You are on page 1of 10

Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol.

110 (3rd series)


Neurological Rehabilitation
M.P. Barnes and D.C. Good, Editors
# 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved

Chapter 27

Rehabilitation of aphasia
ANNA BASSO 1*, MARGARET FORBES 2, AND FRANCOIS BOLLER 3
1
Department of Neuropsychology, Institute of Neurological Sciences, Milan University, Milan, Italy
2
Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
3
Bethesda, MD, USA

INTRODUCTION approach (functional reorganization), the pragmatic ap-


proach, as well as the neurolinguistic approach. The sub-
The term aphasia refers to the more or less complete
sequent section illustrates some of the major current
loss of the ability to use language as the result of lesions
approaches to aphasia rehabilitation, specifically the syn-
in cerebral areas generally localized in the left half of
dromic approach (also called neoassociationist), the
the brain; these areas are responsible for the ability to
cognitive neuropsychological approach, and the social ap-
speak, understand, read, and write. Because language
proach. The chapter then provides examples of specific
is complex, the term aphasia covers heterogeneous
methods. While all intervention strategies may be classi-
disorders that may have little in common. Indeed, it
fied, more or less correctly, into one or another of the
can be argued that the ways of being aphasic are prac-
above categories, it is not possible to mention the hun-
tically infinite. Besides varying from one person to an-
dreds of specific interventions to be found in the litera-
other, aphasia is far from static in any given patient and
ture, some of which have been described only briefly
is susceptible to changes, both spontaneously and as a
and in reference to a single case. The chapter concludes
result of rehabilitation.
with a review of efficacy studies on aphasia therapy.
Unfortunately, the definition of aphasia does not say
much about characteristics of the loss: whether, for in-
EPIDEMIOLOGY
stance, language is impaired in all of its aspects, or
whether there is a selective loss of one or more processes No specific data on the incidence of aphasia are available
(such as auditory comprehension or reading); or whether but an approximate count can be inferred from the inci-
aphasia is a single condition, or takes several forms that dence of stroke. This varies from 1.8/1000 (Di Carlo et al.,
may selectively impair one linguistic component (pho- 2003) to 4.5/1000 (Wade et al., 1986) new cases per year;
nology, syntax, semantics), leaving the others relatively overall prevalence is 65/1000 individuals (ILSAWG,
unimpaired. Even more ambiguous is the word rehabil- 1997). With reference to the Italian population (approxi-
itation, which is supposed to cover all the different treat- mately 58 million), these figures indicate an incidence of
ments intended to improve impaired function. The only 105 000 to 261 000 new cases per year and a prevalence of
thing common to all aphasia treatments is that patient 3.8 million individuals affected by stroke. For the USA,
and therapist try to communicate with each other. The the annual incidence is thought to be around 600 000
content of the communication and what the therapist and the prevalence up to 4.6 million people. There is ev-
does in order to elicit a response from the patient vary idence suggesting that the incidence of stroke has de-
from place to place and from clinician to clinician. creased over the past 50 years (Caradang et al., 2006).
Following a brief overview of the epidemiology and The incidence of aphasia following stroke varies from
the clinical characteristics of aphasia, this chapter will 21% in some studies (Brust et al., 1976) to 38% in others
present the major traditional approaches to rehabilitation. (Pedersen et al., 1995). For the Italian population, these
They include the stimulation approach (also called data indicate that incidence ranges from 22 000 (21%
classic), the behavior modification approach, Lurias of 1.8/1000) to 99 000 (38% of 4.5/1000) new cases of

*Correspondence to: Anna Basso, Department of Neuropsychology, Institute of Neurological Sciences, Milan University,
Milan, Italy. E-mail: anna.basso@gmail.com
326 A. BASSO ET AL.
aphasia per year. Kirshner and Jacobs (2009) note that re- school maintained that language is a complex, holistic,
ports of incidence in the USA generally omit aphasia and indivisible function, not represented in the brain by
caused by conditions other than stroke, but estimate that a number of discrete centers, but a property of the total
approximately 170 000 new cases per year result from brain. The two principles of the stimulation approach are
stroke. Pedersen et al. (1995) compared the incidence the idea that aphasia is a unitary disorder varying in
of aphasia in acute and chronic cases (6 months) and severity but not in type, and the assumption that knowl-
found a reduction ranging from 38% in the acute phase edge about language is not lost but cannot be accessed
to 18% in the chronic phase. because of cerebral damage. Deriving directly from these
In addition to vascular etiology, traumatic brain inju- two principles is a global approach to treatment that var-
ries due to traffic accidents, war, or other events also ies only according to the severity of the disorder and that
cause a substantial number of brain injuries requiring spe- is based on the automatic-voluntary dissociation. The
cialized rehabilitation. Other etiologies of aphasia include required response is facilitated and automatically
gunshot wounds, encephalitis, and anoxia, although they obtained; facilitation is then progressively reduced until
account for a smaller number of cases. If no other med- the response is intentional. Wepman (1951), Schuell
ical complications arise, the language of people with (Schuell et al., 1964), and Darley (1982) are among the
aphasia resulting from these causes is expected to recover most important representatives of this school.
to some extent. On the other hand, a number of conditions
such as CNS tumors and neurodegenerative diseases Behavior modification approach
(Alzheimer disease and especially primary progressive
aphasia) also cause language disturbances, but because This approach stems from the application to aphasia
these diseases are progressive, further language decline therapy of the principles of operant conditioning.
is expected rather than recovery, and therefore the In the 1960s psychologists were quite interested in learn-
methods and goals of language treatment are quite dif- ing and some researchers investigated whether learning
ferent. For this reason, language problems due to these in aphasic subjects followed the same rules as learning
etiologies will not be discussed in this chapter. in normal subjects. They concluded that the majority of
aphasic individuals are able to learn new materials using
OVERVIEW OF THE HISTORYOF the same strategies as normal subjects, but learning is less
APHASIA THERAPY efficient (Tikofsky and Reynolds, 1962, 1963; Edwards,
1965; Brookshire, 1971). Principles of operant condition-
Examples of treatment for aphasia can be found, mainly ing and programmed instruction were then put to use in
in German speaking countries, after World War I, but one aphasia therapy. The two most important techniques used
of the first systematic studies can be attributed to the sem- in programed instruction are shaping and fading and they
inal work of Weisenburg and McBride (1935). Aphasia both assume that the required behavior or a similar one
therapy became common practice only after World War exists in the patients repertoire of responses.
II. At that time knowledge about aphasia was rather
scarce and treatment was not informed by a clear-cut the- LURIAS FUNCTIONAL APPROACH
ory. Very different methods were put into practice but it is
possible to recognize some common threads, and the best- This is a good example of unity of theory and practice and
known approaches will be very briefly mentioned. Any of a coherent therapeutic system. Luria (1963, 1970; Luria
taxonomy one proposes, however, will be an oversimpli- et al., 1969) distinguished functional disturbances due to
fication, and the different categories tend to overlap. the temporary loss of activity in some brain areas that can
The stimulation approach, the behavior modification rapidly resolve by themselves and do not require treat-
approach, the pragmatic approach, and the neurolinguis- ment from the functional disturbances resulting from
tic approach have more or less succeeded one another. In the irreversible destruction of brain tissue. The damaged
contrast, Lurias functional approach was developed in a function can never be restored to its previous form and
different country and in a different culture. These ap- therapy must be directed towards the reorganization of
proaches will be briefly discussed. The syndromic (or the function by transferring it to other brain structures
neo-associationist) and the cognitive approaches, still or functional systems. The patient must be taught to per-
very popular, will be described in more detail, together form the damaged operation through new roundabout
with the social (or consequences-based) approach. methods by means of a partially new neural organization.

Stimulation approach PRAGMATIC APPROACH


A large number of very heterogeneous interventions The pragmatic approach has evolved from the applica-
are generally grouped under this label. Followers of this tion of linguistic knowledge to aphasia therapy. In the
REHABILITATION OF APHASIA 327
stimulation approach the patient was required to use lan- similarly is highly heterogeneous. The neoassociationist
guage in standardized situations, such as confrontation school flourished in the 1960s and 1970s in Boston with a
naming and picture description. With the appearance of group of neurologists and psychologists (particularly
pragmatics aphasia therapists broadened their view of Norman Geschwind, Harold Goodglass, Frank Benson,
successful responses in therapy, and began to take an in- Edith Kaplan, and others) who have had a lasting and
terest in patients capacity to communicate through any important influence on clinical studies in aphasia. The
channel, not only through spoken language. A number of classic aphasia syndromes (Broca, Wernicke, conduc-
researchers demonstrated that aphasic patients capacity tion, transcortical, anomic, global) were redescribed
to communicate is better preserved than their capacity to and reanalyzed in terms of more sophisticated linguistic
express themselves through language (Wilcox et al., analyses and anatomical knowledge.
1978; Kadzielawa et al., 1981; Foldi et al., 1983) and a va- One of the most recent detailed descriptions of the
riety of formal functional evaluations were developed classic syndromes is that of Benson and Ardila (1996),
(Sarno, 1969; Holland, 1980). The best-known therapeu- who define a syndrome as a cluster of symptoms plus
tic implementation is Promoting Aphasics Communica- a specified brain lesion. They argue that although
tive Effectiveness (PACE) (Davis and Wilcox, 1985). limited and imperfect, the syndrome classification
originally developed by the nineteenth-century continen-
NEUROLINGUISTIC APPROACH tal investigators remains basically accurate, replic-
able, and clinically useful (Benson and Ardila, 1996,
Neurolinguistics is the branch of linguistics that analyzes pp. 111112).
the language impairments that follow brain damage in However, according to Albert et al. (1981) and Prins
terms of the principles of language structure. The term et al. (1978), only approximately 20% of chronic aphasic
neurolinguistic is neutral about the linguistic theory it subjects can be reliably classified in one of the classic
refers to, but any linguistically based approach to apha- syndromes. Poeck, considering the problem of aphasic
sia therapy is based on the principle that language has an syndromes, argued that they are to a large extent, arte-
internal organization that can be described by a system facts produced by the vascularization of the language
of rules. The neurolinguistic approach stresses the role area and not natural combinations of symptoms that
of language in aphasia and analyzes it according to prin- necessarily co-occur (Poeck, 1983, p. 84). According
ciples of theoretical linguistics. to Schwartz, aphasia syndromes are not real entities
The first linguistically based typology of aphasic im- because one cannot delineate for each category an
pairments is probably that of Roman Jakobson (1964), essence or idealized pattern which is invariant and
although Alajouanine and colleagues (1939, 1964) had al- hence shared by all members of the group (Schwartz,
ready stressed the role of some linguistic phenomena in 1984, p. 5).
aphasia. Many authors have underlined the importance Whatever the nature of the classic syndromes, they
of linguistic theory for aphasia therapy (Hatfield, are still very popular among clinicians and have been
1972; MacMahon, 1972; Hatfield and Shewell, 1983; the starting point for treatment for many years. A num-
Lesser, 1989; Miller, 1989), but linguistic analyses were ber of aphasia batteries, e.g., Boston Diagnostic Aphasia
not carried out in great detail until interest in aphasia Examination (BDAE) (Goodglass and Kaplan, 1983),
expanded beyond the field of neurology to disciplines Revised Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) (Kertesz,
such as linguistics, speechlanguage pathology, and 2007), and Porch Index of Communicative Ability (PICA)
psychology. (Porch, 1967), allow subject classification, and the syndro-
mic diagnosis has been considered a valid theoretical basis
PRESENT APPROACHES for treatment. A person with global aphasia, for instance,
All the previously mentioned approaches have contrib- has severely damaged comprehension (as well as all other
uted in a more or less direct way to the present situation verbal behaviors) and treatment of comprehension is ini-
in aphasia treatment in which it is possible to identify tiated; a conduction aphasic person has disproportionately
three very different approaches to therapy: the neoasso- impaired repetition, and treatment for repetition is initi-
ciationist or syndromic approach, the cognitive neuro- ated; an anomic person has numerous anomias and treat-
psychological or impairment-based approach, and the ment for naming disorders is initiated. The relationship
social or consequences-based approach. between the symptom and the treatment is, however,
loose, and many different treatments for the same
impaired behavior have been suggested.
Neoassociationist or syndromic approach
To illustrate, all of the following treatments for
The neoassociationist approach (also called syndromic) naming disorders can be grouped under the umbrella
derives directly from the stimulation school and of the syndromic approach: semantic features analysis
328 A. BASSO ET AL.
(Kiran and Thompson, 2003); combined presentation of each other is more than the linguistic content of the sen-
the oral and written word to be named (errorless learn- tences themselves.
ing) or phonological and orthographic cues (errorful Treatment starts from an analysis of how conversation
learning) (Fillingham et al., 2006); delayed copy and re- evolves between two normal interlocutors and utilizes the
call (CART) plus repetition or repetition only (Beeson rules of conversation in order to build up a normal con-
and Egnor, 2006); semantic therapy designed to activate versation with an aphasic individual. The aim of this treat-
the semantic network and minimize errors (production ment is to train subjects with aphasia to use their residual
was discouraged) (Davis et al., 2006); and repetition communicative capacity in the real world and to restore
combined with four yes/no questions about the seman- their capacity to maintain a real conversation. This type
tics or phonology of the word (Raymer et al., 2007). It of therapy does not start from the symptoms, but from
also encompasses novel approaches such as melodic in- the end-point of treatment and applies linguistic knowl-
tonation therapy (MIT), which aims to harness the intact edge about natural conversation (Basso, 2010).
musical skills of the right hemisphere to enable commu-
nication (Albert et al., 1973).
The cognitive neuropsychological or
The underlying principles are not very different from
impairment-based approach
those underlying the stimulation approach. The basic
idea is that although language cannot be accessed, it is The classic anatomo-clinical approach provided knowl-
not lost. Hence target responses are elicited by facilitat- edge about the relationships between lesions and func-
ing the patients production by any possible means. tions, but in spite of the accumulation of knowledge,
Aphasia is no longer considered a unitary disorder, but the problem of inferring the structure of normal brain
the linguistic analysis of verbal behaviors (reading, writ- functions from behavioral dysfunctions was far from
ing, comprehension, production) is still rather imprecise. being resolved and some researchers were dissatisfied
Many clinicians consider that the difficulty of a lan- with the methodology of clinical neuropsychology. Car-
guage task is based solely on a hierarchy of linguistic amazza and McCloskey, for instance, wrote, It is not an
units: phonemes, morphemes, words, and sentences. exaggeration to say that over one hundred years of re-
Compared to the stimulation approach, exercises are search on cognitive disorders has shed little light on
more varied and tailored to the various syndromes; the nature of normal cognitive processes and the form
therapists are more concerned with the formal aspects of their dissolution in conditions of brain damage
of therapy (which could be a legacy of the behavior (Caramazza and McCloskey, 1988, p. 519).
modification approach) and the question of the efficacy In recent years, researchers have become less inter-
of rehabilitation is a central topic of research. ested in the localization of cognitive functions in the
One limitation of this approach is that treatment ad- brain and more concerned about the nature of the cogni-
dresses the superficial symptoms without trying to iden- tive mechanisms. The basic aim of cognitive neuropsy-
tify the underlying cause of the impaired behavior. As a chology was to provide a model of normal cognitive
consequence it is not possible to establish whether a par- processing and to explain impaired performance in
ticular treatment is better or worse than a different one terms of damage to one or more components of the nor-
and, even if it has been demonstrated that the treatment mal cognitive function. Cognitive neuropsychologists
has been effective, it is difficult to understand why and argued for a functional approach to the study of the
therefore to rationally suggest it for other subjects. How- mind explicitly independent of the study of the brain,
ard and Hatfield (1987) argue, Too often. . .the relation- and introduced the use of information-processing
ship between deficit and treatment is based on some models, which provide a rational basis for the character-
implicit idea of how treatment has its effects, which ization of patterns of impaired performance in terms of
has no good justification or scientific support (p. 106). damaged subcomponents. They were interested in iden-
A global treatment that can be subsumed under the tifying the functional locus of the damage that caused
heading of syndromic approach that avoids this criticism the symptoms by analyzing the patients performance
is treatment based on the analysis of conversation. Con- of various tasks in relation to a model of normal proces-
versation is the prototype of language use and the form sing. The use of pathological data for the study of the
in which we all learn our native tongue; it is the most normal cognitive system requires some assumptions,
common type of familiar discourse during which two among which are the modularity and the subtraction
or more participants take turns speaking and listening. assumption.
A conversation is a collaborative endeavor in which par- The modularity assumption says that a complex cog-
ticipants recognize a common goal. What the partici- nitive function consists of a series of functionally inde-
pants say at any moment is determined by the pendent subcomponents or modules that perform
common final goal, but what they communicate to different functions and interact with other parts of the
REHABILITATION OF APHASIA 329
system; the subtraction assumption says that no new et al., 2000; Luzzatti et al., 2000; Kiran et al., 2001; Peach,
cognitive structure is created as a consequence of the 2002; Rapp and Kane, 2002; Raymer et al., 2003; Sage
lesion; pathological transformations of normal cognitive et al., 2005; Viswanathan and Kiran, 2005).
functions obey constraints determined by the normal Sentence-level problems are very frequent in aphasia
structure of the system and can be inferred from the but in contrast to the numerous treatments for naming
analysis of the normal structure. The relevance of these disorders, sentence-level disorders have only rarely
assumptions for treatment is evident. If a cognitive func- been tackled. Recently sentence-level treatments have
tion (such as naming, for instance) consists of a series of benefited from contributions from psycholinguistic
independent components that can be separately dam- studies and the cognitive neuropsychological approach.
aged and no reorganization is possible, a detailed Two types of treatment are now rather common; one
diagnosis will locate the damage and therapy will be spe- based on the mapping hypothesis and another on the lin-
cifically directed to that component and not to unspeci- guistic theory of wh-movements.
fied naming disorders. According to the mapping hypothesis, the verbs se-
It is clear that the more explicit the reference model, mantic information does not only specify the core mean-
the more precise can be the diagnosis based on the model. ing of the verb, it also dictates the number of arguments
A functional diagnosis is different from a syndromic involved and their role in the event. Thus the core mean-
diagnosis, such as Broca aphasia or Wernicke aphasia. ing of rob is to deprive someone; it is a two-argument
A syndromic diagnosis essentially involves classifying verb and the thematic roles involved are an agent (caus-
the aphasia according to which cluster of symptoms it fits ing the deprivation) and a patient (experiencing the rob-
best. A number of symptoms can generally be present in a bing). If an aphasic patient still understands the core
syndrome, but no particular one is necessary for the meaning of rob but has lost information about its the-
diagnosis. In other words, taking Broca aphasia as an matic grid he or she will have nothing to map onto syntax
example, the classic symptoms of reduced speech, and will have problems understanding sentences such as
speech apraxia, and agrammatism are not always present The thief robbed the old lady unless using knowledge
in subjects classified as having Broca aphasia. of the world where it is more probable for a thief to rob
A functional diagnosis, on the other hand, involves an old lady than vice versa. The mapping hypothesis has
determining the functional damage underlying the super- given rise to many therapy programs for comprehension
ficial symptoms, and two subjects with the same func- and production. Treatment is focused on emphasizing
tional damage will show the same symptoms. the centrality of the verb and its relationship to the nouns
A precise and correct diagnosis, however, does not in the sentence, and attempts to link sentence structure
dictate what to do. The contribution of cognitive neuro- to sentence meaning (for review see Marshall, 1995;
psychology to aphasia therapy is a negative contribu- Mitchum et al., 2000).
tion because the more precise the diagnosis the more The second type of treatment is based on the linguistic
constrained the therapeutic choices rationally related theory of wh-movement. According to linguistic theory
to the impairment. When the cause of an impaired behav- noncanonical sentences have two levels of representa-
ior is unknown, the choice of possible interventions is tion: the underlying or d-structure (akin to the basic
very large but the opposite is true when the underlying Subject-Verb-Object pattern in the English language)
cause is known. and the s-structure. Noncanonical sentences are derived
The past 30 years have seen important progress in our from the d-structure through application of rules which
understanding of the normal processing of single words. involve movement of a constituent from its original
In addition, there is now widespread consensus on a position into a new position in the s-structure (for
dual-route model according to which reading and spell- instance, the subject in the canonical sentence Martin
ing can be achieved through a lexical route, which allows is eating the fruit moves from the front position and
the correct pronunciation and spelling of stored words, takes the last position in the passive sentence the fruit
and a non-lexical route, which allows conversion of sub- is eaten by Martin, a noncanonical sentence). One of the
lexical units of phonemes or graphemes into sequences two major types of movement is wh-movement. To form
of graphemes or phonemes, respectively. Only the a wh-question, wh-, which refers to a constituent that
conversion mechanisms are dedicated to reading and spell- occupies a certain position in the d-structure, is moved
ing; the lexical routes utilize parts of the lexicalsemantic to the front of the sentence. When movement occurs,
system that are also used in other tasks, such as auditory a trace (t) is left behind in the original position (Martin,
comprehension and naming. The dual-route model has in the previous example). Most English interrogative
prompted many of the more recent papers on therapy words start with wh- (who, what, where, why, etc.),
for reading and writing disorders, and treatment is which explains the term wh-movement. Difficulty com-
generally focused on the impaired route (e.g., Beeson prehending noncanonical sentences in English, such as
330 A. BASSO ET AL.
passives, object relatives, and certain forms of wh- but included only rehabilitated subjects, and the possible
question, is a widely reported characteristic of agram- effect of spontaneous recovery was ignored (Butfield
matic aphasia. and Zangwill, 1946; Marks et al., 1957; Leischner and
Treatment involves meta-linguistic knowledge of Lynk, 1967; Sands et al., 1969; Sarno and Levita, 1979).
verb properties and movement, and takes into account The subsequent group of studies compared treated
both the lexical and syntactic properties of sentences. and untreated subjects, thus taking into account sponta-
The first steps do not differ significantly from mapping neous recovery, but results were equivocal because ther-
therapy, being mainly concerned with improving knowl- apy was found to have a significant effect on recovery in
edge of the thematic role information about verbs. Then some (Hagen, 1973; Basso et al., 1975, 1979; Gloning
instructions concerning the movements of various sen- et al., 1976; Shewan and Kertesz, 1984; Poeck et al.,
tence constituents are provided and aphasic subjects 1989; Mazzoni et al., 1995), but not in others (Vignolo,
are trained to produce wh-movements (see Thompson 1964; Pickersgill and Lincoln, 1983; Lincoln et al.,
et al., 2003; Thompson and Shapiro, 2007). 1984). The effect of rehabilitation has also been studied
by comparing results obtained by speech therapists and
The social or consequences-based approach volunteers (Meikle et al., 1979; David et al., 1982; Wertz
Clinicians have always stressed the fact that aphasia et al., 1986; Hartman and Landau, 1987; Marshall et al.,
has a significant impact on the whole life of individuals 1989). Treatment was always found to be effective, but
suffering from the disorder, upsetting their relationships none of the studies that adopted this strategy found sig-
with other family members, colleagues, and friends. nificantly better results for the group of subjects treated
Aphasic individuals suddenly find themselves deprived by speech therapists.
of their role, socially isolated, and unable to express their As can be seen, the results are not clear-cut: they are
wishes and emotions. positive (rehabilitation has a significant effect on re-
Recently the idea that disabled persons have a right to covery) in about half of the studies that have a control
an environment without barriers has taken root and group and in all studies comparing therapists and volun-
therapy has turned to the external environment, trying teers (however, without a difference between the two
to modify it to adapt it to the aphasic person. Examples groups). On the other hand, no significant difference
of this category are the Supporting Conversation for was found between treated and untreated patients in
Adults with Aphasia (Kagan, 1998; Kagan et al., 2001) about 50% of the studies comparing these two groups.
and the Conversational Coaching approaches (Holland, One common element in the negative studies was
1991; Hopper et al., 2002), which aim to teach effective brevity of treatment. Some researchers have specifically
strategies to the normal interlocutor rather than to tackled the issue of amount of treatment. With different
modify the verbal behavior of the aphasic individual. nuances, all of these studies report better recovery for
It is impossible to delineate the boundaries of the in- subjects who received more treatment (Brindley et al.,
terventions that can go under the name social or conse- 1989; Denes et al., 1996; Hinckley and Carr, 2005; Basso
quences-based approach. They can run the gamut from and Caporali, 2001). Furthermore, Bhogal et al. (2003)
breathing therapy to improving the ability of the person considered all works published between 1975 and 2002
with aphasia to return to work. Some examples of the in which recovery of a group of treated patients was
differences between the social approach, which focuses compared with that of a group of nontreated patients,
on changing the environment and the impairment-based and they identified those that presented sufficient data
approach, which aims to rehabilitate the aphasia itself, for reanalysis (n 10). Significantly more therapy ses-
can be found in a recent book written by aphasia re- sions were carried out in the positive studies than in
searchers and clinicians (Martin et al., 2008). Two differ- the negative ones, thus confirming the importance
ent clinicians describe the suggested treatment for each of amount of treatment.
of five aphasic individuals, one clinician from the point Scientific evidence in support of efficacy of aphasia
of view of the impairment-based approach and the other therapy has come from meta-analyses and systematic
from the social point of view that aims to minimize the reviews. Four meta-analyses (Whurr et al., 1992; Robey,
consequences of aphasia. 1994, 1998; Rohling et al., 2009) and two systematic
Table 27.1 summarizes the various approaches to reviews (Cicerone et al., 2000; Cappa et al., 2003),
aphasia rehabilitation and their theoretical underpinnings. which were subsequently updated (Cappa et al., 2005;
Cicerone et al., 2005), were conducted. All meta-
analyses confirmed the effectiveness of rehabilitation.
EFFICACY OFAPHASIA THERAPY
Finally, the latest Cochrane review (Kelly et al., 2010)
The first group of studies on treatment efficacy, per- concludes Significant differences between the groups
formed in the 1950s and 1960s, reported positive results, scores were few but there was some indication of a
REHABILITATION OF APHASIA 331
Table 27.1
Approaches to aphasia rehabilitation and their theoretical underpinnings

Stimulation Approach Holistic School


Simulation of inaccessible language mainly through Language is a complex, indivisible psychological function, a
comprehension exercises that vary only according to the property of the total brain. Aphasia can only vary in severity;
severity of the aphasic disorder in aphasia, language is not lost but inaccessible
Behavior Modification Approach Operant Conditioning
Applies to aphasia therapy based on the principles of Human behavior is determined by external stimuli; verbal
operant conditioning and programed instruction. Shaping behavior is not qualitatively different from other behaviors.
and fading are the most important techniques. Stresses Only external stimuli and responses can be studied
methodology scientifically
Functional Reorganization Approach Luria
Analysis of all the steps underlying the execution of Language functions are based on a network of neurological
the impaired task and conscious execution of each structures, each playing a different role but all contributing
step, with external aids. Conscious substitution to correct processing. Aphasia syndromes differ according
of the impaired link with one from an undamaged to the site of lesion, which interferes with a basic component
system of a language function
Pragmatic Approach Pragmatics
The main goal of therapy is to restore communicative Stresses communication and studies the use of
competence by whatever means: language, gestures, mimic, language in context. Views aphasia as a communication
drawing, and so forth disorder
Neurolinguistic Approach Neurolinguistics
Scattered and rather vague suggestions to base therapy Analyzes in terms of a linguistic theory the language
on linguistic principles. Principles of Chomskys impairments that follow brain damage
competence-performance dichotomy and
Neoassociationism
transformation grammar have been used
Language is the sum of a number of faculties 
Neoassociationist or Syndromic Approach comprehension, production, reading, writing. Damage to
Therapy is still mainly based on stimulation, but more attention different areas of the brain differently affects verbal
is given to the level of the linguistic disorder (phonemic, behavior
lexical, or syntactic) and therapy varies according to the type
Cognitive Neuropsychology
of aphasia. Much research on aphasia therapy effectiveness
Language consists of a series of independent subcomponents
Cognitive Neuropsychological or Impairment-based that perform different functions and interact with other
Approach parts of the system. Impaired language performance is
Therapy targets the damaged sub-component(s) of explained in terms of damage to one or more of the
language, as inferred from a model of normal language subcomponents in a model of normal language processing
processing
Social Consequences
Social or Consequences-based Approach Aphasia, with its effects on both language and communication,
Therapy aims to reduce whatever barriers prevent aphasic results in barriers to an individuals ability to participate in
people from using language and communication to life
participate in life

Modified from Basso A (2003). Aphasia and its Therapy, by permission of Oxford University Press.

consistency in the direction of results which favoured sufficiently long times (or sufficient intensity) are effi-
the provision of speech and language therapy (SLT). cacious while others are not.
However, since the therapeutic interventions were ex- The American Society of Rehabilitative Medicine
tremely varied (conventional, group treatment, com- (Cicerone et al., 2000, 2005) and the European Federa-
puter-mediated), the number of sessions was low in tion of Neurological Societies (EFNS) (Cappa et al.,
five studies (max 48), and trained volunteers delivered 2003, 2005) independently conducted systematic re-
the treatment in two studies it is no wonder that signif- views. Cappa et al.s review (2003, 2005) concludes with
icant differences were few. The most plausible a grade B recommendation; Cicerone et al. (2000, 2005)
explanation is that some treatments delivered for classify 11 studies in class I. Both reviews reach the
332 A. BASSO ET AL.
conclusion that sufficient experimental evidence exists Beeson P, Rewega M, Vail S et al. (2000). Problem-solving ap-
to recommend treatment of aphasia. proach to agraphia treatment: interactive use of lexical and
Moss and Nicholas (2006) studied another variable: sublexical spelling routes. Aphasiology 14: 551565.
time from onset. They analyzed 23 studies including a Benson F, Ardila A (1996). Aphasia. A Clinical Perspective.
Oxford University Press, New York.
total of 57 patients subdivided into six groups ac-
Bhogal SK, Teasell R, Speechley M (2003). Intensity of apha-
cording to the time between the morbid event and the
sia therapy, impact on recovery. Stroke 34: 987993.
beginning of treatment. Data indicate that the effect Brindley P, Copeland CD, Pru M (1989). A comparison of the
of rehabilitation did not diminish with the passing of speech of ten chronic Brocas aphasics following intensive
the years, at least until the seventh year. and non-intensive periods of therapy. Aphasiology 3: 695707.
Brookshire RH (1971). Effects of delay of reinforcement on
probability learning by aphasic subjects. J Speech Hear
CONCLUSIONS Res 14: 92105.
Brust JCM, Shafer SQ, Richter RW et al. (1976). Aphasia in
As this chapter illustrates, aphasia rehabilitation has un- acute stroke. Stroke 7: 167174.
dergone many changes since it became widely practiced Butfield E, Zangwill OL (1946). Re-education in aphasia: a
after the World War II. Many of these changes have been review of 70 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 9: 7579.
the result of increased knowledge in areas such as brain Cappa SF, Benke T, Clarke S et al. (2003). EFNS Guidelines
functioning, learning theory, and the structure of on cognitive rehabilitation: report of an EFNS Task Force.
language. Others have resulted from changing social at- Eur J Neurol 10: 1123.
titudes. Many disciplines, including neurology, linguis- Cappa SF, Benke T, Clarke S et al. (2005). EFNS guidelines on
tics, speech-language pathology, cognitive psychology, cognitive rehabilitation: report of an EFNS task force. Eur J
and others, have contributed to the knowledge base that Neurol 12: 665680.
underlies aphasia rehabilitation. It is highly likely that Caradang R, Seshadri S, Beiser A et al. (2006). Trends in in-
cidence, lifetime risk, severity, and 30-day mortality of
knowledge in these areas will continue to evolve, and that
stroke over the past 50 years. JAMA 296: 29392946.
aphasia therapy will continue to evolve as well. Although Caramazza A, McCloskey M (1988). The case for single-
it is now quite evident that aphasia therapy is effective, it patient studies. Cogn Neuropsychol 5: 517528.
remains true that most aphasic people do not fully re- Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Kalmar K et al. (2000).
cover their language. For those engaged in rehabilitation Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: Recommenda-
of aphasia, the goal remains to provide the best possible tions for clinical practice. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 8:
therapy based on current knowledge, while always at- 15961615.
tending to the developing state of knowledge, and incor- Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Malec JF et al. (2005). Evidence-
porating new knowledge into their treatment of aphasia. based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the
literature from 1998 through 2002. Arch Phys Med Rehabil
REFERENCES 86: 16811692.
Darley FL (1982). Aphasia. WB Saunders, Philadelphia.
Alajouanine T, Ombredane A, Durand M (1939). Le Syn- David R, Enderby P, Bainton D (1982). Treatment of acquired
drome de la Desintegration Phonetique dans lAphasie. aphasia: speech therapists and volunteers compared.
Masson, Paris. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 45: 957961.
Alajouanine T, Lhermitte F, Ledoux et al. (1964). Les compo- Davis CH, Harrington G, Baynes K (2006). Intensive semantic
santes phonemiques et semantiques de la jargonophasie. intervention in fluent aphasia: a pilot study with fMRI.
Rev Neurol (Paris) 110: 120. Aphasiology 20: 5983.
Albert M, Sparks R, Helm NA (1973). Melodic intonation ther- Davis G, Wilcox M (1985). Adult Aphasia Rehabilitation:
apy for aphasia. Arch Neurol 29: 130131. Applied Pragmatics. NFER-Nelson, Windsor.
Albert M, Goodglass H, Helm NA et al. (1981). Clinical as- Denes G, Perazzolo C, Piani A et al. (1996). Intensive versus
pects of dysphasia. Springer Verlag, Vienna. regular speech therapy in global aphasia: a controlled
Basso A (2010). Natural conversation: A treatment for se- study. Aphasiology 10: 385394.
vere aphasia. Aphasiology 24: 466479. Di Carlo A, Inzitari D, Galati F et al. (2003). A prospective
Basso A, Caporali A (2001). Aphasia Therapy or the impor- community-based study of stroke in Southern-Italy: the
tance of being earnest. Aphasiology 15: 307332. Vibo Valentia Incidence of stroke study (VISS). Methodo-
Basso A, Faglioni P, Vignolo LA (1975). Etude controlee de la logy, incidence and case fatality at 28 days, 3 and 12
reeducation du langage dans laphasie: comparaison entre months. Cerebrovasc Dis 16: 410417.
aphasiques traites et non-traites. Rev Neurol 131: 607614. Edwards AE (1965). Automated training for a matching-
Basso A, Capitani E, Vignolo LA (1979). Influence of rehabil- to-sample task in aphasia. J Speech Hear Res 8: 3942.
itation language skills in aphasia. Arch Neurol 36: 190196. Fillingham JK, Sage K, Lambon Ralph MA (2006). The treat-
Beeson PM, Egnor H (2006). Combining treatment for written ment of anomia using errorless learning. Neuropsychol
and spoken naming. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 12: 816827. Rehabil 16: 129154.
REHABILITATION OF APHASIA 333
Foldi NS, Cicone M, Gardner H (1983). Pragmatic categories in fluent aphasia by controlling exemplar typi-
aspects of communication in brain-damaged patients. In: cality. J Speech Lang Hear Res 46: 773787.
S Segalowitz (Ed.), Language Functions and Brain Organi- Kiran S, Thompson CK, Hashimoto N (2001). Training graph-
zation. Academic Press, New York, pp. 5186. eme to phoneme conversion in patients with oral reading
Gloning K, Trappl R, Heiss WD et al. (1976). Prognosis and naming deficits: a model-based approach. Aphasiology
and speech therapy in aphasia. In: Y Lebrun, R Hoops 15: 855876.
(Eds.), Recovery in Aphasics. Humanities Press, Atlantic Kirshner HS, Jacobs DH (2009). eMedicine Specialties. http://
Highlands, NJ, pp. 5762. emedicine.medscape.com/article/1135944-overview.
Goodglass H, Kaplan E (1983). The Assessment of Aphasia and Leischner A, Lynk HA (1967). Neure Erfahrungen mit der
Other Disorders. 2nd edn.. Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia. Behandlung von Aphasien. Nervenartz 38: 199205.
Hagen C (1973). Communication abilities in hemiplegia: Lesser R (1989). Aphasia: theory-based intervention. In:
effect of speech therapy. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 54: MM Leahy (Ed.), Disorders of Communication: The Sci-
454463. ence of Intervention. Taylor and Francis, New York,
Hartman J, Landau W (1987). Comparison of formal language pp. 189205.
therapy with supportive counseling for aphasia due to acute Lincoln NB, McGuirk E, Mulley GP et al. (1984). Effective-
vascular accident. Arch Neurol 44: 646649. ness of speech therapy for aphasic stroke patients: a ran-
Hatfield FM (1972). Looking for help from linguistics. domized controlled trial. Lancet 2: 11971200.
Br J Disord Commun 7: 6481. Luria AR (1963). Restoration of Function After Brain Injury.
Hatfield FM, Shewell C (1983). Some applications of linguis- Pergamon Press, Oxford.
tics to aphasia therapy. In: C Code, DJ M uller (Eds.), Luria AR (1970). Traumatic Aphasia. Mouton, The Hague.
Aphasia Therapy. Edward Arnold, London, pp. 6175. Luria AR, Nyadin VL, Tsvetkova LS et al. (1969). Restoration
Hinckley JJ, Carr TH (2005). Comparing the outcomes of in- of higher cortical function following local brain damage.
tensive and nonintensive context-based aphasia treatment. In: PJ Vinken, GW Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of Clinical
Aphasiology 19: 965974. Neurology. Vol. 3. North-Holland Publishing Company,
Holland AL (1980). Communicative Abilities in Daily Living. Amsterdam, pp. 368433.
University Park Press, Baltimore. Luzzatti C, Colombo C, Frustaci M et al. (2000). Rehabilita-
Holland A (1991). Pragmatic aspects of intervention in apha- tion of spelling along the sub-word-level routine. Neurop-
sia. J Neurolinguistics 6: 197211. sychol Rehabil 10: 249278.
Hopper T, Holland A, Rewega M (2002). Conversational MacMahon MKC (1972). Modern linguistics and aphasia.
coaching: treatment outcomes and future directions. Apha- Br J Disord Commun 7: 5465.
siology 16: 745761. Marks M, Taylor M, Rusk HK (1957). Rehabilitation of the
Howard D, Hatfield FM (1987). Aphasia Therapy. Historical aphasic patient: a summary of three years experience
and Contemporary Issues. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, in a rehabilitation setting. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 38:
Hillsdale, NJ. 219226.
ILSAWG (Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging Work Group) Marshall J (1995). The mapping hypothesis and aphasia ther-
(1997). Prevalence of chronic diseases in older Italians: apy. Aphasiology 9: 517539.
comparing self reported and clinical diagnoses. Int J Epide- Marshall RC, Wertz RT, Weiss DG et al. (1989). Home treat-
miol 26: 9951002. ment for aphasic patients by trained nonprofessionals.
Jakobson R (1964). Towards a linguistic typology of aphasic im- J Speech Hear Dis 54: 462470.
pairments. In: AVS de Reuck, M OConnor (Eds.), Disorders Martin N, Thompson CK, Worrall L (2008). Aphasia Rehabil-
of Language. Churchill Livingstone, London, pp. 2142. itation. The Impairment and its Consequences. Plural Pub-
Kadzielawa D, Dabrowska A, Nowakowska M et al. (1981). lishing, San Diego, CA.
Literal and conveyed meaning as interpreted by aphasics Mazzoni M, Vista M, Geri E et al. (1995). Comparison of lan-
and nonaphasics. Polish Psychol Bull 12: 5762. guage recovery in rehabilitated and matched non-
Kagan A (1998). Supported conversation for adults with apha- rehabilitated aphasic patients. Aphasiology 9: 553563.
sia: methods and resources for training conversation part- Meikle M, Wechsler E, Tupper A et al. (1979). Comparative
ners. Aphasiology 12: 816829. trial of volunteers and professional treatments of dysphasia
Kagan A, Black SE, Duchan JF et al. (2001). Training volun- after stroke. Br Med J 2: 8788.
teers as conversation partners using Supported conversa- Miller N (1989). Acquired speech disorders: Applying
tion for adults with Aphasia (SCA): a controlled study. linguistics to treatment. In: K Grundig (Ed.), Linguistics
J Speech Lang Hear Res 44: 624638. in Clinical Practice. Taylor and Francis, New York,
Kelly H, Brady MC, Enderby P (2010). Speech and language pp. 281300.
therapy for aphasia following stroke. Cochrane Database Mitchum CC, Greenwald ML, Berndt RS (2000). Cognitive
Syst Rev 5: CD000425. treatments of sentence processing disorders: What have
Kertesz A (2007). Western Aphasia Battery (Revised). Har- we learned? Neuropsychol Rehabil 10: 311336.
court Assessment, Inc, San Antonio, TX. Moss A, Nicholas M (2006). Language rehabilitation in
Kiran S, Thompson CK (2003). The role of semantic complex- chronic aphasia and time postonset: a review of single-
ity in treatment of naming deficits: training semantic subject data. Stroke 37: 30433051.
334 A. BASSO ET AL.
Peach RK (2002). Treatment for phonological dyslexia target- Sarno MT, Levita E (1979). Recovery in treated aphasia in the
ing regularity effects. Aphasiology 16: 779789. first year post-stroke. Stroke 10: 663670.
Pedersen PM, Jrgensen HS, Nakayama H et al. (1995). Apha- Schuell H, Jenkins JJ, Jimenez-Pabon E (1964). Aphasia in
sia in acute stroke: incidence, determinants, and recovery. Adults. Harper & Row, New York.
Ann Neurol 38: 659666. Schwartz M (1984). What the classical aphasia categories
Pickersgill MJ, Lincoln NB (1983). Prognostic indicators and cant do for us. Brain Lang 21: 38.
the pattern of recovery of communication in aphasic stroke Shewan CM, Kertesz A (1984). Effects of speech and language
patients. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 46: 130139. treatment in recovery from aphasia. Brain Lang 23:
Poeck K (1983). What do we mean by Aphasic Syndromes? 272299.
A neurologists view. Brain Lang 20: 7989. Thompson CK, Shapiro LP (2007). Complexity in treatment of
Poeck K, Huber W, Willmes K (1989). Outcome of intensive syntactic deficits. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 16: 3042.
language treatment in aphasia. J Speech Hear Disord 54: Thompson CK, Shapiro LP, Kiran S et al. (2003). The role of
471479. syntactic complexity in treatment of sentence deficits in
Porch B (1967). Porch Index of Communicative Ability. Con- agrammatic aphasia: The complexity account of treatment
sulting Psychologists Press, California. efficacy (CATE). J Speech Lang Hear Res 46: 288297.
Prins RS, Snow CE, Wagenaar E (1978). Recovery from apha- Tikofsky RS, Reynolds GL (1962). Preliminary study:
sia: spontaneous speech versus language comprehension. non-verbal learning and aphasia. J Speech Hear Res 5:
Brain Lang 5: 192211. 133143.
Rapp B, Kane A (2002). Remediation of deficits affecting dif- Tikofsky RS, Reynolds GL (1963). Further studies on non-
ferent components of the spelling process. Aphasiology 16: verbal learning and aphasia. J Speech Hear Res 6: 329337.
439454. Vignolo LA (1964). Evolution of aphasia and language rehabil-
Raymer AM, Cudworth C, Haley MA (2003). Spelling itation: a retrospective exploratory study. Cortex 1: 344367.
treatment for an individual with dysgraphia: analysis of gen- Viswanathan M, Kiran S (2005). Treatment for pure alexia
eralization to untrained words. Aphasiology 17: 607624. using a model based approach: evidence from one acute
Raymer AM, Ciampitti M, Holliway B (2007). Semantic- aphasic individual. Brain Lang 95: 204206.
phonologic treatment for noun and verb retrieval impair- Wade DT, Langton Hewer R, David RM et al. (1986). Aphasia
ments in aphasia. Neuropsychol Rehabil 17: 244270. after stroke: natural history and associated deficits.
Robey RR (1994). The efficacy of treatment for aphasic per- J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 49: 1116.
sons: a meta-analysis. Brain Lang 47: 582608. Weisenburg T, McBride KE (1935). Aphasia, A Clinical
Robey RR (1998). A meta-analysis of clinical outcomes in the and Psychological Study. The Commonwealth Fund,
treatment of aphasia. J Speech Lang Hear Res 41: 172187. New York. (Reprinted 1964, Hafner).
Rohling ML, Faust ME, Beverly B et al. (2009). Effectiveness Wepman JM (1951). Recovery from Aphasia. Ronald Press,
of cognitive rehabilitation following acquired brain injury: New York.
a meta-analytic re-examination of Cicerone et als (2000, Wertz RT, Weiss DG, Aten JL et al. (1986). Comparison of
2005) systematic review. Neuropsychology 23: 2039. clinic, home, and deferred language treatment for aphasia:
Sage K, Hesketh A, Lambon Ralph M (2005). Using errorless a Veterans Administration cooperative study. Arch Neurol
learning to treat letter-by-letter reading: contrasting word ver- 43: 653658.
sus letter-based therapy. Neuropsychol Rehabil 15: 619642. Whurr R, Lorch MP, Nye C (1992). A meta-analysis of studies
Sands E, Sarno MT, Shankweiler D (1969). Long-term assess- carried out between 1946 and 1968 concerned with the
ment of language function in aphasia due to stroke. Arch efficacy of speech and language therapy treatment for
Phys Med Rehabil 50: 202206. aphasic patients. Eur J Disord Commun 27: 117.
Sarno MT (1969). The Functional Communication Profile: Wilcox MJ, Davis GA, Leonard LB (1978). Aphasics compre-
Manual of Directions. New York University Medical hension of contextually conveyed meaning. Brain Lang 6:
Center, New York. 362377.

You might also like