord itself (the plateau following the peak). The
following beat). MGl recurs in bars 4-5, 8-9,12-13, specific accelerando-ritardando shape of MG2 will
16-17, and 20-21, though the upbeat is shortened be examined more closely later on.
in bars 8, 12, and 16 (cf. Figure 1). It thus The 101 following MG2 in bars 2, 6, 18, and 22
contains a single 101 of variable nominal length. (plateau in Figure 3) represents another inter-ges-
Its realization was strongly context-dependent: ture interval, nominally three or four eighth-notes
The quarter-note upbeat at the beginning of the long. Relative to the' last 101 of MG2, this interval
piece (bar 0) was short relative to the following was much shorter proprortionally, even in bar 22,
Diversitll and Commonalitll in Music Performance: An Ana/lIsls of Timln\; MIcrostructure In Schumann's "Triiumerei" 239

where it received the fermata. The fermata had its overlapping each other. Simultaneously, other
maximal effect on the inter-gesture interval, dou- voices articulate shorter two-note cadential
bling its duration relative to bars 2 and 18, but it gestures which, as can be seen in Figure 3,
also affected the whole preceding MG2. In bars 10 essentially govern the timing pattern. In each
and 14, the inter-gesture IOI is bridged by the in- MG, both lOIs constituting these accompanying
ner-voice imitation gesture MG2i, which has a motifs were lengthened, the first, unaccented one
timing pattern that is almost the mirror image of usually more than the second, accented one. This
MG2 in that it accelerates during the first three is explained by the harmonic function of the tone
IOIs and slows down only at the end. The first IOI cluster initiating the unaccented 101, which is
is long because it also represents the last 101 of that of suspense followed by resolution. In part,
MG2; the relative length of the second IOI may be however, it may also be due to final lengthening of
transitional, or it may reflect final lengthening of the preceding, overlapping MG.
MG2 which was absorbed by MG2i. 6 In summary, these observations illustrate
The remaining MGs, which descend in a chain several general principles of performance timing:
from the melodic apex reached by MG2, are (1) Whole phrases tend to show global tempo
patterned differently in phrase Types a and b. The curves characterized by an initial accelerando and
grouping structure of the Type a chain is indicated a more pronounced final ritardando whose degree
with slurs in the score (Figure 1). MG3a consists reflects the degree of finality of the phrase in the
of four (a1) or five (a2) eighth-notes in the soprano hierarchical grouping structure. (2) "Riding" on
voice, with accent on the penultimate; a shorter the global pattern, individual MGs often show a
accompanying gesture occurs in the tenor voice similarly curved local pattern, though accent
(al only). It can be seen in Figure 3 that placement, harmonic factors, and the influence of
lengthening on the accented note occurred in overlapping MGs in other voices may modulate
performance. MG4a is similar in rhythmic that pattern in various ways. Gesture-final
structure to MG3a and exhibits a similar timing lengthening is commonly observed.
pattern, except that the final unstressed eighth-
note 101 was lengthened as well, especially in D. Principal components analysis
phrase a2, where the final ritardando tilted the An alternative way of capturing commonalities
timing profile. MG5a, reinforced by shorter among different performances is offered by the
secondary MGs in the bass voice, is similar to statistical technique of principal components
MG4a, except that in phrase al it leads into a (factor) analysis. This method decomposes the
final half-note that coincides with the onset of data matrix (N performances by M lOIs) into a
MG6a. Its timing pattern is a progressive number of independent components or factors,
lengthening, similar to that observed in MG4a. In each of which resembles a timing profile (i.e., M
phrase a2, the final ritardando is in full control. standardized "factor scores"). The original data
The final gesture in phrase aI, MG6a, is in the are approximated by a weighted sum of these fac-
bass voice and leads back to the low-pitched tonic tors; the weights, which differ for each perfor-
which doubles the tonic of the next MG 1. Its first mance, are called "factor loadings" and represent
IOI coincides with the final lengthened IOI of the correlations between the performance timing
MG5a, and the last two lOIs coincide with the profiles and the factor score profiles. The degree of
quarter-note upbeat of MG1 and are both approximation (the percentage of the "variance ac-
lengthened as well. The lOIs in between are counted for" or VAF) depends on the number of
considerably shorter. The total timing profile of factors that are considered significant; a common
MG6a is not unlike that of MG2, which it criterion employed here is that they should have
resembles rhythmically. The final gesture in "eigenvalues" greater than 1. (Eigenvalue = N
phrase a2, MG6f, is a truncated version of the times an individual factor's proportion of VAF.)
preceding MGs and shows an enormous slowdown The first factor always accounts for the largest
in tempo; note, however, the discontinuity in the amount of variance and represents a kind of cen-
ritardando between MG5a and MG6f, which is a tral tendency or most common pattern. Additional
manifestation of final lengthening in MG5a. factors account for increasingly less variance and
The melodic chain of type b is also divided into thus represent patterns shared by fewer perfor-
four MGs, but they are organized differently. The mances. A standard technique for simplifying the
slurs in the score are not entirely consistent here; factor loadings and thereby increasing the inter-
what distinguishes the gestures is the fact that pretability of the factors is called "Varimax rota-
they occur in different voices, imitating and tion." It increases large factor loadings and re-
240 Repp

duces small ones without changing the number of the overall timing pattern and cause high
factors or the total VAF; however, the VAF is re- correlations among the performances, with the
distributed among the rotated factors. result that the interesting variation among the
Principal components analysis thus can reveal shorter lOIs is lost. In fact, a principal
whether there is more than one shared timing components analysis conducted on the complete
pattern represented in the sample of 28 data yielded only a single factor, indicating that
performances. If there is essentially only one way all pianists observed the major ritardandi, which
of performing the piece, then a single factor is not very surprising. Therefore, it was decided to
should explain most of the variance in the data. If analyze only the data for bars 0-8 (phrases al and
there are several radically different timing bI), which did not include any extreme
patterns, then several orthogonal factors should lengthenings (cf. Figure 3). Because of the
emerge. Idiosyncratic timing patterns will not parallelism of the timing profiles of these phrases
lead to a significant factor and will constitute part to those of similar type (see Figure 3), such a
of the variance not accounted for. Since all timing restricted analysis essentially captures the whole
profiles are standardized and intercorrelated in performances minus the major ritardandi.
the course of the analysis, differences in overall This analysis yielded four significant factors.
tempo are automatically disregarded. Together they accounted for 76% of the variance.
Although it would seem desirable to conduct the For individual performances, the VAF ranged
analysis on the complete performances, the large from 60% to 90%. The timing profiles representing
ritardandi observed universally at the ends of the first three factors are shown in Figure 4, and
phrases a2, bI, b2, and b3, as well as the large the factor loadings of the 28 performances are
slowdown at the fermata in phrase a2, dominate listed in Table 5.

1OOO-rr---r---,--r--:-::!-:--~-.....---r---,

<il lOOO -rr---,c---;---,---,---.,.--r---,-..,--,


.s
CJ) OIJ
w
a:
o
D
CJ)
a:
ot-
D

..
~ 400 -hji-,TTM-rrrrrn-TlTl+n-rrrr-.+nlTTTrmTTrrrr+TTrITTT+-rnT1'Trh-rTTTn-I
1000rr--,---,---,----,----,---,r---,...-----,

400, , .... .
~-M~~-M~~_M~~_M~~-M~~_M~~_M~~_M~_
O~~~~NNNNMMMM.~.~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~=~

BARS/EIGHTH-NOTES
Figure 4. Timing patterns of the first three principal components for bars 0-8. The standardized factor scores were
converted into milliseconds by multiplying them with the average within-performance 101 standard deviation and
adding this product to the grand mean 101. Bar lines and MG brackets are added for orientation.
Diversitu and Commonality in Music Performance: An Analuszs of Timzng Mzcrostnlcture In Schumann', "Triiumerez" 241

Table 5. Sorted rotated factor loadings of the 28 ARG, NEY, ESC and a number of other
performances, bars 0-8. (Loadings smaller than 0.4 are performances shared features of this pattern. Due
omitted.) to the statistical orthogonality of the factors, this
timing pattern is radically different from that of
II III IV Factor I. It is characterized by a very short initial
upbeat (essentially reduced to an eighth-note,
SCH 0.844 though it was rarely that extreme in the actual
ASH 0.760 0.423 performances; see below); relatively short long
BRE 0.738 lOIs (i.e., low plateaus); no accelerando but a
DAV 0.695 pronounced ritardando during MG2; pronounced
CAP 0.695 0.438 0.41 I lengthening in MG3a, MG3b, and MG5b, with
SHE 0.692 extreme prolongation of the IOI in position 8-4,
KAT 0.685 which accommodates a grace note.
KUB 0.672 Factor III (VAF = 15%) is the "Cortot factor"; the
ARR 0.664 0.463
three performances by this artist were the only
ZAK 0.644 0.460
ones that showed substantial loadings. Its timing
aRT 0.637
CUR 0.615 0.429 pattern is most unusual: a relatively short upbeat
KRU 0.593 0.495 0.445 in MGl; a short final lOI in MG2 (positions 2-2
DEM 0.551 0.495 and 6-2); marked speeding up during MG3 with a
HOI 0.888 following ritardando extending through MG4 to
H03 0.886 the endofMG5; and rushing during the end of
H02 0.837 MG5b, followed by a pronounced ritardando
ARG 0.770 during MG6b.
NEY 0.665 0.524 Factor N (VAF =6%) seems less important and
ESC 0.482 0.626 will not be discussed in detail, as no artist showed
GIA 0.595 0.491
a high loading on it (Table 5).
BUN 0.588
The three factor timing patterns shown in
KLI 0.523 0.570
NOV 0.509 0.549 0.464 Figure 4 do not correspond exactly to any real per-
C03 0.876 formance, though they are similar to certain per-
Cal 0.875 formances, as indicated by the factor loadings
CO2 0.850 (Table 5). Most artists' performances are best de-
Mal 0.501 0.582 scribed by a weighted combination of several fac-
tors; thus, for example, the CAP timing profile is a
combination of Factors I, II, and III. Such a com-
Factor I (VAF = 30%) represents a timing bination will of course result in an attenuation of
pattern shared (partially) by a large number of extreme features. About one fourth of the variance
performances; half the pianists had their largest was not explained by the four factors extracted,
loading on this factor, with SCH, ASH, and BRE and most of that variance was probahly nonran-
. leading the group. This pattern has the following dom, representing the artists' individuality.
features: a relatively long initial upbeat (MGl) It had to be asked at this point whether the
and pronounced lengthening of the last 101 of Horowitz and Cortot factors emerged only because
MG6, which accompanies the upbeat to the next each of these artists was represented by three
MGl (positions 4-8 and 8-8); general lengthening different performances. 7 The principal compo-
of long lOIs (plateaus in the profile); a relatively nents analysis was repeated with only a single
long accelerando phase in MG2 followed by a performance of each of these pianists included
pronounced lengthening of its last 101; and small (COl and H02). The three factors that emerged
but regular gestural accent peaks for MG3-MG6, (VAF = 71%) were highly similar to the first three
except for special emphasis at the end of MG5b factors of the earlier analysis. The second and
(positions 8-3 to 8-5). This pattern reflects the third factors still had their highest loadings in
melodic/rhythmic structure even more clearly H02 and COl, respectively. The patterns offactor
than the average timing profile displayed in loadings for the three factors in the two analyses
Figure 3. correlated 0.96, 0.98, and 0.95, respectively. Thus,
Factor II (VAF = 25%) may be called the the Horowitz and Cortot factors are not artifacts
"Horowitz factor," since the three performances by due to the over-representation of these artists in
that artist showed the highest loadings, though the sample. They represent two true alternatives
242 Repp

to the "standard" pattern of performance timing the three instances in which the MG1 upbeat is
instantiated by Factor I-alternatives that only nominally a quarter-note. The leftmost panel
Horowitz and Cortot dared to choose in nearly shows the unconstrained situation at the
pure form, but that several other pianists incorpo- beginning of the piece (bar 0). It can be seen that
rated partially into their timing strategies. very few pianists played this initial upbeat-or
the following intergesture interval, as the case
E. Detailed analyses may be-"as written" (ratio of 1). The large
In this section, we examine how melodic majority played the upbeat quarter-note short
gestures and other details of the score were relative to the intergesture interval. Several
executed by using, as it were, a magnifying glass pianists (NEY, COl, BUN) come very close to
on the data. While the emphasis has so far been playing it as if it were an eighth-note (ratio of 0.5),
primarily on commonalities, the analyses are now and one (ARG) does so without question. This may
directed increasingly towards uncovering artistic have been a deliberate anticipation of the
diversity. They reveal the different ways in which (notational) shortening of the upbeat in later
a gesture may be shaped by pianists of great incarnations of MGl, but it is a true deviation
authority. They also reveal some unusual, and from the written score and is perceived as such.
occasionally questionable, interpretations of Later, however, when the quarter-note upbeat
notational details in the score. In addition to occurs together with two eighth-notes in the bass
demonstrating the range of individual differences, voice (top center and right-hand panels in Figure
these analyses illustrate what temporal patterns 5), pianists are more evenly divided between those
are preferred by a majority of the artists, and who shorten the upbeat and those who lengthen it
what patterns are never used, and hence relative to the intergesture interval. While the
presumably unacceptable. average ratio is close to 1, there is enormous
1. MG 1. This gesture consists of merely two variation among individual artists, and some who
tones forming the pitch interval of an ascending severely shortened the initial, unconstrained
fourth. It is followed, however, by a chord whose upbeat (e.g., ARG, BUN) do not repeat this
timing, though not strictly part of the melody, is tendency later on.
also of interest. MG1 appears six times: Three The lower row of panels in Figure 5 shows
times with a nominal quarter-note upbeat (bars 0, instances where the upbeat is nominally an
4, and 20) and three times with a nominal eighth- eighth-note. In two cases (left-hand and center
note upbeat (bars 8, 12, 16), one of which (bar 16) panels) the upbeat is accompanied by a bass-voice
is written as a grace note and thus is open to the eighth-note that marks the approaching end of
assignment of an even shorter value. Two of the MG6 and of a whole phrase, so here the
instances of the gesture are relatively uncon- overwhelming tendency is to lengthen the upbeat,
strained because the upbeat is unaccompanied sometimes to an extent corresponding to a
(bars 0 and 16); in the other cases the upbeat coin- nominal quarter-note (ratio of 2; BUN and ARR in
cides with eighth-notes in the bass voice (MG6). bar 12). In bar 16, where the upbeat is notated as
Figure 5 presents the individual data in terms of a grace eighth-note, a significant minority of
two 101 ratios, N(BC) and B/C, where A is the pianists plays the upbeat shorter than an eighth-
duration of the intragesture 101 (i.e., the upbeat), note, in one case (KAT) shorter than a sixteenth-
B is the intergesture 101 preceding the chord note (ratio of 0.5). The majority, however, still
(nominally two eighth-notes) and C is the lengthen it, and at least one (ASH) plays the grace
intergesture interval following the chord note as if it were a quarter-note (ratio of 2).
(nominally three eighth-notes). If the score were There is little indication of clustering or
played literally, N(BC) should be either 0.2 or 0.4, bimodality in these data. Although there are some
depending on whether the upbeat is notated as an "outliers," the values of the ratio seem to be rather
eighth-note or a quarter-note, and B/C should be evenly distributed over a wide range. Correlations
0.67. In Figure 5, however, the ratios have been of individual timing patterns across the different
normalized with respect to the underlying eighth- instances of MG1 are not high, though some
note pulse (assumed to be isochronous for this consistencies can be seen. Thus ARR and BUN
purpose), so that the expected "literal" ratios always tend to lengthen the upbeat, whereas
(effectively, tempo ratios) are equal to 1 in all ARG, DEM, and COI-3 tend to shorten it. Some
cases. pianists, perhaps to be regarded as "literalists,"
Let us consider first the ratio plotted on the consistently avoid extremes (e.g., ESC, KRD,
abscissa. The top row of panels in Figure 5 shows MOI,ZAK).
Bars 0/1 Bars 4/5 Bars 20/21
1.6 I
I
I i 1.6 1.6 D
I

I
Oem
.Bun
Nov
~
1.4~
Nov.
1 .Arr I 1.4.- 1.4 - Arg. Ho1
~

-.,..'
I
'"

.
;:,:
Argo Oem. Arg. M.
One
' ...
I Ash Arr. n
# Arre
N~
Q
3
1.2-t 1.2 1.2 _ Oeme ~

--
I
fl 3
I C01.

Bun
She
,lre
Sene
~ Cur
Ho3
Co2. I Bu
!
~

1~
... Co2

Col Gia'"
~
;:,:
art Cap ~

1
Co1. Co3 Oav. 1Ii
I
Co2 Bre r;
0.8~ I I 0.8 , I I 0.8 Co3
I
'\:J
~
-.

--
c'
0 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.2 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 0.6 ".8 1.2 1.4
~;:,:
CO Bars 8/9 Bars 12/13 Bars 16/17 R
;;,.
1.6 1.8 1.8 I , ;:,:

, Arr I
;;,.
;:,:
Nov I Bun. , ,
1.4 Gia. 1.6
Gia I 1.6 - ''-""
~
r;;
Oem
Arr


Nov I
I
I

I

Bun'
,
,
...,.,
Q

:j

, 1.4,- , art
1.2 Arg . , . Ash 1.4 3

I

, . . ..~u I

,,,
~a~. ,. Hal I
Ash
;:;


I
$:
- 1.2
I
1.2,- r;
.c ur.

Bre
, ,
I

~::!
....
1
ap
, ,

r;
0.8 1
I

I
, Kub I ;;;
~
'Ho3. ... C01 Co2, ;:,:

0.6 . . 0.8 , 0.8 . (F>

~
0.6 0.8 1.2 1.4 1.6 1.8 0.8 1.2 1.6 2 2.4 0.4 0.8 1.2 1.6 2 2.4 :::
S
;:,:
;:,:
'I":
A/(B+C) -j
...
~;
:::
Figure 5. Timing patterns of the six instances of MG1 and the following chord. (See text for explanation of ratios.) Ratios of 1 represent equal underlying ;;;
beats (Le., equal tempo). The upper panels plot the three instances where the upbeat.is nominally a quarter-note, while the lower panels plot the three ~.
instances where the upbeat is nominally an eighth-note (a grace note in bar 16). The dashed lines indicate a doubling or halving of the nominal duration in
f-.j
execution. oj;"
\.n
244 Repp

With regard to the BIC ratio, plotted on the have been fitted with a quadratic function (i.e., a
ordinate, there is more consistency. In all six parabola), which seems to describe their temporal
instances, which are notationally equivalent, shape rather wel1. 9 Each gesture accelerates
there is a strong tendency to lengthen the rest initially and then slows down at the end. This
preceding the chord, sometimes to the extent that final ritardando is least pronounced in bars 9-10
the onset of the chord occurs in the middle of the and 13-14; it is more evident in bars 1-2, 5-6, and
intergesture interval (ratio of 1.5). Among those 17-18; and in bars 21-22, preceding the fermata, it
who tend to play the chord very late are ARG, is dramatic. Except in this last instance, there is a
ARR, BUN, and NOV. Nevertheless, there are tendency for the penultimate IOI to rise slightly
some pianists who do not show that pattern, most above the parabolic trajectory. This was evidently
notably Cortot. due to including in the average performances such
Finally, it should be noted that the two ratios as Cortot's which, as we have seen in Figure 4
are uncorrelated across different artists. (Factor III), showed a pronounced tendency to
Evidently, the timing ofMG1 is quite independent shorten the last 101.
of the timing of the chord in the intergesture
interval. s
2. MG2. This is the signature melodic gesture of
the piece, and its manner of execution is crucial to
the impression of a performance of "Traumerei." It
offers a unique opportunity to investigate the 1400
temporal shaping of a gesture, for several reasons: 0 Bars 1/2
(1) It comprises six notes (i.e., five IOIs)-a
sufficient number of degrees of freedom for any 1200 Bars 5/6

A Bars 9/10
constraints on temporal shape to emerge very
Bars 13/14
clearly; (2) it is unidirectional in pitch and
uninterrupted by metric accents-the accent that
---.
(J) 1000 '"
0 Bars 17/18
E
would normally occur on the note following the
bar line is clearly suspended in this case; (3) it ---- 800 Bars 21/22

recurs six times, with slight variations; and (4) 0


pianists are likely to give special attention to its
execution. 600
MG2 occurs in bars 1-2, 5-6, 9-10, 13-14, 17-18,
and 21-22. (We will not consider the imitation
gesture MG2i in detail.) The versions in bars 1-2 400
and 17-18 are identical; during the last 101, two 1-6 1-7 1-8 21 2-2
grace notes (a written-out arpeggio) occur in the BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
left-hand accompaniment of the melody. In bars 5-
6 and 21-22, the penultimate pitch interval is
extended from a fourth to a major sixth; in
addition, the melody in bars 21-22 leads to a
Figure 6. Geometric mean lOIs for the six versions of
fermata and also lacks the grace notes during the MG2, with best-fitting quadratic functions. The abscissa
last 101. In bars 9-10 and 13-14, there are no labels refer to bars 1 and 2.
grace notes, the penultimate melodic interval is
reduced to a minor third in the first instance, The temporal shapes of the six versions of MG2
there is a change of key (to B-flat major) in the were examined and fitted with quadratic functions
second instance, and the occurrence of MG2i in in each of the 28 individual performances (a total
the middle voices creates a forward movement of 168 instances). It emerged that most individual
that is absent in the other variants. Therefore, artists' timing patterns (87% of all instances)
timing differences are to be expected reflecting could be described well by parabolas, with fits
these factors. ranging from good to excellent. What varied
The average temporal shapes of the six versions between artists and between different instances of
of MG2 are shown in Figure 6. The five lOIs for MG2 were the curvature and elevation of the
each version, representing the geometric means parabolic functions, but not so much their
across all 28 performances, are plotted here on a goodness of fit (however, see Footnote 9). The data
linear scale. The five data points for each version of six individual artists are shown in Figure 7.
Diversit1/ alld Commonahtliln l'vll1SIC Performance: An Anal1/sls of Timin" Microstmctllre 111 5chumaml's "Triillmere:" 245

1200 - , - - - - , - - - - - , - - . , . - - - - , - - - . m 1200 ..,......--r---,--,---r-.,-,

1000 1000

800 - 4 \ - \ - - t - - - - t - - . ; - - - I t - - r l ' - i 800 -t---4---1---+-_++--"'''''''''

1-6 1-7 1-8 2-1 2-2 1-6 1-7 1-8 2-1 2-2
1600....---,---,-----,--.,-----,-.....,

1400
1000
--. 1200-t---+---f---+---ff---;
(f)

.s 800 +---+---+---+-:>.<.+-,I4-;----i 1000 -j-l,---+---f---+--r-t---;

o 800-t-~-+---+----l-,4~+---i

400-!--~~f=:::::=-+--+-_I 400-!---+-~-T~-+--f----i
1-6 1-7 1-8 2-1 2-2 1-6 1-7 1-8 2-1 2-2
1400 -.----,--,---,-----r----, 1600..,......--,----r----,,.---.,---.....,

1200 1400

1200 - i - - - + - - - + - - - i f - - - + - , f - - - i
1000 -+---+----;---++-~
____1

1000 -t---t---+-+-I-f--,.<p>o"6-"71
800 -+--'rl-----;---f-+h~_,f_-__1

400 +--+--=+=:::::l!!::::::PL-~-l
1-6 1-7 1-8 2-1 2-2 1-6 1-7 1-8 2-1 2-2

BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
Figure 7. MG2 timing data for six individual artists, with best-fitting quadratic functions. Where the pattern deviated
markedly from a parabolic shape, the data points are connected with straight lines. See Figure 6 for the legend of
symbols.

The two panels on top illustrate two individual Horowitz and Cortot. Horowitz's curves are fit
cases that were fit well by parabolas but differed fairly well by parabolas but generally lack the
in curvature: The highly modulated timing curves initial acceleration shown by most other pianists.
of ARR contrast with the flatter ones of SCH. The Cortot shows a truly deviant pattern: In all three
center panels show one representative perfor- performances (which spanned 18 years the last
mance of each of the two great individualists, 101. This seems to indicate that he grouped the
246 Repp

penultimate tone with the following long tone, exceptions, any individual execution of this phrase
treating it like an upbeat; in fact, the first four can be characterized by a single curvature
IOIs are fit well by a parabola. This idiosyncratic parameter, with some additional freedom in global
timing shape held only for the first five instances tempo (vertical displacement), which is not
of MG2, however; in bars 21-22, preceding the represented in the figure.
fermata, Cortot produced a beautiful parabolic
timing shape in all three performances. Finally, as
can be seen in the bottom panels of Figure 7, ARG
and BUN, two highly variable pianists, intermit-
tently adopted the alternative timing pattern in-
stantiated by Cortot; one other pianist who did so
1400
was CUR. The only other type of deviant timing
pattern was shown by ORT who, in bars 9-10 and 0 Q = 20

13-14 only, lengthened the third IOI above the


parabolic trajectory, which led to a W-like shape.
1200 Q= 40

A Q = 60
The quadratic functions (y = C + Lx + Qx 2 ) that A. Q= 80
fit the large majority of individual phrasal shapes ..--...
(j) 1000 0 Q = 100
are characterized by three parameters: a positive E
constant C that reflects vertical displacement,
related to overall tempo; a generally negative
---
0 800

\I
Q = 120

coefficient L that reflects horizontal-vertical


displacement of the curve in x-y coordinate
space;10 and a positive coefficient Q that reflects 600
the degree of curvature of the concave (U-shaped)
parabola. A question of great theoretical interest
was whether there are any constraints among the 400
1-6 1-7 1-8 2-1 2-2
three parameters; in principle, of course, they are
quite independent of each other. The coefficients L BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
and Q were plotted against each other for all 146
individual gestures that followed a parabolic
shape. There was a remarkably tight linear
relationship between these two parameters, with Figure 8. Family of quadratic functions that describes the
correlations ranging from 0.93 to 0.98 across the timing constraint on MG2 observed by the large majority
six versions of MG2. The reason for this result is of pianists. The functions vary in curvature (Q), roughly
that the location of the minimum of the function is over the observed range. The other coefficients of the
quadratic functions were obtained by the average
relatively fixed. Therefore, as the curvature of the regression equations. Vertical displacement should be
parabola increases, the negative slope of the considered an additional free parameter.
tangent at xO also increases.
If the x coordinate of the minimum is relatively To give some impression of individual
fixed, then the constant C must be correlated with differences in tempo modulation during MG2,
the y intercept of the function and should increase Figure 9 plots the average Q coefficient for bars 1-
as a function of curvature Q. There were indeed 2,5-6, and 17-18 (which were generally executed
positive linear correlations between C and Q, similarly; cf. Figure 6) against that for bars 21-22,
ranging from 0.79 to 0.93. 11 Thus, both Land C with individual artists identified. (Cortot is
are fairly predictable from Q, which leads to the excluded from this plot.) Most pianists employed
conclusion that the expressive timing patterns of moderate modulation in the earlier instances of
the large majority of the performances of MG2 can MG2, but gave a very expressive shape to the last
be characterized by a single family of parabolas, instance in bars 21-22, which led to the fermata.
varying in Q only. This family of functions is Some, most notably DEM, used strong modulation
shown in Figure 8; it was generated by increasing in all instances. Horowitz's three performances
Q from 20 to 140 in steps of 20, and by setting L are at the other extreme. They appear relatively
and C according to their average linear unmodulated despite a substantial ritardando
regressions on Q. Figure 8 embodies a constraint because he did not show any initial accelerando;
on the expressive timing shape of MG2 that was therefore, his timing patterns were fit by
obeyed by the majority of pianists. With few parabolas with low curvature.
DiversitVi1nd Commonalitv in Music Performance: An Analusis of Timll1\i Microstructure in Schumann's Triilin/erei" 24;"

tempo, the slower and the more inflected was MG2


(see also Footnote 8).
3. Grace notes accompanying MG2. Only three of
the six instances of MG2 have grace notes (really
160
I
Dav I a written-out arpeggio) in the left hand during the
Na~
De_
140
last 101; they occur in bars 2, 6, and 18. Since all
Ash Cap six instances of MG2 followed a parabolic
ru Ki~ Zak trajectory, on the average (cf. Figure 6), the grace
120
(\J
Esc
She

Arg Ka~
notes as such were not responsible for the
An
~ 100 lengthening of the final 101 and the systematic
,,- Cur 0 Sre
(\J
Neye
SCh
constraints that it followed. Rather, they seemed
(f) 80
Ie Kli
to be fitted into whatever temporal shape the
Gia
0: pianists chose for the primary melodic gesture. We

0)
60

Un!.

now turn our attention to the execution of these


HaZ.
40
Ha3 grace notes, which exhibited surprising
variability.
20
Hal I In the score, the grace notes are represented as

o, I nominal sixteenth-notes. However, since the


preceding notes in the left hand are nominally
o 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 160
(tied-over) quarter-notes, there is literally no time
BARS 1/2, 5/6, 17/18 for the grace notes in the rhythmic scheme; thus
they must be "taken away" from the preceding or
following note values. The standard way of
execution suggested by the spatial placement of
Figure 9, Individual variations in tempo modulation the grace notes in the score is to play them within
during MG2. The average curvature (Q) for three the last 101 of MG2-that is, after the onset of the
instances of the gesture is plotted against the curvature
for the last instance (bars 21-22). Only cases following
penultimate melody tone in the soprano voice, but
the parabolic timing shape are included. before the onset of the final long tone and the
accompanying two-tone chord. However, only 12 of
This "parabolic constraint" on the timing shape the 24 pianists consistently played the passage
of an expressive gesture is of great theoretical according to this conventional interpretation of
interest, as it supports Todd's (1985) suggestion of the notation. (The two repeats of bars 1-8 were
the parabola as a basic timing function. It also examined separately in this analysis.) It seems
appears to be consistent with his more recent that this brief passage offered pianists ample
modelling (Todd, 1992b), even though he proposed opportunity to indulge in "deviations from the
that a linear function can account for most of the score."
timing variation; clearly, a quadratic function did Most of the variants are illustrated
even better. The parabolic constraint may thus be schematically in Figure 10, which uses horizontal
understood as an allusion to physical motion (cf. displacement of the critical notes in bar 2 to
Todd, 1992b), which most performers aim at, and indicate the actual succession of tones. The
which listeners with some musical education find standard version is shown in (a). As shown in (b),
aesthetically pleasing (Repp, 1992b). some pianists delayed the last note in the left
Since MG1, the following inter-gesture interval, hand until after the octave chord in the right
and MG2 may form a coherent half-phrase that is hand: ARR (slightly, bar 6 only), DAV (bar 2 only),
planned and executed as a single expressive unit, MOl (bar 6, once in bar 2), NOV (only the first
their timing relationships acrosS the 28 time in bar 2, but very dramatically), and ZAK
performances were examined (in bars 0-2 only). (consistently). Others (c) played all notes of the
None of the three coefficients characterizing the left hand before those of the right hand: ARG
parabolic shape of MG2 correlated significantly (except the second time in bar 6), HOI and H02
with either of the two 101 ratios considered in (bar 2). In a variant of that pattern, the right-
connection with MG1 (Figure 5). However, the hand chord was arpeggiated (DEM, consistently)
total interval preceding the onset of MG2 or mysteriously interleaved with the broken chord
correlated moderately (0.59-0.72) with all three in the left hand (Horowitz, in bar 6 only, but in all
MG2 coefficients. Thus, the slower the initial three performances).
rn all instances mentioned so far, the onset of
the last melody tone of MG2 did follow the two
grace notes, so the grace notes were within; the
last r01 of the gesture. This made it possible to
ask about the relative timing of the grace notes
(a)
Jl within the ror (see below). rn some further cases,
however, one or both of the grace notes coincided
with the final melody tone. Thus, SCH consis-
tently played the right-hand octave tones simul-
taneously with the first grace note (d). BUN (bars
2 and 18) and KUB (bars 2 and 6, second repeat
only) played the right-hand tones simultaneously
with the second grace note (e). Finally, Cortot (bar
6, in all three performances; also bar 18 in COl)
played all tones simultaneously as a single chord
CD, as prescribed only in bar 22 of the score. (BUN
'3M.
* also did this once in bar 6.)12
r1J To investigate the relative timing of the grace
notes within the last ror of MG2, provided they
did fall within that lOr (cases a-c in Figure 10),
(c)
Jl the r01 was divided into three parts (A, B, and C),
defined by the respective tone onsets. Two interval
ratios were calculated: A/(BC), which indicates the
relative delay of the onset of the first grace note,
~
* and B/C, which represents the duration of the first
r1J grace-note ror relative to the second. If the first
melody tone and the two grace notes were played
as a triplet, for example, the two ratios would be
(d) I.J Jl 0.5 and 1, respectively; if the grace notes were
played as thirty-second notes following a
sixteenth-note rest, the ratios would both be 1.
Figure 11 plots A/(B+C) against B/C for all in-
~
* stances where these ratios could be determined.
,r1J (Bars 2 and 6 are each represented by a single ra-
tio, averaged across the two repeats.) A wide
range of ratios was found, but most data points
(e)
Jl form a cluster in the lower left quadrant, suggest-
ing that the majority of pianists agreed on a par-
ticular timing pattern. The central tendency of
that cluster is around A/(B+C) = 0.4 and B/C = 0.5.
* This means that the grace notes were not played
evenly: The first grace note started about 40% into
the ror, and the second grace note was about
twice as long as the first, which means it started
(f)
Jl about 60% into the ror, on the average.
Considering the compressed scale of the ordinate
in the figure, however, it is clear that there was
I wide variation in the relative durations of the two
~
* grace notes, even within the cluster of relative
conformity. (Some of this variation undoubtedly
reflects measurement error magnified by ratio cal-
Figure 10. Schematic illustration of six observed manners culation.) What was almost always true, however,
of execution of the grace notes accompanying MG2. The
notation for bars 2 and 18 is shown, but it stands for bar
is that the first grace note started during the first
6 as well. Spatial displacements of the notes symbolize half of the lOr (A/(B+ C) <1), and the second grace
temporal displacements. note ror was longer than the first (H/C < 1).
Diversitv and Commonalihl In Music Performance: An Anal\fsls of Timing Microstructure 11l Schumann's "Triiumerei" 249

2.5
I
0 Bar 2

2 -f--- - Bar 6

t::. Bar 18
0
Zak
V\ Zak

She.6
1.5 L
o
--
Co2
Bun
CO
1
~o

-
let::. Co10

oJa-o_t
t::.
~_O
0
0
t::.. She
o Kli
~ t::.
0.5 ~I ~ Cao
L...J

~u t
0
R~ :e~
-t::.

a
a 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 1.6
A/(B+C)
Figure 11. Relative timing of the grace notes within the last 101 of MG2, for cases (aHc) in Figure 10. The abscissa
shows the ratio of the time before the onset of the first grace note (A) and the remainder of the 101 (Be); the ordinate
shows the ratio of the time between the two grace note onsets (B) and the time from the onset of the second grace note
to the end of the 101 (C). Ratios for bars 2 and 6 are averaged over the two repeats.

There were some notable exceptions to this which on the whole were played as in bars 2-3 and
pattern, though no individual pianist was 18-19. MG5a and MG6f in bars 23-24, which
consistently deviant. Assuming that these carried the final large ritardando, will be
instances are not simply slips of motor control, it considered separately later on.
would be interesting to know what led individual As noted earlier, the average timing profiles for
artists to vary their timing patterns in these ways. bars 2-4 and 18-20 were extremely similar, though
4. MG3a-MG6a. We turn now to a closer bars 18-20 were played at a somewhat slower
examination of the MG chains making up the tempo overall (see Figure 3). In the principal
second half of each phrase. The Type a chain components analysis conducted on bars 0-8, the
occurs in identical form in bars 2-4 and in bars 18- first factor (see Figure 4, top panel) exhibited a
20 (see Figure 2). It also occurs in varied and regular sequence of peaks for MG3a-MG6a, which
abbreviated form in bars 22-24. We will not deal reflect lengthening of accented tones and MG-final
specifically with MG3a and MG3b in bars 22-23, lOIs. Detailed examination of the individual
250 Repp

timing patterns, however, revealed enormous significant factors emerged-more than from the
variety, though not without constraints. No two earlier analysis of bars 0-8, where the larger
pianists' interpretations of this MG chain were timing deviations in MG1 and MG2 dominated the
quite the same, and some artists also changed correlation structure. Thus, there were at least six
their patterns significantly between bars 2-4 and distinct (i.e., uncorrelated) timing patterns
bars 18-20. (The two repeats of bars 2-4 will not be underlying the variation in the data; these
considered separately here, though some pianists patterns are shown in Figure 12. None of them is
played even these differently.) fully representative of any individual
To explore this variability in a reasonably performance, however, and together they account
economical way, principal components analysis for only 81% of the variance. The factor scores for
was used once again, but this time only on the 2 x bars 2-4 and 18-20 were highly similar and are
18 36 IOIs comprising MG3a-MG6a in bars 2-4 superimposed in the figure. The factor loadings of
and 18-20, concatenated. From this analysis, six the individual performances are shown in Table 6.

1000
MG6a
1000 -.- Bars 2-4

900 Q] 900 -e- Bars 18-20

800 800

700 700

600 600

500 500

400 400
.... (") If) .... (") If) .... .... (") If) .... ~ (") If) ....
....--.. c\J M M M M ~ ~ ~ ~ c\J M M M M ~ ~ ~ ~
E
.........
1000 1000

(f)
W
900
OJD 900
~
a: 800 800
0
() 700 700
(f)
a: 600 600
0
I- 500 500
()

u..
400
.... ~ (") If) .... ~ (") If) ....
400
.... (") If) .... ~ (") If) ....
c\J M M M M ~ ~ ~ ~. c\J M M M M ~ ~ ~ ~
1000 1000

900
~ 900 @J
800 800

700 700

600 600

500 500

400 400
.... ~ (") If) .... <'? If) .... .... C') If) .... <'? "( r-;-
c\J M M M M ~ ~ ~ ~ c\J M M M M ~ ~ ~ ~

BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE

Figure 12. Rescaled factor scores (underlying timing patterns) for the MG chain of Type a in bars 2-4 and 18-20. The
abscissa labels refer to bars 2-4.
Diversitll and Commonalitll m ivfusic Performance: An Anaillsis o(Timing Microstructure m Schumann's "Triiumerei" 251

Table 6. Sorted rotated factor loadings from the principal components analysis ofMG3a-MG6a. (Loadings below 0.4
are omitted.)

II III IV V VI

ARR 0.773
NEY 0.758
BRE 0.744
ASH 0.708
KAT 0.671 0.437
ESC 0.606 0.416
H03 0.908
H02 0.893
HOI 0.830
ARG 0.629 0.402
COl 0.873
CO2 0.832
cm 0.784
ORT 0.446 -0.593
KUB 0.823
NOV 0.808
SCH 0.786
CUR 0.676 0.541
MOl 0.864
SHE 0.564 0.693
DAV 0.456 0.574
GIA 0.410 0.535 0.564
KLI 0.576 0.602
DEM 0.457 0.436 0.565
ZAK 0.427 0.515 0.540
BUN 0.431 0.410 0.491
KRU 0.414 0.500 0.436 -0.435
CAP 0.455

Factor I (VAF = 18%) is characterized by accel- lengthening in MG5a; and some shortening of the
eration during MG3a; a fast traversal of MG4a; a final 101 in MG6a. Some additional peaks appear
dramatic ritardando within MG5a with maximal in bars 18-20. Cortot's real performances are simi-
lengthening of the final, unaccented note (position lar, especially COl and C02, except that the ten-
4-2), further augmented in bars 18-20; and a dencies in bars 18-20 are much more exaggerated.
pronounced lengthening of the last 101 in MG6a In C03, there is much lengthening in position 20-
(position 4-8). The pianists whose interpretations 2 instead of 20-3 (read 4-2 and 4-3 on the abscissa
come closest to this pattern are ARR, NEY, BRE, in Figure 12). Note the sizeable negative loading of
and ASH. ORT.
Factor II (VAF = 17%) is the "Horowitz factor." Factor N (VAF = 14%) shows pronounced final
It is characterized by significant lengthening of lengthening in both MG3a and MG4a; a "flat"
the accented tone in MG3a as well as of the pre- MG5a with slight final lengthening; and a smooth
ceding unaccented tone; slight deceleration during ritardando through MG6a, with a very long last
MG4a; final lengthening in MG5a; and a ritar- 101. Bars 2-4 and 18-20 are executed very simi-
dando during MG6a, followed by a sudden short- larly. This beautifully regular pattern comes clos-
ening of the last 101. Some of these tendencies est to the actual interpretations of KUB, NOV,
were exaggerated in bars 18-20 relative to bars 2- and SCH.
4. Horowitz's actual performances (especially H02 Factor V (VAF = 11%) shows a rather flat
and H03) resemble this pattern, except that the pattern, with initial acceleration and pronounced
exaggeration in bars 18-20 is much more dra- final deceleration, but with the final 101 shorter
matic. H03 differs in that bars 2-4 and 18-20 are that the penultimate one. This pattern is the least
executed very similarly, both showing dramatic differentiated, as if the whole MG chain were
lengthening in positions 4-2 and 4-3. thought of as a single gesture. This pattern comes
Factor III (VAF = 14%) is the "Cortot factor." It closest to MOl's performance, though he does
is characterized by a fast start; progressive slow- show some small local peaks in positions 3-5 and
ing down during MG4a and MG5a, with final 4-1.
Repp

Factor VI (VAF = 7%), finally, begins with a accented tone. The factor profiles essentially
pattern for MG3a and MG4a that is very nearly represent a varying focus on individual MGs in
the opposite of Factor II; MG5a shows striking the chain.
final lengthening; and MG6a shows only a small Factor I (VAF = 22%) shows substantial
lengthening of the final 101. There is an overall lengthening in MG3b, much less in MG4b, and
trend to accelerate during the MG chain. This somewhat more in MG5b. The timing profiles for
pattern is not representative of any individual the three instances of the MG chain are very
performance and occurs only in mixed patterns, similar. The pianist most closely associated with
such as those ofKLI, DEM, and ZAK, all of which this pattern is ARR, and a number of other artists,
exhibit pronounced lengthening in position 4-3. including Horowitz, show substantial loadings in
In summary, the information contained in this factor. (In contrast to the Type a chain, there
Figure 12 and Table 6 offers only a moderate was no "Horowitz factor" here.)
amount of data reduction. The timing patterns of Factor II (VAF = 21%) is characterized by total
individual artists were remarkably diverse. absence of lengthening in MG3b and variability
Nevertheless, there are many possible patterns during MG4b and MG5b, usually with more pre-
that never occurred, such as lengthening in accentuation lengthening than accentuation
position 3-4 (the second 101 of MG4a), which lengthening. Two performances showing this pat-
clearly was treated as transitional by all pianists. tern in relatively pure form are Cal and KRU,
Nor were the patterns random in any way; the and several other performances (including C02)
majority of the artists produced very similar show substantial loadings. Thus this was again a
timing patterns in bars 2-4 and 18-20, and also in kind of "Cortot factor," though it was not unique to
the two repeats of bars 2-4 (which were averaged Cortot. However, C03 deviates from this pattern
here). The performances with the highest and is responsible for a unique factor, after all
consistency between bars 2-4 and 18-20 were (Factor VI).
ARR, CAP, HOI, and KUB, whereas the most Factor III (VAF = 14%) is characterized by a flat
variable ones were BUN, Cal, C02, H02, H03, pattern (bars 6-7) or pronounced accelerando (bars
and aRT. That Cortot's and Horowitz's within- 10-11 and 14-15) during MG3b, variable lengthen-
performance variations were carefully planned is ing in MG4b, and pronounced lengthening in
suggested by the fact that they were replicated in MG5b. NEY, KAT, and DAV are the best repre-
different performances (only HOI retained the sentatives.
pattern of bars 2-4 for bars 18-20). Whether The remaining three factors account for much
BUN's and aRT's inconsistencies were similarly less variance (8% in the case of Factors IV and V,
planned is not known, since only a single 6% in that of Factor VI), and each of them is
performance by each pianist was available for largely associated with one particular artist.
examination. These three artists, which earlier analyses have
5. MG3b-MG6b. The Type b version of the MG already revealed to be somewhat eccentric, are
chain occurs three times in the piece: first in bars aRT, BUN, and C03. Thus, aRT put increasing
6-8, and then in bars 10-12 and 14-16. Because of emphasis on MG4b in successive renditions, BUN
the substantial ritardandi that occur during showed a very atypical peak in position 7-7 and
MG6b, it was decided to treat the last six lOIs of tremendous lengthening in MG4b in bar 7 only,
each chain separately. The remaining 3 x 12 = 36 and C03 showed no lengthening in MG3b and
lOIs were subjected to principal components anal- MG4b, and a very variable execution of MG5b.
ysis, which yielded six significant factors, just as On the whole, within-performance variability
for the Type a passage. They accounted for 80% of across the three instances of the Type b chain
the variance. Their timing profiles are shown in seemed larger than for the Type a chain, whereas
Figure 13, and the matrix of factor loadings is between-performance variability seemed some-
shown in Table 7. As for the Type a chain, the what lower. The former observation is not
factor patterns are only rarely representative of surprising, for the two MG chains of Type a are
individual artists' patterns, which more often are identical, whereas the three chains of Type b
a combination of several factor patterns. It may be differ harmonically and melodically. No pianist
recalled that the grand average timing profile showed nearly identical patterns for all three
showed a fairly regular lengthening of pre-ac- instances of Type b. Some pianists were markedly
cented and accented lOIs in each MG, which more variable than others, however, not to say
really represents final lengthening of the erratic. BUN leads the list, which also includes
accompanying secondary MG, which ends with an ARG, C03, DAV, and MOL The relatively most
Diversitll and Commonality in Music Performance: An Anaillsis of Timing Microstructure in Schumanrz's "Triiumerei" 253

consistent pianists were ASH, DEM, H02, KLI, at the end of the piece. A substantial slowdown
NEY, and ZAK. Some pianists (BRE, KRD, SCB) occurs also in bar 16, at the end of period B. Less
were notable for their relatively flat and pronounced ritardandi occur in bar 8, at the end
uninflected rendering of the whole Type b chain. 13 of period A, and in bar 12, at the end of phrase b2.
6. The ritardandi. As is evident from the grand Thus there are four locations where major
average timing pattern plotted in Figure 3, major ritardandi are observed. It may be asked whether
ritardandi occurred at the ends of periods and the temporal shapes of these ritardandi followed
phrases. The largest ritardando is in bars 23-24, any common pattern.

-+- Bars 68
750

650
[I]
550 550

450 450

350 350

-
( J)
E
750
67 7-3 7-5 7-7 8-1
750
6-7 7-1 7-3 75 7-7 8-1

----
(J) DE]
W 650 650
a:
0 550
<-) 550
(J)

a:
0
450 450
f-
~ 350 I
350
LL 67 7-1 7-3 7-5 77 8-1 6-7 71 7-3 7-5 77 . 8-1
750 750

650 650
C2TI
550 550

450 450

350 350
6-7 7-1 7-3 7-5 77 8-1 6-7 7-1 7-3 7-5 7-7 8-1

BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE

Figure 13. Rescaled factor scores <underlying timing patterns) for MG chain of Type b in bars 6-8, and 10-12, and 14-16.
The abscissa labels refer to bars 6-8.
254 Repp

Table 7. Sorted rotated factor loadings from the principal components analysis of MG3b-MG5b. (Loadings below 0.4
are omitted.)

II III IV V VI

ARR 0.923
HOI 0.788
ARG 0.745
H03 0.740
KLI 0.715
MOl 0.701
ASH 0.648 0.521
DEM 0.589 0.582
ESC 0.553 0.490
GIA 0.531 0.455
CAP 0.515 0.438 0.450
COl 0.860
KRU 0.829
BRE 0.733
CO2 0.699
NOV 0.418 0.691
SHE 0.675 0.407
CUR 0.664 0.488
SCH 0.629 0.404
KUB 0.530 0.416
NEY 0.814
KAT 0.746
DAV 0.721
ZAK 0.407 0.642 0.416
ORT 0.893
BUN 0.756
C03 0.863
H02 0.413 0.452

This question was addressed previously by results concerning the temporal shape of MG2.
Sundberg and Verrillo (1980) and Kronman and The decelerando in that ascending melodic gesture
Sundberg (1987) with regard to the ritardandi may be considered an instance of a' local
observed at the ends of performances of "motor ritardando, and the principles involved may be
music," mostly by Bach. Sundberg and Verrillo quite the same. A square-root (convex) function
plotted an average "retard curve" in terms of fitted to reciprocal lOIs implies a quadratic
inverse lOIs and observed that it could be (concave) function fitted to the original lOIs.
described in terms of two linear functions: a However, it is difficult to reach strong conclusions
gradual slowdown followed by a more rapid one. from Sundberg's examination of timing patterns
The latter phase often included only three data averaged across performances of different music.
points, however, and tended to coincide with the The melodic grouping structure must be taken
last melodic gesture in the music. Kronman and into account, which was done only to a very
Sundberg abandoned the bilinear model and limited extent by Sundberg and his colleagues.
interpreted the average retard curve as a single This is much easier, of course, when dealing
continuous function. That function was described with multiple performances of a single piece of
well by expressing local tempo (lIIOl) as the music, as in the present case. Inspection of the
square root of the normalized distance from a grand average timing profile in Figure 3 suggests
. hypothetical zero point located one beat beyond that ritardandi progress smoothly within melodic
the onset of the final tone. Kronberg and gestures but are interrupted between gestures.
Sundberg speculated that this function may be an Thus, the final ritardandi in bars 12 and 16 (read
allusion to physical deceleration in natural bar 8 on the abscissa) can be seen to begin with
activities such as walking. the second 101, but a "reset" occurs after the
That the average ritardando followed a fourth 101, which corresponds to a gestural
quadratic function is intriguing given the present boundary. The final lengthening of MG5b causes
Diversitll and Commonalitllll1 lvfusic Performance: All Allah/sis of Timzlll: Microstructure III Scizwnann 's "Traumae:" :!:1:'

the initial ritardando to "overshoot" its trajectory,


which is resumed with the onset of MG6b. These
ritardandi thus are divided into two sequences of
3 and 4 IOIs, respectively. Similarly, in the large
final ritardando in bars 23-24, which begins as 1000
early as position 23-3 or 23-4, two resets occur,
corresponding to the ends of MG4a and MG5a,
which in this instance are unmistakably indicated 900 I
in the score by "commas." This grand ritardando ---.
(f) /
V
thus is divided into three groups of IOIs: 3(4), 4, 800
and 2. Because meaningful fitting of quadratic
curves requires at least four data points, the
following analyses examine one sequence of four
--0E 700 /
coherent lOIs from each of the three ritardandi,
forming either part of MG6b or of MG5a. The 600 /
analyses were analogous to those conducted on ~
MG2. 500
In each case, it was found that a quadratic curve 12-5 12-6 12-7 12-8
fit the average 101 pattern quite well, perfectly so
in the case of MG5a. These curves are shown in 2200
Figure 14. Quadratic functions were subsequently
fitted to all 28 individual performance timing
patterns of each ritardando section. Most fits
ranged from excellent to acceptable (see Footnote ---.
en
1800
V
I
9). In the case of MG6b (bar 12), there were five
clearly deviant cases (ARG, H03, KUB, and ORT, 51400 /
-
all of whom shortened the last IOI, and KLI, who
showed a rather flat pattern); for MG6b (bar 16)
there was none; for MG6a there was one (ARG,
0
1000 /
/'
who shortened the last 10l).
-.-
~/
The coefficients of the quadratic equations
describing the acceptable (and better) fits were 600
found to be highly correlated in all instances, just 16-5 16-6 16-7 16-8
as for MG2. Thus it was again possible to
construct families of parabolas that capture a
1400
major portion of the individual variation in the
shapes of the ritardandi. These curves are shown
in Figure 15. Taking into account the different ---.
1200 /
time scales on the abscissa, it seems that each
ritardando has its distinctive range of variation,
but all share the property of being largely
(f)

51000 /
parabolic in shape. These results for strictly
intragestural ritardandi support the observations
on more heterogeneous materials by Sundberg
o
800
~
1/
and Verrillo (1980) and Kronman and Sundberg
(1987). 600 I I
A final analysis was conducted on the last two
23-7 23-8 24-1 24-2
lOIs of the piece. While it did not make sense to fit
any function to them, their correlation could be BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
examined, which was 0.87. Thus, the longer the
penultimate 101, the longer the final 10I.l4 Not
only was the relationship quite linear, but the
regression line passed almost through the origin, Figure 14. Ritardando functions: Quadratic functions
suggesting that the two lOIs were in a constant fitted to the grand average (geometric mean) lOIs in
proportion (1:1.8). MG6b (bars 12 and 16) and in MG5a (bars 23-24).
256 Repp

any other artist's, even if just one physical


dimension (timing) is considered. Even the same
artist's performances on different occasions, while
demonstrably similar, are sufficiently different to
be considered distinct and individual events. Yet,
1200 there are also significant commonalities that
0 o ~ -40 apparently reflect constraints on performance, at
o ~ 20 least on those performances that have been
1000
~
A 0=0 deemed suitable for commercial distribution. The
(/J

E .L 0=20 individual variations among performances largely


~ 800
0 0=40 take place within these constraints, although
6 there are always exceptions, representing
600 0= 60
conscious or unconscious transgressions of the
"l 0=80
boundaries established by musical convention.
400 T 0=100
How should those boundaries be characterized?
125 126 12-7 12-8
And are they purely conventional (i.e., arbitrary),
2400 0=0 or do they represent more general laws of motor
0
behavior and perception that music performance
2100
0=40
must conform to in order to be naturally
~1800 A 0=80
(/J expressive?
i=
-=-1500 .L 0=120 It is probably futile to attempt to characterize
Q 1200 0 0= 160 the boundaries of acceptable performance practice.
They are manifold and are likely to contract and
900 0=200
expand as a function of many factors.- It is
600
"l 0=240 theoretically more parsimonious to conceive of a
16-5 16-6 16-7 16-8 performance ideal (norm, prototype) that lies at
1600 the center of the hypothetical space enclosed by
0 0=20
the boundaries. This ideal may be thought of as a
1400 0=0 relatively abstract specification that contains free
A 0=20 parameters, so that a multiplicity (if not an
:-[1200 .L 0=40 infinity) of concrete performances can be
0 0=60 generated that all more or less satisfy the norm.
6 1000
Because of the enormous complexity of serious
800
0=80
music, any concrete performance ideal is very
"l 0= 100
difficult to attain; and, if one were attained, the
T 0=120
600 artist would probably change it the next time,
237 23-8 24-1 24-2
because art thrives on variety. However, the
BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
underlying abstract ideal may remain constant, as
it embodies generally accepted rules of
performance practice. Concrete realizations are
Figure 15. Families of parabolic timing functions for the conceived, perceived, and judged with reference to
ritardandi in MG6b (bars 12 and 16) and in MG5a (bars the underlying norm, but deviation from the norm
23-24). (within certain limits of acceptability) can-to
some extent, must-be an artistic goal. That is,
v. GENERAL DISCUSSION diversity is as necessary as is commonality: Both
The present comprehensive analysis of uniformity and lack of an aesthetic standard are
expressive timing patterns in 28 performances of detrimental to art.
Schumann's "Traumerei" provides an objective One obvious free parameter is tempo. Even
view of the commonalities and differences among though there may be an "ideal tempo" for a piece
great artists' interpretations of one of the of music, this is again an abstraction. There is in
masterpieces of the piano literature. At first fact a range of acceptable tempi, and individuals
glance, the differences are perhaps more striking may differ considerably in what they consider
than the commonalities. There is ample material "the" ideal tempo. This is illustrated by the per-
here to support the view that every artist's formance sample examined here, which represents
performance is, in some sense, unique and unlike a very wide range of tempi, nearly all of which
Diversity and Commonalitv in Music Performance: An Analysis of Timing Microstructure in Schumann's "Triiumerei" 257

seem acceptable to a musical listener (the author reflects is the increasing pattern variability at
must rely on subjective judgment here), even lower levels of the structural hierarchy.
though some sound clearly slow, while others This variability is not likely to reflect a decrease
sound fast, To the author, only ESC really sounds of control over precision in timing, though some of
"too slow" (though perhaps even that judgment it may. Pianists at the level of accomplishment
might change if his performance were heard in the studied here can control their timing patterns
context of the whole "Kinderszenen" suite), while down to a very fine grain. What the variability
that of DAV sounds "too fast," though apparently presumably reflects is a relaxation of the perfor-
(Clara) Schumann wanted it that way (see Repp, mance constraints imposed by the grouping
1992d). An unusually fast "Traumerei" indeed structure at lower levels of the hierarchy. Not only
seems to come across better than an unusually are the boundaries between individual melodic
slow one. gestures perhaps less definite than those between
The present investigation substantiates two ab- larger units, but they are weaker determinants of
stract timing constraints embodied in the hypo- the timing pattern and compete with other local
thetical performance ideal. One of them concerns factors including harmonic progression, melodic
the temporal marking of the melodic/rhythmic pitch contour, and texture. It also seemS that the
structure, the other one has to do with the tempo- major source of timing variation is not artists'
ral shaping of individual melodic gestures, partic- choice of unexpected locations for expressive
ularly of the ritardandi within them. The first lengthening (though some of this occurred, too)
constraint is by now well known and is imple- but varying degrees of emphasis on expected
mented in Todd's (1985) model of expressive tim- locations. Thus, pianists often omitted
ing at the phrase leveL The principle is that lengthening where the detailed grouping structure
boundaries in the hierarchical grouping structure might have predicted it, whereas they
are generally marked by ritardandi whose extent overemphasized other grouping boundaries, as if
is roughly proportional to the "depth" of the to compensate. Sometimes this resulted in the
boundary. Thus the most extensive ritardando oc- creation of hypergestures, which combined two or
curs at the end of the piece, where boundaries at three elementary melodic motifs. In other words,
all levels coincide; substantial ritardandi occur at the lowest level of the grouping hierarchy is
the ends of major sections, such as 8-bar periods; structurally flexible; it permits the marking of
smaller ritardandi occur at the ends of individual group boundaries but does not prescribe it. Artists
phrases and gestures. A performance system such choose from among the possibilities in a manner
as that devised by Todd may come close to the comparable to varying focus in a spoken sentence.
principles embodied in the performance ideal, The other constraint observed in the present
though it is not known at present whether a per- data is more novel and more tentative. It is that,
formance strictly following such rules would in within melodic gestures requiring a ritardando for
fact be perceived as "ideal" by listeners. There is whatever reason, this local tempo change is best
some perceptual evidence, however, that listen- executed such that successive lOIs follow a
ers-even those without much musical educa- parabolic function. This constraint was not only
tion-expect phrase-final and gesture-final observed in the majority of performances at four
lengthening to occur (Repp, 1992a). different locations in the music (nine, if the
What is clear from the performances examined different occurrences of MG2 are counted
here is that variability increases at lower levels of separately), but it also is in agreement with the
the structural hierarchy. Virtually every per- observations of Kronman and Sundberg (1987) on
former observes the major ritardandi at the ends the shape of final ritardandi, as originally
of major sections, though in different degrees. described by Sundberg and Verrillo (1980).
When the complete performance timing profiles Sundberg and Verrillo also provided perceptual
were subjected to principal components analysis, data suggesting that listeners prefer ritardandi
only a single factor emerged, which reflected the corresponding to the original (quadratic) timing
qualitative conformity in that regard. When the curves over other possible timing profiles, and a
analysis was restricted to the first 8 bars only, recent study by this author (Repp, 1992b) on the
four factors emerged. When the analysis focused perceptual evaluation of different timing patterns
on half a phrase, omitting major ritardandi, six for MG2 led to similar conclusions.
factors emerged. It seems paradoxical that the It is not clear at present whether the parabolic
number of independent factors increases as the timing constraint can also be applied to melodic
number of data points decreases. What this gestures that do not include any pronounced
258 Repp

ritardando; certainly other factors, such as accent and Cortot are two of the most unusual pianists of
location, would have to be taken into account. The this century, and their greatness may lie precisely
larger the number of successive tones in a melodic in their individuality, which challenges the
gesture, the stronger the constraint is likely to be; listener. However, in the context oflistening to 28
a minimum of five tones (4 lOIs) is required. It different performances in sequence, which height-
also remains to be seen whether a similar timing ens one's sensitivity to differences and perhaps in-
constraint operates at higher levels of the creases one's reliance on an internalized perfor-
grouping hierarchy. Todd (1985) postulated a mance ideal, the unusual performances do not fare
parabolic timing function as the prototype for . so well. The author's favorites are the perfor-
within-group timing at the phrase level, though mances by Curzon, Brendel, and Ashkenazy,
apparently more for the sake of convenience than which usually were in the middle field in the vari-
for any stringent theoretical or empirical reason. ous analyses reported above, and hence were
Kronman and Sundberg (1987) hint at a mentioned only rarely. The author's aesthetic
grounding of the parabolic timing constraint in ideal seemed to correspond to the central tendency
elementary principles of (loco)motion, but without of the sample examined here. Whether this ideal
elaborating on this hypothesis. Todd (1992b) has represents a stable representation of traditional
claimed that a piecewise-linear function is performance norms or whether it is a form of psy-
sufficient to describe local timing changes during chophysical adaptation to the range of the perfor-
a performance, but his own (rather limited) data mance sample is an intriguing question that war-
suggest that a quadratic function provides a better rants further investigation. Also, the absence of
fit. At this point, the general hypothesis may be the context of the preceding and following pieces
stated that a parabolic timing profile is in some of "Kinderszenen" must be acknowledged; inter-
sense "natural" for both performer and listener, pretations that sound unusual in isolation may
and probably not for purely conventional reasons. sound more convincing in context. Finally, it must
A better understanding of the origins of this be noted that the author's impressions derived not
constraint may elucidate the meaning of the only from the timing patterns of these perfor-
motion metaphor that is often applied to music mances, but also from their intensity patterns
and its performance (cf. Todd, 1992a, 1992b; (both "horizontal" and "vertical"), their articula-
Truslit, 1938). tion and pedaling, and their overall sound quality.
The present performance analyses revealed no There are many parameters that contribute to the
clustering of individual artists according to sex, subjective impression of a performance, but the
age, or national origin. Their individuality timing pattern is probably the most important
apparently transcended these other factors, whose one. Nevertheless, future performance analyses
relevance to music performance may be will have to consider these other parameters
questioned in any case. There is no doubt, (which are much more difficult to measure in
however, that in this sample of highly recorded performances) as well as the subjective
accomplished artists, perhaps precisely because of impressions of more than one experienced listener.
their level of artistry, some performances seemed Ultimately, we would like to know why individual
unusual and even deviant. Subjective impressions performers play the way they do, and what mes-
will have to suffice for the time being: Long before sage they are sending to listeners (cf. Kendall &
objective measurements confirmed the actual Carterette, 1990).
deviations in their timing patterns, the The present research illustrates one of two
performances by Argerich, Bunin, and Cortot, and complementary approaches to the objective
to a lesser degree those by Horowitz and Ortiz, investigation of musical performance. The other
struck the author as eccentric and distorted. approach is represented by the work of Sundberg
Interestingly, some of these pianists (especially and his colleagues on a system of performance
Bunin) also turned out to be inconsistent within rules (e.g., Friberg, 1991; Sundberg, 1988;
their own performance, as if they had no fixed Sundberg, Friberg, & Fryden, 1989) and by Todd's
concept and were exploring possibilities. Other (1985, 1992a, 1992b) modelling of musical motion.
artists, however, were remarkably consistent, for These models are ingenious and important, but
example Arrau. The author's subjective they need to be tested on large performance data
impressions were not only highly reliable on bases, which do not exist at present. In this study,
relistening, but they also correspond to what a very modest beginning was made towards
professional critics have to say about some of accumulating and organizing data of sufficient
these pianists' performances. Certainly, Horowitz depth and variety (though still restricted to a
Diversity and CommonaLitv in Music Performance: An AnaLvsis of Timing Microstructure in Schumann's "Triiumerei" 259

single composition and a single parameter, Horowitz, J. (1990). The ivory trade: Music and the business of music
timing) to present a challenge and testing ground at the Van Cliburn International Piano Competition. New York:
Summit Books.
for emerging models of music performance. 15 Kendall, R. A, & Carterette, E. C. (1990). The communication of
Soon, of course, much more extensive data bases musical expression. Music Perception, 8, 129-163.
should become available with the help of Kronrnan, U., & Sundberg, J. (1987). Is the musical ritard an
technological marvels such as the MIDI-controlled allusion to physical motion? In A. Gabrielsson (Ed.), Action and
grand piano. There are exciting times ahead for Perception in Rhythm and Music (pp. 57-68). Stockholm:
Publications issued by the Royal Swedish Academy of Music
research on music performance, one of the most No. 55.
advanced and culturally significant human skills. Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal
music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lipman, S. (1984). Glenn Gould: His dissent/ An obituary
REFERENCES (originally published in 1982). In S. Lipman, The house of music:
Bengtsson, 1., & Gabrielsson, A. (1980). Methods for analyzing Art in an era of institutions (pp. 79-95). Boston: Godine.
performance of musical rhythm. Scandinavian Journal of Lipman, S. (1990). Arguing for music/Arguing for culture. Boston:
PsychoLogy,21,257-268. Godine.
Brendel, A. (1981). Der Interpret muss erwachsen sein. Zu Lussy, M. M. (1882). Musical expression: Accents, nuances, and tempo
Schumann's "Kinderszenen." Musica, 6, 429-433. in vocal and instrumental music. London: Novello.
Chissell, J. (1987). Liner notes for Claudio Arrau's recording Palmer, C. (1989). Mapping musical thought to musical
(Philips 420-871-2). performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Clarke, E. F. (1982). Timing in the performance of Erik Satie's Perception and Perfornlance, 15, 331-346.
'Vexations.' Acta Psychologica, 50, 1-19. Povel, D.-J. (1977). Temporal structure of performed music: some
Clarke, E. F. (1983). Structure and expression in rhythmic preliminary observations. Acta Psychologica, 41, 309-320.
performance. In P. Howell, I. Cross, & R. West (Eds.), Musical Repp, B. H. (1990). Patterns of expressive timing in performances
structure and cognition (pp. 209-236). London: Academic Press. of a Beethoven minuet by nineteen famous pianists. Journal of
Clarke, E. F. (1985). Some aspects of rhythm and expression in the Acoustical Society of America, 88, 622-641.
performances of Erik Satie's "Gnossienne No.5." Music Repp, B. H. (1992a). Probing the cognitive representation of
Perception, 2,299328. musical time: Structural constraints on the perception of timing
Clarke, E. F. (1988). Generative principles in music performance. perturbations. Cognition, 44, 241-281.
In J. A. Sloboda (Ed.), Generative processes in music (pp. 1-26). Repp, B. H. (1992b). A constrainton the expressive timing of a
Oxford: Clarendon Press. melodic gesture: Evidence from performance and aesthetic
Cline, E. (1985). Piano competitions: An analysis of their structure, judgment. Music Perception, 10,221-242.
value, and educational implications. Doctoral dissertation, Indiana Repp, B. H. (1993a). Objective performance analysis as a tool for
University. the musical detective. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
Clynes, M. (1983). Expressive microstructure in music, linked to 93 (forthcoming).
liVing qualities. In J. Sundberg (Ed.), Studies of music performance Repp, B. H. (1993b). On determining the global tempo of a
(pp. 76-181). Stockholm, Sweden: Publication issued by the temporally modulated music performance.
Royal Swedish Academy of Music No. 39. Reti, R. (1951). The thematic process in music. New York: Macmillan.
Clynes, M. (1987). What can a musician learn about music Riemann, H. (1884). Der Ausdruck in der Musik. In P. Graf
performance from newly discovered microstructure principles Waldersee (Ed.), Sammlung musikalischer Vortriige (pp. 43-64).
(PM and PAS)? In A. Gabrielsson (Ed.), Action and perception in Leipzig: Breitkopf und Hartel.
rhythm and music (pp. 201-233). Stockholm, Sweden: Publication Seashore, C. E. (1936) (Ed.) Objective analysis of music performance.
issued by the Royal Swedish Academy of Music No. 55. Iowa City, IA: The University Press (University of Iowa Studies
Friberg, A. (1991). Generative rules for music performance: A in the Psychology of Music, Vol. IV).
formal description of a rule system. Computer Music Journal, 15, Seashore, C. E. (1938). Psychology of music. New York: McGraw-
56-71. Hill. (New York: Dover Publications, 1967.)
Gabrielsson, A. (1974). Performance of rhythm patterns. Seashore, C. E. (1947). In search of beauty in music: A scientific
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 15,63-72. approach to musical esthetics. New York: Ronald Press.
Gabrielsson, A (1985). Interplay between analysis and synthesis Shaffer, 1. H. (1980). Analysing piano performance: a study of
in studies of music performance and music experience. Music concert pianists. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials
Perception, 3, 59-86. in motor behavior (pp. 443-455). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Gabrielsson, A. (1987). Once again: the theme from Mozart's Shaffer, 1. H. (1981). Performances of Chopin, Bach, and Bartok:
Piano Sonata in A major (K. 331). In A. Gabrielsson (Ed.), Action Studies in motor programming. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 326-
and perception in rhythm and music (pp. 81-103). Stockholm, 376.
Sweden: Publication issued by the Royal Swedish Academy of Shaffer, 1. H. (1984). Timing in solo and duet piano performances.
Music No. 55. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 577-595.
Gabrielsson, A. (1988). Timing in music performance and its Shaffer, 1. H. (1989). Cognition and affect in musical performance.
relation to music experience. In J.A. Sloboda (Ed.), Generative Contemporary Music Review, 4, 381-389.
processes in music (pp. 27-51). Oxford: Clarendon Press. Shaffer, 1. H., Clarke, E. F., & Todd, N. P. (1985). Metre and
Gabrielsson, A., Bengtsson, I., & Gabrielsson, B. (1983). rhythm in piano playing. Cognition, 20, 61-77.
Performance of musical rhythm in 3/4 and 6/8 meter. Shaffer, 1. H., & Todd, N. P. (1987). The interpretative component
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 24,193-213. in musical performance. In A. Gabrielsson (Ed.)! Action and
Hartmann, A (1932). Untersuchungen tiber metrisches Verhalten perception ill rhythm alld music (pp. 139-152). Stockholm,
in musikalischen Interpretationsvarianten. Archiv fur die Sweden: Publication issued by the Royal Swedish Academy of
gesamte Psychologie, 84, 103-192. Music No. 55.
260 Rqp

Stein, E. (1962). Form and performance. New York: Knopf. (New different artists. Cortot's three performances intercorrelated
York: Limelight Editions, 1989.) between 0.80 and 0.82, and Horowitz's between 0.81 and 0.92.
Sundberg, J. (1988). Computer synthesis of music performance. In Only three other values in the 28 x 28 intercorrelation matrix
J. A. Sloboda (Ed.), Generative processes in music (pp. 52-69). exceeded 0.80: CAP /ZAK (0.86), KAT/SHE (0.82), and
Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. CUR/KRU (0.81). On the other hand, some of the lowest
Sundberg, J., Friberg, A., & Fryden, L. (1989). Rules for automated correlations were observed between Cortot's and Horowitz's
performance of ensemble music. Contemporary Music Review, 3, performances (0.15 to 0.44), and also between Cortot and
89-109. several other artists (ARG, BUN, ESC, KLI, NEY, NOV). Only
Sundberg, J., & Verrillo, V. (1980). On the anatomy of the retard: two other correlations fell below 0.30: BUN/MOl (0.26) and
A study of timing in music. Journal of the Acoustical Society of H01/SCH (0.26). Most correlations were between 0.4 and 0.7.
America, 68, 772-779. Thus Cortot and Horowitz were rather extreme cases in the
Todd, N. P. (1985). A model of expressive timing in tonal music. present sample, which agrees with their general reputation as
Music Perception, 3, 33-58. highly individual artists.
Todd, N. P. (1989). A computational model of rubato. 8The possible covariation of adjacent as well as distant lOIs was
Contemporary Music Review, 3, 69-88. investigated in a principal components analysis on the large
Todd, N. P. MeA. (1992a). The dynamics of dynamics: A model of (190 x 190) matrix of intercorrelations among all lOIs,
musical expression. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, computed across the 28 performances. Its purpose was to
91,3540-3550. "retrieve" the melodic grouping structure through the factors
Todd, N. P. McA. (1992b). The kinematics of musical expression. extracted, on the expectation that lOIs would covary more
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America (in press). strongly within MGs than between. This expectation was not
Traub, A. (1981). Die "Kinderszenen" als zyklisches Werk. Musica, fulfilled; the analysis yielded a very large number of significant
6,424-426. factors, none of which captured much of the variance. They did
Truslit, A. (1938). Gestalt und Bewegung in der Musik. Berlin- reflect correlations among some adjacent lOIs and especially
Lichterfelde: Chr. Friedrich Vieweg. between lOIs in structurally analogous positions across phrases
of the same type. However, for example, there was no
significant relationship between the timing of MG1 and MG2,
FOOTNOTES nor even between the beginning and the end of MG2.
'Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 92, 2546-2568 (1993). 9Unfortunately, the software used for curve fitting (DeltaGraph)
I An effort is made in this manuscript to distinguish between notes, did not prOVide a measure of goodness of fit. Therefore,
which are graphic symbols, and tones, which are sound events. statements about this aspect of the data will remain
However, when it comes to grace notes, the distinction cannot impressionistic in this paper.
easily be made, since "grace tone" is not an acceptable term and lothe L coefficient can be understood as follows: If the constant C
"grace-note tone" is awkward. It should be understood, then, (without loss of generality) is assumed to be 0, the parabola
that "grace note" refers either to the notated symbol or the must pass through the origin, the (0,0) point. L represents the
resulting sound, depending on the context. The same ambiguity slope of a tangent through the origin. It is zero when the
holds for "chord," although "tone cluster" is an acceptable term minimum of the parabola is located at the origin. It becomes
for the sound event that may be used on occasion. negative when the minimum of the parabola moves to a
21n a third case (NEY), it was discovered that the two repeats were positive value along the abscissa, but since the left branch of
virtually identical from bar 5 on, suggesting duplication by the the parabola must still pass through the origin, this implies a
recording engineers (see Repp, 1993a). negative value along the ordinate for the minimum.
3In the designation for a "position," such as 5-8, the two lIthe correlations varied systematically across the six instances of
numbers stand for the bar and the eighth-note 101 within it, MG2: The highest correlations (0.91-0.93) were obtained for
respectively. In an expression such as "bars 5-8," however, the bars 1-2 and 17-18; lower correlations (0.85-0.86) hold for bars
two numbers stand for the first and last bars in the range 9-10 and 13-14; and the lowest correlations (both 0.79) were
referred to. found for bars 5-6 and 21-22. The less tight relationship here
4Most of the performances in the latter group strike the author as must be due to variations in global tempo among the pianists,
mannered. It seems that these pianists deliberately tried to play which may be only weakly related to the degree of tempo
differently from the norm, but were not willing or able to do so modulation in MG2.
consistently. Perhaps they intended to convey an 12For that matter, the fermata chord in bar 22, which is clearly
improvisatory quality. notated as reqUiring simultaneity of all tones, was not played
5Todd (1992a, 1992b) has recently revised and extended his by all pianists in that way: DAV and NOV played a grand
model of expressive timing. An application of this model to the arpeggio, NEY played a partial arpeggiO (two grace notes in the
present data would be most interesting but exceeds the scope left hand, as in bars 2, 6, and 18), and ASH played only the
of this paper. lowest tone in advance. These variants may have been
6That is to say, there may be an implicit, expressively modulated occasioned by small hands (note that three of the pianists are
eighth-note pulse going through the longer lOIs. As suggested female), for the chord requires a large span.
by the extent of the MG boxes in Figure 2, the first of these l30ne detail skipped in this analysis is the timing of the melody
pulses "belongs to" the preceding MG, but its actual duration grace note during the last 101 of MG5b (position 8-4). Its onset
cannot be determined unless it is marked by some tonal event generally occurred near the middle (40-60%) of the 101. A few
in another voice. MG2i, which breaks up the inter-gesture pianists played it a little earlier (CAP, C02) or later (ARG, ESC,
interval in the soprano voice, may track the implicit pulse SCH, H02, ARR). Two artists (C03, DAV) omitted it
induced by the primary MG2 and thus may reveal its final altogether.
lengthening. 14In a final display of eccentricity, BUN produced a terminal 101
7Although these performances were by no means identical, they of nearly 5 seconds duration, with ORT not far behind.
were more similar than almost any pair of performances by 15The data matrix is available from the author.

You might also like