You are on page 1of 8

Biol. Cybern.

88, 9198 (2003)


DOI 10.1007/s00422-002-0336-z
 Springer-Verlag 2003

A mode hypothesis for nger interaction during multi-nger


force-production tasks
Frederic Danion1 , Gregor Schoner2 , Mark L. Latash3 , Sheng Li3 , John P. Scholz4 , Vladimir M. Zatsiorsky3
1
UMR 6559 Mouvement et Perception, Faculte des Sciences du Sport, Universite de la Mediterranee, CNRS, 163, Avenue de Luminy,
CP 910, 13288 Marseille cedex 9, France
2
Institut fur Neuroinformatik, Ruhr-Universitat Bochum, Bochum, Germany
3
Department of Kinesiology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pa., USA
4
Department of Physical Therapy, University of Delaware, Newark, Del., USA

Received: 7 December 2001 / Accepted in revised form: 17 April 2002

Abstract. Finger forces are known to change involun- emphasized in a number of now-classical works (Bern-
tarily during multi-nger force-production tasks, even stein 1935; Gelfand and Tsetlin 1966). Within the
when a ngers involvement in a task is not consciously current study, we propose an approach that allows
changed (the enslaving eect). Furthermore, during switching the analysis of motor control problems from a
maximal force-production (MVC) tests, the force pro- set of performance variables to a set of hypothetical
duced by a given nger in a multi-nger task is smaller central control variables during tasks involving multi-
than the force generated by this nger in its single-nger nger force production.
MVC test (the force-decit eect). A set of hypothetical When a person produces isometric force with a subset
control variables modes is introduced. Modes can be of ngers within a hand, the other ngers of the hand
estimated based on individual nger forces during also produce certain forces (Zatsiorsky et al. 1998, 2000;
single-nger MVC tests. We show that a simple formal for similar ndings see also Kilbreath and Gandevia
model based on modes with only one free parameter 1994). Such involuntary force production by nonin-
accounts for nger forces during a variety of multi-nger tended ngers has been termed enslaving. The ex-
MVC tests. The free parameter accounts for the force- plicitly involved ngers are termed master-ngers, and
decit eect, and its value depends only on the number the other force-producing ngers are called slave-ngers.
of explicitly involved ngers. This approach oers a Due to the enslaving, there is no direct correspondence
simple framework for the analysis of nger interaction between neural commands to individual ngers and
during multi-nger actions. nger forces. It is unclear whether there exists a set of
independent variables controlled by the CNS during
multi-nger force production tasks. The purpose of this
work is to introduce such a hypothetical set of central
variables, that we call modes.
1 Theoretical framework We hypothesize that, for each single-nger task, the
CNS controls a unique variable (a mode) leading to
Studies of human voluntary movements commonly use force production by the master-nger as well as by the
performance variables in particular kinematic, kinetic, enslaved ngers. For instance, when a subject produces
and electromyographic ones to formulate and test force voluntarily with the index nger (I), mode I is
hypotheses on the control and coordination of move- recruited by the CNS. Due to the enslaving phenome-
ments (Hogan 1984; Atkeson 1989; Gottlieb et al. 1989; non, mode I also leads to force production by the
Uno et al. 1989; Rosenbaum et al. 1995). Indeed, it is middle (M), ring (R), and little (L) ngers. Similarly,
very tempting to formulate control hypotheses using voluntary force production by either of these latter
readily measurable variables rather than poorly acces- ngers is assumed to involve corresponding modes
sible or even metaphorical ones (cf. the equilibrium- (mode M, mode R, and mode L, respectively). There-
point hypothesis: Feldman 1986; Latash 1993; Feldman fore, a mode can be viewed as a collective variable,
and Levin 1996). However, the theoretical impossibility which leads to activation of many hand muscles
of the central nervous system (CNS) issuing control bringing about a specic relationship among nger
signals expressed in performance variables has been forces. A following question emerges: can a superpo-
sition of modes account for nger interaction during
two-, three-, and four-nger tasks? The objective of the
Correspondence to: F. Danion present study is to show that the mode approach is able
(e-mail: danion@laps.univ-mrs.fr, to account for force patterns during maximal voluntary
Tel.: +33-491-172265, Fax: +33-491-172252) contraction (MVC) tasks by several ngers, based on
92

force patterns recorded during single-nger MVC tasks. where i refers to a nger (i = I, M, R, L), and j refers to
We develop a formal model and test its ability to ac- a mode (j = I,R). For instance, FI FI;I FI;R .
count for data published earlier by our group, for a The results obtained for each nger force are presented
broad variety of studies including those of the eects of in Table 1. Actual forces measured during I, R, and IR
fatigue, aging, and handedness on multi-nger force tasks are presented in the upper part of Table 1. The
production. model predicts much higher forces than those observed
experimentally, which is due to the model failing to
account for the phenomenon of force decit. Indeed,
2 Model many studies have reported that during MVC tasks, the
force produced by a given nger in a multi-nger task is
2.1 Notion of the mode smaller than the force generated by this nger in its single-
nger task (Li et al. 1998b; for similar ndings see also
The outline of the model is presented in Fig. 1. The Ohtsuki 1981; Kinoshita et al. 1996); typically, force
model assumes the existence of four modes that the CNS decit increases when the number of ngers explicitly
manipulates according to the task. During MVC tasks, involved in a task increases. In order to account for the
each mode is assumed to be either maximally recruited force-decit eect, we have introduced in the model a free
(state = 1) or not recruited at all (state = 0), depending parameter G that attenuates the result of force summa-
on the explicitly involved nger combination. Each tion. Figure 2 illustrates this modication of the model
mode contributes to force production by each nger. In where, after summation of the eects of individual modes,
Fig. 1, this is illustrated with symbols with two sub- the result is attenuated by a factor G < 1. The magni-
scripts: the rst refers to a nger while the second refers tude of G is dened using published experimental data.
to a mode contributing to force production by this
nger. For example, in a task IR (i.e., MVC by the index
and ring ngers), the force of the index nger will reect 2.2 Identication of the G factor
a superposition of FI;I and FI;R . In the same task, the
middle and little ngers will also produce force reecting To obtain the best estimate of G, we used the most com-
superpositions of (FM;I and FM;R , and (FL;I and FL;R , plete data set (averages across subjects) on single-hand,
respectively. multi-nger MVC production published by Zatsiorsky
Let us consider the simplest possibility when super- et al. (1998). For each nger combination, a value of the
position of dierent modes results in a simple summa- G factor was dened as the ratio of the actual total force
tion of forces. During MVC production by I and R to the predicted force computed as the sum of forces
ngers, one can expect: expected from individual mode eects. For instance, in
X the previous example of the IR task (Table 1), G equals
Fi Fi;j
j
82.3/134.2, that is 0.61 (see row b).

Fig. 1. The mode approach. This diagram


illustrates the notion of modes during a
maximal force-production (MVC) task by the
index (I) and ring (R) nger (IR) task. During
this two-nger task, two modes are activated
(MI and MR , while the two other ones (MM
and ML , involving the middle and little ngers,
respectively) remains silent. Given that each
mode inuences the force produced by each
nger, one needs to combine the eect of MI
and MR to predict forces of individual nger
during the IM task. See text for further details
93

Table 1. Computation of the gain factor G. This table shows how Table 2. Values of the gain factor G as a function of the task
nger forces produced during MVC production by the index or the performed by dierent nger(s) to generate an MVC. The value of
ring nger (I and R tasks) can be possibly used to predict individual G was computed using the method described in Table 1
nger forces during MVC production by the index and ring nger
(IR task). In this task G equals 0.61 (see row b), so that the pre- Task G
dicted and experimental values of total force are identical (circled
numbers). Other experimental values are provided for comparison. I 1
See text for further details M 1
R 1
Fingers F1 FM FR FL FTotal L 1
Task IM 0.61
9
> IR 0.61

Exp. Data
I 49.1 10.5 5.5 2.7 67.8 >
= IL 0.61
R 9.4 16.5 29.9 10.6 66.4
> MR 0.61
>
; ML 0.6
IR 37.0 14.5 21.6 9.2 82.3
.............................................................................................9 RL 0.64
a I+R 58.5 27.0 35.4 13.3 134.2 > > IMR 0.45
=

Model
IML 0.43
>
> IRL 0.44
; MRL 0.45
b 0.61 35.9 16.6 21.7 8.2 82.3
(I+R) IMRL 0.38

Fig. 2. The mode approach and force decit. Modes do not sum up
linearly. In order to account for the force-decit eect, we propose
that when several modes are simultaneously activated, their combined
eect is altered by a gain factor G < 1 Fig. 3. Value of the gain factor (G) as a function of the number of
modes simultaneously activated (N). For two- and three-nger tasks
(N 2 or 3), errors bars indicate the standard deviation of G across
Table 2 shows that the G values obtained for each the tasks; note the small size of the error bars. Curve-tting techniques
task. When the number of ngers explicitly involved in show that the equation G 1=N 0:712 can account rather well for the
drop in G as N increases
a task increased, the value of G decreased. By contrast,
G values were similar across tasks with the same
condition and subject group to provide an optimal t to
numbers of explicitly involved ngers. For two-nger
the data.
tasks G ranged from 0.61 to 0.64, for three-nger tasks
It is fair to admit that other functions could account
it ranged from 0.43 to 0.45, and for the four-nger
similarly well for the basic relation between G and N .
task it was 0.38. Obviously, for one-nger tasks G was
For instance, the following function could be used:
equal to 1.
Figure 3 shows the relation between the value of G 1
(averaged across tasks with the same number of explic- G p  a
N
itly involved ngers, N ) and N . To capture the depen-
dence of G on N , we tted the data with a function where N is the number of explicitly involved ngers. The
G 1=N y using the least-squares algorithm in MAT- square root of N can be interpreted as the length of a
LAB (MathWorks, MathWorks, Natick, Mass.). The mode vector, M MI ; MM ; MR ; ML , where MI ; MM ; MR ,
best t was obtained with the exponent y 0:712, ac- and ML are either 1 or 0. The length of this vector is 1.41,
counting for 99.9% of the variance. Based on this result 1.73, and 2 for N 2; 3, and 4, respectively. Then,
we have decided to introduce a function G 1=N y in normalizing the length of the mode vector leads to G
the model, where y is dened for each experimental factors of 0.71, 0.58, and 0.5. These values are higher
94

than the experimental ones (0.61, 0.44, and 0.38) by a one of the eleven multinger tasks. There is a signicant
nearly constant value (a  0:12). This formalization, strong correlation (R 0:978, p < 0:001) between the
similarly to 1=N y , also has one free parameter (a), which predicted and actual data. The model is able to account
can be subject and task dependent. for 95.3% of the variance. On average, individual nger
forces are predicted with an absolute error of 2:2N .
A similar analysis was performed to quantify the
3 Comparison to the published data performance of the model at the level of the force-
sharing pattern. During each task, each nger produced
The model is tested with ve data sets, all of them a certain percentage of the total force produced by all
originating from our group. There are also studies by involved ngers. The comparison between the predicted
other groups reporting individual nger forces during and experimental shares proved to be highly signicant
multi-nger task (e.g., Ohtsuki 1981; Kinoshita et al. (R 0:978, p < 0:001). On average, individual nger
1996; Santello and Soechting 2000). However, given that shares are predicted with an absolute error of 2.7%.
data relative to single-nger tasks are missing or Note that, because the gain factor G aects all nger
incomplete, modes cannot be assessed. Note also that, forces in a similar way, the force-sharing pattern (ex-
in most of those studies, subjects performed a grasping pressed as percentages of total force) predicted by the
task, and the mechanical constraints necessary to stabi- model is independent of G.
lize the handheld object are not taken into account by the
present model. Among our own studies, data sets were
selected bearing two intentions in mind: (i) we wanted to 3.2 Danion et al. (2000a)
test the model in a wide spectrum of experimental
conditions, and (ii) we gave priority to data sets that were This study investigated the eects of fatigue on multi-
explicitly reported in the original papers. nger force production. Young healthy subjects (n 14)
generated force against loops positioned either at the
middle of the distal phalanges (distal site), or at the
3.1 Zatsiorsky et al. (1998)
middle of the proximal phalanges (proximal site). All
subjects were right-handed and performed the tests with
In this study, young healthy subjects (n 10) pressed
the dominant hand. Only single- and four-nger tasks
with the ngertips of the dominant hand on sensors
were tested (i.e., I, M, R, L, and IMLR). These tasks
placed on a horizontal surface. All subjects were right-
were performed before and after a fatiguing exercise
handed. All 15 nger combinations were tested (i.e., I,
consisting of 60 s at 100% of MVC with all four ngers
M, R, L, IM, IR, IL, MR, ML, RL, IMR, IML, IRL,
acting together. Modes were dened based on data
MRL, and IMRL). Modes were dened based on data
averaged across subjects in the single-nger tasks. The
averaged across subjects in single-nger tasks. The
quality of the t was assessed by comparing the
function G 1=N y was used to account for the force-
predicted nger forces during the four-nger tasks with
decit eect when several modes were simultaneously
the actual data. Optimal G values were obtained
activated. As demonstrated earlier, y 0:712 is the
separately for the two sites of force application, and
optimal value for this dataset.
also before and after the fatiguing exercise.
Forty-four points are plotted in Fig. 4, with each
During force production at the distal site, the corre-
point corresponding to the force of a particular nger in
lation between the predicted and experimental forces
was signicant both before (R 0:995, p < 0:01), and
after (R 0:999, p < 0:001) the exercise. On average, the
absolute errors were 1:5N and 0:7N , respectively. The
value of G dropped during fatigue (0.60 vs 0.47).
During force production at the proximal site, the
correlation between the predicted and experimental
forces was just under the level of signicance before the
exercise (R 0:933, p 0:07), and not signicant after
the exercise (R 0:747, p > 0:1). On average, the abso-
lute errors were 3:3N and 2:7N , respectively. The value
of G dropped during fatigue (0.51 vs 0.41).

3.3 Danion et al. (2000b)

The goal of this study was to compare multi-nger force


production in young (n 7) and elderly (n 7) subjects
Fig. 4. Linear regression between the predicted and experimental
during MVC tasks at the distal and proximal sites. All
forces. The individual nger forces predicted by the mode approach subjects were right-handed and performed the tests with
are plotted as a function of the experimentally recorded nger forces the dominant hand. Only single- and four-nger tasks
reported by Zatsiorsky et al. (1998). See text for further details were used. The setup was the same as that used in
95

Danion et al. (2000a), and the quality of the t and the The value of G was stable across the two-nger tasks,
optimal G values were assessed in the same way as for with small dierences between the hands. For the
the earlier study. dominant hand, the value of G was 0.66 for the IM task
During force production at the distal site, the cor- and 0.65 for the RL task. For the nondominant hand,
relation between the actual and predicted forces was these values were 0.65 and 0.64, respectively. For the
signicant for both young (R 0:987, p < 0:05) and four-nger tasks, the value of G was 0.44 and 0.43 for
older adults (R 0:998, p < 0:01). On average, the the dominant and nondominant hands, respectively.
absolute errors were 1:4N and 0:6N, respectively. The Based on the G values, we computed the 12 individual
value of G was very similar for both subject groups nger forces related to the three multi-nger tasks. The
(0.592 vs 0.591). correlation between the predicted and experimental
During force production at the proximal site, the forces was highly signicant for both hands (R > 0:985,
correlation between the predicted and actual forces was p < 0:001). The average absolute errors for the
borderline signicant for the young adults (R 0:938, dominant and nondominant hands were 1:5N and 1:4N ,
p 0:06). However, this correlation was signicant for respectively.
the older adults (R 0:995, p < 0:01). On average, the
absolute errors were respectively 2:7N and 0:9N , re-
spectively. The value of G was again very similar for 4 Comparison with the neural network
both young and elderly subjects (0.518 vs 0.522). by Zatsiorsky et al. (1998)

A neural network (Zatsiorsky et al. 1998) has been


3.4 Li et al. (2000a) proposed to account for nger interaction during
maximal force production by one or several ngers. As
This study compared multi-nger force production by illustrated in Fig. 5, the network consists of three layers:
the right and left hands in a group of right-handed (i) the input layer that models a central neural drive, (ii)
subjects (n 13). Forces were produced by the nger- the hidden layer modeling the transformation of the
tips. Small wooden blocks shaped to t comfortably central drive into signals to the muscles serving several
under the subjects palms were placed underneath each ngers (multi-digit muscles), and (iii) the output layer
palm. In this experiment, all single-, two- and four- representing force production by individual ngers. The
nger tasks were tested. Data averaged across subjects output of the hidden layer is set inversely propor-
were used to test the model. For each hand, the quality tional to the number of ngers involved. In addition,
of the t and the optimal G values were dened using direct connections between the input and output layers
the same method as for the data of Zatsiorsky et al. represent signals to the hand muscles serving individual
(1998). ngers (unidigit muscles).
The value of G was rather stable across the two-nger Zatsiorsky et al. (1998) used a complete data set for
tasks. The average value of G was 0:62  0:04 (standard MVC tasks with all 15 nger combinations. They used
deviation across all six two-nger tasks) for the right the backpropagation-error algorithm to dene an opti-
hand, and 0:59  0:05 for the left hand. For the four- mal set of weights within the three-layer network
nger tasks, the value of G was 0.47 and 0.42 for the (Fig. 1). More formally, the model can be presented by
right and left hands, respectively. Based on the G values, the following equation:
we have computed the 28 individual nger forces related
to the seven multi-nger tasks. The correlation between
the predicted and experimental forces was highly sig-
nicant for both hands (R > 0:986, p < 0:001). On av-
erage, the absolute errors for the right and left hands
were 1:0N and 1:2N , respectively.

3.5 Li et al. (2000b)

In this study, an objective was to investigate possible


dierences between the dominant and nondominant
hands in a group composed of ve right-handers and
ve left-handers. Force production was generated at
the ngertips using the same setup as in Li et al.
(2000a). In this experiment, subjects performed all Fig. 5. The articial neural network proposed by Zatsiorsky et al.
single-nger tasks, two two-nger tasks (IM and RL), (1998). The network consists of three layers: (i) the input layer that
models a central neural drive, (ii) the hidden layer modeling the
and the four-nger task. The method used to t the transformation of the central drive into signals to the muscles serving
data was the same as for Li et al. (2000a). The model several ngers (multi-digit muscles), and (iii) the output layer
was adjusted separately for the dominant and non- representing force production by individual ngers. Figure adapted
dominant hands. with permission from Zatsiorsky et al. (1998)
96
0 1 0 1 0 1 2 3 0 1
FI 2 3 xI FI 49:1 14:0 9:4 7:7 MI
B C wI;I wI;M wI;R wI;L
B C BFMC 6 10:5 38:0 16:5 6:7 7 B M M C
BFM C B xM C B C 6 7 B C
B C 16 wM;I wM;M wM;R wM;L 7 7 B C B F R C 1 6 5:5 12:9 29:9 15:0 7 B M R C
B FR C 6 7 B xR C B C N 0:712 6 7 B C
B C N64 wR;I wR;M wR;R wR;L 5 B B C @ FL A 4 2:7 4:1 10:6 24:8 5 @ M L A
B C C
@ FL A @ xL A
wL;I wL;M wL;R wL;L
0 1 0 1 4
vI 0 0 0 xI
B C B C The performance of the neural network was tested by
B 0 vM 0 0 C B xM C comparing the 60 individual forces predicted by this
B C B C
B
B 0 0 vR 0C B C
C B xR C 1 model to the actual forces. On average, the neural
B C B C network provided a slightly better t to the data than the
@0 0 0 vL A @ xL A
mode approach, as indicated by the coecient of
correlation (R 0:989 vs 0.976) and the average error
(1:2N vs 2:2N ). There are, however, advantages to the
where N is the number of ngers involved in the task, Fi mode approach. First, this approach uses only a few
is the force of the ith nger (i = I, M, R, L), xi are the data sets (single-nger trials) to generate predictions for
inputs into the network, wi;j and vi are connection multi-nger tasks with only one free parameter (y), while
weights. The inputs to the network were set at xi 1 if the network uses data from both single- and multi-nger
the ith nger was explicitly recruited by the task, or tasks to optimize connection weights. Second, even
xi 0 otherwise. The rst term in the right-hand side of without this free parameter, the mode approach can
the equation represents the contribution to nger forces still generate predictions for the force-sharing pattern
mediated by the hidden layer, while the second term (expressed as percentages of the total force).
represents the action of direct projections from the input
layer.
Our mode approach can be formalized by the
5 Discussion
following equation:
0 1 2 3 0 1 The mode approach has been tested in a wide variety of
FI wI;I wI;M wI;R wI;L MI experimental conditions. The results show that in most
BFM C 6 wM;I wM;M wM;R wM;L 7 B C
B C 6 7 B MM C of these conditions, the approach has been rather
B F R C G6 wR;I wR;M wR;R wR;L 7 B MR C
B C 6 7 B C accurate. One notable exception is force production at
@ FL A 4 wL;I wL;M wL;R wL;L 5 @ M L A the proximal phalanges. We would like to oer two
mutually nonexclusive reasons for the relatively poor
2 performance of the model in this task. First, subjects
may feel clumsy during such tests, since everyday tasks
with the same abbreviations as for (1), plus G as the rarely involve force production at proximal phalanges.
scaling factor (G 1=N y and Mi as the input for each Second, we could never completely secure the positions
mode, with each mode being represented by a vector of the loops along the phalanges. Therefore the points of
column of the 4
4 matrix. The inputs were set at force application could dier across ngers and across
Mi 1 if the ith nger was explicitly recruited by the trials. During force production at the distal site, such
task, and Mi 0 otherwise. displacements have relatively small eects on the lever
Coecients in (1) were dened based on the data in arms with respect to proximal phalanges. In contrast,
all 15 nger tasks. In contrast, coecients in (2) were during force production at the proximal site the relative
dened based only on single-nger tasks. The optimal change in the lever arms can become more signicant,
values for the data set of Zatsiorsky et al. (1998) are leading to more noise in the data.
presented in (3) and (4):
0 1 2 3 0 1 5.1 A single framework for dierent phenomena
FI 32:7 9:1 3:8 3:7 xI
B C 6 7 B C
BFMC 6 14:2 29:0 13:6 3:5 7 B xM C The mode approach provides an attractive framework
B C 6 7 B C
B F R C 1 6 9:0 10:9 7 B C for the study of multi-nger interaction since it
B C N6 20:2 22:4 7 B xR C
B C 6 7 B C integrates in a simple way the phenomena of sharing,
@ FL A 4 8:8 7:6 16:0 17:2 5 @ xL A
decit, and enslaving. First, the mode approach
suggests that the sharing pattern of the total force in
2 3 0 1
16:9 0 0 0 xI multi-nger tasks results simply from a linear super-
6 7 B C position of central variables (i.e., modes). The dierent
6 0 10:1 0 0 7 B xM C shares of ngers during multi-nger MVC tasks (cf. Li
6 7 B C
6
6 0 0 8:3 0 7 B C
7 B xR C 3 et al. 1998a) result from the dierent enslaving pat-
6 7 B C terns during single-nger tasks. Second, this approach
4 0 0 0 7:2 5 @ xL A
proposes a simple relationship accounting for the force
decit observed during multi-nger tasks. Indeed, the
97

gain factor G depends only on the number of modes N This idea was partly introduced in Fig. 2 of Zatsior-
simultaneously activated (this N dependence being sky et al. (2000), showing parallel changes in the
captured by a decreasing function). More specically, slave- and master-nger forces during a force ramp.
we found that, for a given value of N , the value of G The gain factor in (4) applies to all components of the
does not depend on the exact set of ngers involved in matrix and, as such, leads to proportional force-decit
a task. eects in the forces produced by master- and slave-
Third, the enslaving eect is captured in the settings ngers.
of the modes themselves. The numbers accounting ex-
plicitly for the enslaving are the 12 o-diagonal terms in
the 4
4 matrix in (4).
5.4 Implications for the minimization of secondary
More specically, the mode approach emphasizes
moments
that the enslaving and force-sharing phenomena are in-
timately related. For instance, a change in a single co-
It has previously been proposed that force sharing
ecient in the 4
4 matrix in (4) is accompanied by
patterns emerge because the CNS tries to minimize
changes in both sharing and enslaving. In contrast, the
the total moment produced by all ngers about the
model suggests that force decit and enslaving are phe-
longitudinal functional axis of the hand (principle of
nomena of dierent origins. Indeed, changing the value
minimization of secondary moments; Li et al. 1998a,b).
of the exponent y has no eect on force sharing and
It has also been shown that during one-, two-, or three-
enslaving, if both are expressed as percentages of the
nger tasks, enslaving helps reduce the secondary
total force. This result is supported by published data.
moment (Zatsiorsky et al. 2000).
For instance, it has been shown that during fatigue,
The mode approach suggests that minimization of
force decit increases (leading to smaller values of G)
secondary moments can follow from the force-sharing
while enslaving tends to decrease (see Danion et al.
patterns during single-nger tasks. Are modes the cause
2000a). Similarly, in elderly people there are relatively
or the consequence of secondary-moment minimiza-
small changes in the force decit (and in G), and rela-
tion? We suggest that modes are developed based on
tively large and signicant changes in the enslaving
everyday motor tasks involving the hand. Many ev-
(Danion et al. 2000b).
eryday tasks involve stabilizing a gripped object, which
requires balancing nger moments with respect to the
thumb. If a person is asked to select a comfortable
5.2 Occlusion of enslaving thumb position for a grip task, this position commonly
coincides with the functional axis of the hand/forearm
Studies by Zatsiorsky et al. (1998, 2000) have shown (Li et al. 1998b). Extensive practice of such tasks may
that enslaving eects are nonadditive. Moreover, in be expected to lead ultimately to sharing patterns that
certain cases, the enslaving eects induced by several comply with the principle of minimization of secondary
explicitly activated ngers could be smaller than the moments.
enslaving when only one nger was explicitly recruited
(this eect has been termed occlusion of enslaving). One
can nd such examples in Table 1 of Zatsiorsky et al.
(1998), where data averaged across subjects are report- 5.5 Implications for the uncontrolled manifold hypothesis
ed. For example, during a single-nger MVC task by the
R nger, the enslaved force produced by the L nger is The notion of modes has been recently introduced in a
10:6N , whereas during a three-nger MVC task (IMR) series of studies investigating the structure of force
the L nger force is only 5:1N . Equation (4) also variability during multi-nger tasks (Latash et al. 2001,
predicts a smaller enslaving force (8N ) for the L nger 2002; Scholz et al. 2002). The analysis in these studies
during the IMR task based on the mode approach. was based upon a hypothesis that the CNS controls the
Thus, this approach can account for relatively subtle ngers using a set of central variables, or modes. The
eects such as the occlusion of enslaving, although introduction of modes in those studies was done mostly
quantitatively the prediction diers somewhat from the for computational purposes. The present study suggests
experimental results. that the notion of modes is applicable across multi-nger
force-production tasks and can be successfully applied
to studies of dierent subject subpopulations and the
eects of fatigue.
5.3 Force decit in enslaved ngers

In previous studies, the phenomenon of force decit


has been always emphasized for the master ngers 5.6 Generality of the mode approach
(i.e., ngers explicitly required to produce force). The
current approach views force decit as being equally In the present study we have shown that nger
applicable to both master and slave ngers. The interaction can be captured by a matrix (representing
matrix in (4) implies that forces produced by slave the modes), a vector (dening which modes are recruit-
ngers remain proportional to master-nger forces. ed), and a gain factor (accounting for force decit). A
98

question emerges: how general are the matrix and the Gottlieb GL, Corcos DM, Agarwal GC (1989) Strategies for the
gain factor? First, there are obviously uctuations in control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of
both across subjects. Besides dierences in muscle freedom. Behav Brain Sci 12: 189250
Hogan N (1984) An organizational principle for a class of volun-
strength, some subjects have a larger force decit and/ tary movements. J Neurosci 4: 27452754
or enslaving. Second, the matrix and the gain factor Kilbreath SL, Gandevia SC (1994) Limited independent exion of
depend on the experimental conditions. For instance, in the thumb and ngers in human subjects. J Physiol (Lond) 479:
young healthy subjects, the magnitude of force decit 487497
depends on the setup used for force production, and on Kinoshita H, Murase T, Bandou T (1996) Grip posture and forces
the site of force production (i.e., distal or proximal during holding cylindrical objects with circular grips. Ergo-
nomics 39: 11631176
phalanges). Therefore, the main contribution of this Latash ML (1993) Control of human movement. Human Kinetics,
work is not to propose a set of magic numbers that could Campaign, Ill.
account for nger interaction in all subjects and all Latash ML, Gelfand IM, Li Z-M, Zatsiorsky VM (1998b) Changes
tasks, but rather to introduce a specic framework that in the force-sharing pattern induced by modications of visual
facilitates analysis of nger forces during both single- feedback during force production by a set of ngers. Exp Brain
Res 123: 255262
and multi-nger force-production tasks.
Latash ML, Li Z-M, Zatsiorsky VM (1998a) A principle of
At a more general level, the main contribution of the error compensation studied within a task of force produc-
force mode approach is to provide an example of how tion by a redundant set of ngers. Exp Brain Res 122:
analysis of motor control problems can be performed 131138
using a set of hypothetical independent central control Latash ML, Scholz J, Danion F, Schoner G (2001) Structure of
variables computed based on a set of performance vari- motor variability in marginally redundant multi-nger force
production tasks. Exp Brain Res 141: 153165
ables (forces) measured in a few simple motor tasks. We
Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schoner G (2002) Motor control strategies
hope that motor control studies that use kinematic and revealed in the structure of motor variability. Exerc Sport Sci
kinetic variables recorded during various motor tasks Rev 30: 2631
will follow this example and perform analyses of control Li S, Danion F, Latash ML, Li Z-M, Zatsiorsky VM (2000a)
processes using adequate sets of central control variables Finger coordination in multi-nger force production tasks in-
that are dierent from the performance ones. Such a volving ngers of the right and/or the left hand. J Appl Bio-
mech 16: 379391
generalization of the force mode approach is far from
Li S, Danion F, Latash ML, Li Z-M, Zatsiorsky VM (2000b)
trivial and will probably require major eort. Characteristics of nger force production during one- and two-
hand tasks. Hum Mov Sci 19: 897923
Li Z-M, Latash ML, Newell KM, Zatsiorsky VM (1998a) Motor
References redundancy during maximal voluntary contraction in four-
nger tasks. Exp Brain Res 122: 7178
Atkeson CG (1989) Learning arm kinematics and dynamics. Annu Li Z-M, Latash ML, Zatsiorsky VM (1998b) Force sharing among
Rev Neurosci 12: 157183 ngers as a model of the redundancy problem. Exp Brain Res
Bernstein NA (1935) The problem of interrelation between coor- 119: 276286
dination and localization (in Russian). Arch Biol Sci 38: 135 Ohtsuki T (1981) Inhibition of individual ngers during grip
[an English translation is available: Zatsiorsky VM (2001) In: strength exertion. Ergonomics 24: 2136
Latash ML (ed) Classics in movement science. Human Kinet- Rosenbaum DA, Loukopoulos LD, Meulenbroek RGM, Vaughan
ics, Champaign, Ill.] J, Engelbrecht SE (1995) Planning reaches by evaluating stored
Danion F, Latash ML, Li Z-M, Zatsiorsky VM (2000a) The eect postures. Psychol Rev 102: 2867
of fatigue on multi-nger co-ordination in force production Santello M, Soechting JF (2000) Force synergies for multi-ngered
tasks in humans. J Physiol (Lond) 523: 523532 grasping. Exp Brain Res 133: 457467
Danion F, Latash ML, Zatsiorsky VM (2000b) The eect of aging Schieber MH (1991) Individuated movements of rhesus monkey:
on multi-nger force production. In: Proceedings of the 24th means of quantifying the independence of the digits. J Neuro-
Annual Meeting of the American Society of Biomechanics, physiol 65: 13811391
Chicago, Ill., 1922 July, pp 8788 Scholz J, Danion F, Latash ML, Schoner G (2002) Understanding
Feldman AG (1986) Once more on the equilibrium-point hypoth- nger coordination through analysis of the structure of force
esis (k model) for motor control. J Mot Behav 18: 1754 variability. Biol Cybern 86: 2939
Feldman AG, Levin MF (1995) The origin and use of positional Uno Y, Kawato M, Suzuki R (1989) Formation and control of
frames of reference in motor control. Behav Brain Sci 18: 723 optimal trajectory in human multi-joint arm movement. Min-
804 imum torque-change model. Biol Cybern 61: 89101
Gelfand IM, Tsetlin ML (1966) On mathematical modeling of the Zatsiorsky VM, Li Z-M, Latash ML (1998) Coordinated force
mechanisms of the central nervous system (in Russian). In: production in multi-nger tasks: nger interaction and neural
Gelfand IM, Gurnkel VS, Fomin SV, Tsetlin ML (eds) network modeling. Biol Cybern 79: 139150
Models of the structuralfunctional organization of certain Zatsiorsky VM, Li Z-M, Latash ML (2000) Enslaving eects in
biological systems. Nauka, Moscow, pp 926 [an English multi-nger force production. Exp Brain Res 131: 187195
translation is available: 1971 edn, MIT Press, Cambridge,
Mass.]

You might also like