You are on page 1of 4

Published Online: July 3, 2012

COMMENTARY

Timed Tests and the Development of Math Anxiety

By Jo Boaler

Premium article access courtesy of Edweek.org.

Read more FREE content!

Printer-Friendly

Email Article

Reprints

Comments

Like

Mathematics education is in crisis: A third of all schoolchildren end up in remedial


math courses, and the level of interest in the subject is at an all-time low. This is a result, in
part, of schools in the United States heading down a fast-moving track in which the purpose
of math has been reduced to the ranking of children and their schools. Math has become a
performance subject. Children of all ages are more likely to tell you that the reason for
learning math is to show whether they get it instead of whether they appreciate the
beauty of the subject or the way it piques their interest. The damage starts early in this
country, with school districts requiring young children to take timed math tests from the age
of 5. This is despite research that has shown that timed tests are the direct cause of the
early onset of math anxiety.

Timed math tests have been popular in the United States for years. Unfortunately, some of
the wording in the Common Core State Standards may point to an increased use of timed
tests. From the 2nd grade on, the common standards give math fluency as a goal. Many
test writers, teachers, and administrators erroneously equate fluency with timed testing.
It is critical that we take a moment to review the emerging evidence on the impact of timed
testing and the ways in which it transforms childrens brains, leading to an inevitable path of
math anxiety and low math achievement.

The personal and educational consequences of math anxiety are great. Math anxiety affects
about 50 percent of the U.S. population and more women than men. Researchers know
that math anxiety starts early. They have documented it in students as young as 5, and that
early anxiety snowballs, leading to math difficulties and avoidance that only get worse as
children get older. Researchers also know that it is not related to overall intelligence.

Until recently, we have not known the causes of math anxiety and how it affects the brain,
but the introduction of brain-imaging research has given us new and important evidence.
Sian Beilock, an associate professor of psychology at the University of Chicago, for
example, has found that when children are put under math stress, they are unable to
execute math problems successfully. The stress impedes their working memorythe area of
the brain where we hold math facts. Beilock found that stressful math situations cause
worries that compete for the working memory, causing it to be blocked. She also found that
math anxiety has an impact on those with high, rather than low amounts of working
memorythe very students who have the potential to take mathematics to higher levels.

In Beilocks recent research conducted with children in 1st and 2nd grade, she found that
levels of math anxiety did not correlate with grade level, reading level, or parental income.
For the most capable students, the research confirms, stress impedes the functioning of
their working memory and reduces achievement. Research conducted at Stanford
revealed that math anxiety changes the structure and workings of the brain.

When I moved to the United States from Europe a few years ago, I was shocked to learn
that many school districts give children, as early as 1st grade, 50 math problems to solve in
three minutes. For many students, it is not an exaggeration to describe this experience as
torturous. When teachers of 2nd and 4th graders in one elementary school I visited asked
students to write down how the test made them feel, responses showed that the test
prompted anxiety in one-quarter of the students in each class, but that anxiety was not
correlated with test success. Indeed, some of the students with the highest levels of success
were those who indicated the greatest anxiety and made comments such as I feel nervous.
I know my facts, but this just scares me.
It should not come as a surprise that the highest achievers displayed the greatest anxiety;
in fact, neuroscience tells us that these students experience the greatest degree of cognitive
dysfunction. But this anxiety does not only affect high-achieving students. Second graders
from across the achievement spectrum described the tests as making them feel upset and
unhappy and that they are terrible at math.

"It is critical that we take a moment to review the emerging evidence on the
impact of timed testing and the ways in which it transforms childrens brains."

Timed tests have been given to young children in school districts in the United States with
the best intentions, but with negative consequences for many years. The brain research that
has emerged recently could be the impetus for shifting the momentum. But the inclusion of
the word fluency in the common standards may mean that educators will continue to use
these tests, and that they will even be included as part of the new common-core
assessments.

There are many good teaching strategies for encouraging fluency in math, but the ones that
are effective are those that simultaneously develop number sensethe flexible use and
understanding of numbers and quantitieswithout instilling fear and anxiety. Strategies that
involve reasoning about numbers and operations, such as the pedagogical approach called
number talks, are ideal for developing fluency with understanding.

Beyond the fear and anxiety, timed tests also convey strong and negative messages about
math, suggesting that math ability is measured by working quickly, rather than thinking
deeply and carefullythe hallmark of high-level mathematical thinking. The ideas students
develop about math in elementary school are critical for their future in the subject.

Policies in education rarely draw from research knowledge. But I would argue that this
particular policyof giving young children timed math testsis one of the clearest ways
schools damage children, and we now have evidence of the extent of the damage.

The United States faces a serious problem with widespread underachievement in


mathematics, and insufficient numbers of students available to continue mathematical,
scientific, and technological innovations. Educators and policymakers share an important
goal: to create math classrooms where students are excited to learn the subject, rather
than being stressed and worried about their performance under pressure. There is no
disagreement about the goal, but policies that require the testing of young children under
timed conditions may be inadvertently achieving the opposite. Assessments for the common
core could break or perpetuate this cycle of damage. Lets hope they do the former.

Jo Boaler is a professor of mathematics education at Stanford University and the author of


Whats Math Got To Do With It? How Parents and Teachers Can Help Children Learn to Love
Their Least Favorite Subject. (Penguin, 2009).

You might also like