You are on page 1of 16

Trainee Prize in Counselling Psychology 2009 (Runner-up)

Gay monogamy and extra-dyadic sex:


A critical review of the theoretical and
empirical literature
Kristoff Bonello

Background and objectives: The issue of gay monogamy and extra-dyadic sex has challenged
heteronormative relationship conventions for decades and controversial research findings have been widely
cited. This paper reviews a small but meaningful body of theoretical propositions and empirical evidence
that looks at monogamy and nonmonogamy in male couples and the functions of extra-dyadic sex.
Method: Papers were retrieved through searches in a range of sources and were sifted for relevance to this
review. Data were synthesised using meta-ethnographic procedures (Noblit & Hare, 1988) in order to
provide an interpretative representation of the literature. Critical appraisal of the literature is given
throughout and a discussion of the implications for psychotherapeutic practice is also presented.
Recommendations for future research are formulated in conclusion.
Results: Most couples established non-monogamous relationships for reasons of sexual variety and were
equally adjusted and functional as their monogamous counterparts. Non-monogamy in gay-male couples
does not inevitably conflate with relationship dissatisfaction in such dyads. Monogamy was sustainable for
many but not for others who broke their original agreements. The sparse coverage of LGB issues in the
literature coupled with the shortage of robust empirical evidence on gay male coupledom signals psychologys
need to address this area further in its research.

S SEXUAL NON-MONOGAMY, adopted however, argued that non-monogamous gay

I by gay male couples, a sign of relationship


dysfunction? Sexual exclusivity in gay male
relationships, or the lack thereof, has been
relationships are compatible with mental
health and equally conducive to satisfactory
and functional committed relationships
subjected to polemical research findings for (Bonello & Cross, in press; LaSala, 2004a/b).
decades. Whilst monogamy remains inextri- Over the years, evidence has consistently
cably equated with commitment, intimacy shown that a sizeable majority of gay relation-
and stability in most heterosexual relation- ships were sexually non-exclusive (Advocate
ships (Reiss et al., 1980), sexual non- Sex Poll, 2002; McWhirter & Mattison, 1984).
monogamy in gay male couples has often It has been argued that men, in contrast to
been depicted as a symptom of dyadic malad- women, are better able to separate sex from
justment, dysfunction or pathology (Bell & emotion, in a process termed compartmental-
Weinberg, 1978; Peplau & Gordon, 1983). isation, and may, therefore, offer an explana-
Bowenian family theory holds that couples tion for the large number of gay men who
who experience conflict and tension within establish sexually non-monogamous relation-
their relationships often seek out a third ships (Banfield & McCabe, 2001; Bonello &
person, e.g. a sexual partner, in order to Cross, in press; LaSala, 2004b, p.3).
deflect anxiety. This, however, results in symp- Conversely, it has also been posited that some
tomatic emotional triangles that are thought gay men may not have adequate emotional
to have adverse impacts on the couple (Kerr skills required to preserve long-term intimate
& Bowen, 1988). Recent research has, relationships (Brown, 1995) yet such a

Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009 51


The British Psychological Society ISSN 0269-6975
Kristoff Bonello

conclusion seems reliant on a position of other research has called into question the
assumed isomorphism between heterosexual democratisation of gay relationships which
and homosexual relationship structures were found to be wrought by silence rather
(Coyle & Kitzinger, 1995). than honest communication about extra-
Most research conducted in the field of dyadic sexual activity (Worth, Reid &
gay relationships has notably been placed in McMillan, 2002). Research has shown that
the context of the AIDS pandemic. Some sexual exclusivity is an issue some gay couples
research has inferred that increases in struggle with at some point in their relation-
monogamy amongst gay couples were precip- ships and its negotiation may possibly be a
itated by a fear of HIV and, therefore, a source of conflict for some (Silverstein, 1981;
protective measure to preclude infection Greenan, 2003). This may account for the
(Berger, 1990). Other findings have contra- disjunction between what is pledged and what
dicted this position concluding that gay men is actually honoured within some monoga-
have not modified their relationship styles for mous couples.
sexual health reasons (Davidovich et al., 2001; Despite the extensive literature
Halkitis et al., 2004). There has been renewed evidencing few significant differences in
interest in the sexual behaviour of gay men in psychological functioning and adjustment
recent years. Some authors have pointed out between heterosexual and homosexual indi-
that psychologys concern with gay sexual viduals (e.g. Coyle, 1993; Gonsiorek, 1991;
behaviour has been linked to epidemiology Pillard, 1988), lesbian, gay and bisexual
and thus increasingly focused on health (LGB) people have often been reported to
promotion and disease avoidance (e.g. show higher than average rates of access to
Flowers et al., 1999). Health psychology has psychotherapeutic services (Bell & Wein-
been at the forefront of such research and has berg, 1978; Liddle, 1997; Morgan, 1992).
formulated theoretical foundations that are Relationship issues have been found to be a
derivative and based on in situ prevention high mental health concern for gay men,
methods, sexual decision making and safe-sex and lesbian women, who sought therapy with
practices (Flowers et al., 1999). Gay men have clinical psychologists (Murphy, Rawlings &
been found to be at higher risk of contracting Howe, 2002) and disagreement over sexual
sexually transmitted diseases, e.g. HIV, due to exclusivity was cited as a frequent presenting
increased sexual contact with sero-unknown problem in male couples (Greenan, 2003).
and sero-discordant casual partners (Sigma, Furthermore, a recent randomised survey of
2004). Such findings continue to drive world- APA-member licensed psychologists who
wide bans on blood donations from gay men, treated LGB clients in the US (N=378) found
the removal of which could increase transfu- such clients to be a significant part of the
sion-transmission of HIV by 60 per cent in the average caseload of these practitioners
UK (Soldan & Sinka, 2003). Despite a need (Murphy, Rawlings & Howe, 2002, p.187).
for health promotion programmes aimed at Such findings might, therefore, indicate the
targeting high-risk groups, research has importance for practitioners to have a
shown that gay men who engage in casual sex thorough understanding of gay relationship
outwith the primary relationship often do so structures in order to assist gay couples effec-
after safe-sex practices are agreed within the tively, yet extant literature raised concern
dyad (Bonello & Cross, in press; LaSala, over psychologists knowledge and training
2004b). Such findings may, therefore, be on LGB issues (cf. Division 44/Committee
indicative of different sub-groups within gay on Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Concerns
culture and possibly draw distinction between Joint Task Force on Guidelines for
those gay men in non-exclusive unions and Psychotherapy with Lesbian, Gay and
their single counterparts who might not be Bisexual Clients, 2000). Psychologists atti-
similarly bound by safe-sex rules. However, tudes toward LGB populations continued to

52 Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009


Gay monogamy and extra-dyadic sex

improve and less practitioners viewed homo- Method


sexuality as pathological (Kilgore et al., Studies were located by undertaking
2005). Notwithstanding, formal education in searches in a range of sources: PsychInfo,
LGB issues remained inadequate amongst Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews,
professionals and graduate trainees (Kilgore Ovid Medline, reference lists, websites (e.g.
et al., 2005; Murphy, Rawlings & Howe, Google Scholar) and grey literature
2002). Furthermore, it has also been found sources. Searches yielded a vast amount of
that some psychologists refrained from abstracts pertaining to the broad search
working with gay clients since they felt ethi- terms applied (e.g. gay relationship; homo-
cally compromised in light of insufficient sexual; monogam, same-sex relationship;
training and competence (Murphy, Rawlings extra-dyadic; casual sex; non-monogam;
& Howe, 2002). Sexual satisfaction versus sexual exclusives). These were then sifted for
staying sexually exclusive was one specific relevance to the objectives of this review and
issue that surveyed psychologists listed as an inclusion criteria were applied thus elimi-
area in which they required formal training nating those studies which focused on male
(Murphy, Rawlings & Howe, 2002, p.186). It coupledom in the context of sexual health
has also been evidenced that insufficient or HIV infection (Attree, 2004). A total of
knowledge and training on LGB issues nine papers, published from 1985 onwards,
precipitates heterosexist bias amongst were selected and retrieved for inclusion in
mental health providers (Garnets et al., this review (four empirical studies, four theo-
1991; Phillips, 2000) and may, therefore, retical studies and one case-study). Table 1
lead to the framing of gay relationships summarises the main characteristics of the
within heterosexual models. It appears reviewed studies.
evident that research investigating gay male Data from the empirical and theoretical
relationship styles is, therefore, of para- studies were synthesised using meta-ethno-
mount importance. Research in this field graphic methods (Noblit & Hare, 1988) in
may help in avoiding harmful heterosexual order to construct an interpretative repre-
prescriptions that propagate poor practice in sentation of the evidence rather than an
therapies offered to LGB populations. aggregative map. Data synthesis, through
The focus of this review is on the theoret- meta-ethnographic means, involves making
ical and empirical evidence pertaining to gay a whole into something more than the parts
male monogamy and extra-dyadic sex as alone imply (Noblit & Hare, 1988, p.28).
reported in the literature to date. It synthe- Synthesis was achieved by identifying and
sises the data from a small but meaningful comparing key themes and concepts across
number of papers to provide a critical and the studies whilst retaining the authors orig-
interpretative representation of the evidence. inal terms and metaphors. This allowed for
It is beyond the scope of this paper to eval- the reciprocal translation of findings
uate any of the broader issues outlined above (Noblit & Hare, 1988). The latter process
and it, therefore, does not review the litera- served to identify gay couples patterns of
ture pertaining to the sexual health of gay relating, it documented the process by which
men, psychologists competence and training couples manage outside sex and it allowed
or the AIDS epidemic. The objective is to for the recognition of gaps in the evidence
answer the questions: (a) Is the sexual non- (Attree, 2004).
monogamy adopted by gay male couples a sign of Studies were critically evaluated for the
relational dysfunction? (b) For what reasons do quality and robustness of their methods of
gay men establish sexually non-exclusive unions? enquiry. This was done by following conven-
and (c) How do monogamous and non-monoga- tional review guidelines as set out by the
mous couples compare on relationship quality and Centre for Reviews and Dissemination (NHS
psychological adjustment? CRD, 2001). Unfortunately, there seems to

Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009 53


Table 1: Summary of studies characteristics.

54
Adam (2006) Blasband & Greenan (2003) Kurdek & Schmitt Kurdek & Schmitt LaSala (2004a) LaSala (2004b) Worth, Reid & Yip (1997)
Peplau (1985) (1986a) (1986b) McMillan (2002)

Methodology Theoretical Empirical Case-Study Empirical Empirical Empirical Theoretical Theoretical Theoretical
Kristoff Bonello

Aim of Study To investigate Gay male Case-Study Comparing Relationship Comparing Exploration of gay To shed light on To determine how
male couples relationship styles, reporting clinical relationship quality of male relationship mens reasons for monogamy, trust sexual exclusivity
agreements of sex boundaries and work with a gay quality of couples in sexually quality of sexually non-monogamy and sexual is defined by gay
outside the quality differences couple negotiating Heterosexual exclusive and non- monogamous and and its effects on behaviour in gay male couples
primary between open and sexual exclusivity Married, exclusive unions non-monogamous the primary male couples
relationship closed Heterosexual male couples relationship
relationships Cohabiting and
Gay and Lesbian
Relationships

Sample no 70 80 1 couple 185 132 121 65 20 60

Ages (Mean = 34) (Median = 31) Early 30s Individual group- (Mean = 33.92) (Mean = 43; (Mean = 43; (Mean = 36) (Mean = 42.9)
means reported SD =11.26) SD = 11.65)

Data Interviews Questionnaires Case-Study Questionnaires Questionnaires Questionnaires In-depth In-depth Semi-structured
Interviews Interviews Interviews

Measures None Love Scale (Rubin, None Symptom Checklist Dyadic Adjustment Dyadic Adjustment None None None
1973); Self-Rating 90 (Derogatis, Scales (Spanier, Scales (Spanier,
Scales 1977); 1976); Love/Liking 1976, 1989)
Relationship Scales (Rubin,
Beliefs Inventory 1970)
(Eidelson &
Epstein, 1982);
others

Outcomes Monogamy No significant Recommendations No differences in Groups equivalent No significant Non-monogamous Monogamous Inconsistencies
common amongst differences in for clinical work psychological on demographics; differences in couples found to relationships between
younger gay men relationship with gay couples adjustment Open couples lived relationship be compatible marked by expectational and
or men new to quality between experiencing between all groups together longer quality between with functional infidelity anxieties behavioural
relationships open and closed relationship than closed open and closed relating exclusivity
couples conflict around couples; No couples
exclusivity differences in
psych. adjustment

Context Canada USA USA USA USA USA USA New Zealand United Kingdom

Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009


Gay monogamy and extra-dyadic sex

be no scientific consensus over what consti- sexual exclusivity with infidelity or


tutes good qualitative research (Sandelowski cheating. Monogamy was openly discussed
et al., 1997). For the purpose of this review, and agreed on by most couples yet some
qualitative papers were appraised using men were reported to have merely acqui-
earlier developed protocols of assessment for esced to their partners requests. Two studies
such studies (Henwood & Pidgeon, 1992; (e.g. Adam, 2006; LaSala, 2004b) found
Mays & Pope, 2000; Popay et al., 1998). some gay mens choice of monogamy to be
This paper will first report on the synthe- based on the heterosexual models of relating
sised findings of monogamous couples which in which they were socialised during their
will be followed by those of open couples as formative years and with which they closely
well as comparative findings between these identified. For example:
groups. A discussion of the appraisal of all Its monogamous and its a trust thing
the reviewed papers and a concluding discus- and its off limits. You know my parents
sion will be presented thereafter. are married for 35, I dont know, 40 years
or whatever and my mom still looks at
Gay monogamous couples: guys and my dad still looks at women and
Beliefs and values its just, its off limits.
Cultural ideals and historical norms have (Adam, 2006, p.11)
endorsed sexual exclusivity, and rejected Well I wanted someone special to share
non-monogamy, amongst heterosexuals for my life with, one person to be with, share
many decades (Reiss, 1980). However, it has happy times, sad times, grow old
long been argued that gay men do not share together. [Monogamy] is a natural thing.
similar historical and cultural baggage, the If youre sharing your life with one
absence of which has led gay couples to person, its natural. Its a physical
construct their own unique relationships commitment as well as emotional.
using a trial-and-error approach (Harry & (LaSala, 2004b, p.7)
DeVall, 1978). Notwithstanding, as is evident However, LaSala (2004b) found that some
in the reviewed studies, it would be a mistake gay men recognised a propensity for jealousy
to assume that all gay men enter sexually within themselves which appeared to moti-
non-exclusive unions or that indeed their vate these men toward monogamy. These
monogamous relationships are destined to findings also accord with empirical findings
fail. In all the synthesised studies some gay that demonstrated jealousy to be a
men were found to establish monogamous common reason for the choice of exclusive-
relationships which were largely described as ness (cf. Blasband & Peplau, 1985). Other
rewarding, lasting and successful. Mono- couples settled on monogamy for fears that
gamy was sustainable for most but not for their sexual health might be compromised
others who eventually agreed to open up by outside sex (LaSala, 2004b) yet such
their relationships to outside sex (Blasband concern did not preclude others from
& Peplau, 1985) or who had broken their discarding monogamy scripts altogether,
original agreements (LaSala, 2004a/b). which is discussed in the section on non-
exclusive couples. Empirical evidence across
Settling for monogamy all the studies showed that frequencies of
The topic of intimacy, fidelity and trust was couples who self-identified as sexually
central to gay mens perceptions of a monogamous were comparable to those who
committed relationship which was distinctly were non-monogamous. However, some of
equated with sexual exclusivity. The men in the men who agreed on monogamy had
these studies established the ground rule of broken their agreements by having outside
monogamy from the outset of their relation- sex at least once (LaSala, 2004a; Yip, 1997).
ships and associated the transgression of In this regard, the latter men who broke

Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009 55


Kristoff Bonello

their agreements might constitute a & Robins, 1973; Warren, 1974) but empirical
different sub-group within monogamous evidence over the last 20 years has contra-
couples. Unfortunately, as discussed later, dicted such a trend (Blasband & Peplau,
some empirical studies failed to control for 1985; Kurdek & Schmitt, 1986b; LaSala,
this subgroup in their statistical analyses 2004a; Yip, 1997).
when comparing open and closed couples I also know, being a man myself, that I
on several variables (e.g. Blasband & Peplau, have to try really hard not to have sex
1985; Kurdek & Schmitt, 1986b). Further- outside the relationship. I have a real
more, one study, which indicated 52.9 per need to do that sometimes Its a reality
cent of the sample (N=60) to be behav- of being sexual people, sexual beings.
iourally exclusive (Yip, 1997), sampled gay (Worth, Reid & McMillan, 2002, p.247).
men solely from Christian movements. Such At times we all, and I think this also
men may have, therefore, been more likely applies to heterosexual couples, have a
to uphold conservative sexual ethics than the mutual sexual attraction with someone
men in the other studies which failed to other than your lover. It is nice to have
report on religious affiliation. For example: the attention, to have a conquest to boost
Christians should really stick with one ones self-esteem and just have sex with
partner Its something that affects who someone new.
we are and the people we grow into. It (Blasband & Peplau, 1985, p.403)
has something to do with the fact that we Despite that some men experienced diffi-
are made in Gods image and that we are culty in initiating discussions with their part-
capable of faithfulness. ners around boundary renegotiation it
(Yip, 1997, p.294) seemed their unhappiness with monogamy
did not inhibit them from secretively
Dyadic communication and trust breaking their original agreements (Adam,
Reading across the studies a theme of uncer- 2006; LaSala, 2004b; Worth, Reid &
tainty and underlying distrust about pre- McMillan, 2002). However, the reported
established monogamy emerged. Some men dissatisfaction was not associated with the sex
were found to be in relationships charac- they had with their primary partner but was
terised by anxieties over whether sexual related to the issue of sexual exclusivity
exclusivity was in fact sustainable over time (LaSala, 2004b).
(e.g. Worth, Reid & McMillan, 2002). In this Im actually trying to work out staying
regard, despite that monogamy was agreed monogamous with him as much as I can
between the dyads, some men doubted I would like to be monogamous. I have
whether their partners were in fact sex outside the relationship even though
upholding their agreements or whether they I havent told him I sort of understood
themselves were happy to remain sexually that he doesnt want to know anything
exclusive. Moreover, it appeared that these about any details.
men were reluctant to initiate communica- (Adam, 2006, p.13)
tion about the possibility of opening up the Not all monogamous relationships were
relationship to outside sex since they feared marked by infidelity anxieties, underlying
their partners would terminate the relation- distrust and secretiveness (Worth, Reid &
ship. Such findings seem to infer that some McMillan, 2002, p.237). Other monogamous
male couples, who establish monogamy at couples in the reviewed studies were not
the outset of their unions, begin to progress reported to experience conflict over sexual
toward seeking outside sexual contact due to exclusivity but maintained satisfactory
their innate desire for sexual variety. This committed relationships over time (Adam,
theoretical discourse has been proposed by 2006; Kurdek & Schmitt 1986a/b; LaSala,
several investigators in the past (e.g. Saghir 2004a/b; Yip, 2006). However, it should be

56 Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009


Gay monogamy and extra-dyadic sex

noted that that all qualitative studies in this for reasons of sexual pleasure rather than
review utilised convenience samples which emotional bonding (Banfield & McCabe,
might signify that couples who experienced 2001; Townsend, 1995). Central to these
dyadic conflict, at the time the studies were mens stories across the studies was the clear
in progress, could have self-selected out. distinction between recreational sex and
Equally, those not experiencing conflict emotional commitment. The evidence
could have self-selected in thus over-repre- showed that sexual variety and the avoidance
senting that sub-group. However, the empir- of possessiveness over ones partner were
ical research carried out by Blasband & important themes in the choice of non-
Peplau (1985) found consistency between monogamy which did not detract from their
partners agreements about sexual exclu- feelings for their partners (Blasband &
sivity and their actual behaviour (p.407). Peplau, 1985). For example:
These findings were later contradicted by It enables us to be mostly monogamous
Yip (1997). by choice, to preserve feelings of
From all the synthesised studies it autonomy and personal freedom along
appears that monogamy within gay relation- with commitment. It maintains
ships can be a sustainable option for some perspective e.g. I just love him, I dont
couples over time. Notwithstanding, this may own him!
not be the case for others who prize sexual (Blasband & Peplau, 1985, p.403)
variety. These men faced communication The fact that I have sex outside the
barriers around boundary re-negotiation for relationship and he may or may not have
fears of losing their partners. It should be sex outside the relationship, in absolutely
noted that the latter men were not seeking no way takes away from the fact that we
to terminate their relationships to which are completely and totally committed to
they were emotionally committed. each other and totally in love and we will
spend the rest of our lives together and
Non-monogamous gay couples: we both know it I consider sex outside
Emotional fidelity vs. sexual exclusivity the relationship to be completely, 100
In all the synthesised studies many couples per cent recreational.
had mutually agreed to be sexually non- (Adam, 2006, p.19)
exclusive. Unlike their monogamous coun- LaSala (2004b) also found that 10 of his sub-
terparts, these men generally disassociated sample of non-monogamous men (N=28)
outside sex from infidelity and articulated a viewed non-exclusivity as having both posi-
need for emotional monogamy within their tive and negative effects on the primary rela-
relationships (Adam, 2006; LaSala, 2004b; tionship, the latter effects being jealousy.
Yip, 1997). In this regard, outside sex was not Moreover, most men in this study experi-
perceived as an act of cheating or a threat enced positive impacts on their relation-
to the dyad but, to the contrary, it reinforced ships, e.g. improved sex life, which is
dyadic commitment in some couples discussed shortly.
(LaSala, 2004b). Most men described a pref-
erence for sexual variety, personal freedom Rules and boundaries in non-monogamous
and egalitarianism in their relationships and couples: Protecting the primary relationship
explained their ability to avoid becoming So how do gay couples who endorse extra-
emotionally involved with casual partners dyadic sex manage their relationships? Across
(Blasband & Peplau, 1985; LaSala, 2004b; all studies it was invariably reported that
Yip, 2007). Such findings seem to concur these men had established clear rules and
with theories purporting men to have a boundaries that regulated outside sex in
propensity for compartmentalisation and regard to health and emotional commit-
hence to seek out extra-marital encounters ment. Most couples had agreements

Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009 57


Kristoff Bonello

regarding the practice of safe-sex with As reported earlier, gay men who were non-
outside partners in order to prevent the monogamous were able to cognitively sepa-
cross-transmission of disease and, ultimately, rate outside sex from their emotions. The
to protect the dyad (LaSala, 2004b). Safe-sex primary function of this compartmentalisa-
signified the use of prophylactics when tion was described as a way of protecting the
engaging in anal intercourse with casual part- couples relationship and hence avoiding
ners or complete abstinence from this form what Bowenian family therapists termed
of sex. Unprotected oral sex was not reported troublesome emotional triangles (Kerr &
to be of concern in any of the studies. Bowen, 1988; Yip, 1997).
Furthermore, jealousy still permeated some We are both very sensitive to the threat of
mutually agreed non-exclusive unions other men forming emotional bonds
(Adam, 2006; LaSala, 2004b) but further with us that we neither invite nor want
boundaries were developed to assuage such If a [an outside] partner makes
feelings. Common amongst some gay men statements that indicate the development
was the agreement to circumscribe outside of an unwanted emotional bond, we cool
sex through the practice of threesomes only off sexual activity with that guy.
and, therefore, individual encounters with (LaSala, 2004b, p.13)
others were proscribed (Adam, 2006; LaSala, However, pragmatic rules were also devel-
2004b). In this regard, outside sex was only oped to complement their cognitive efforts.
allowed when a third person was invited to In this regard, non-exclusive couples who
join the dyad. Moreover, some men had engaged with casual partners individually
further prescripts in place to avoid both (e.g. anonymous sex and visiting sex clubs)
displacing the other partner and the forma- delimited their encounters by establishing
tion of triads, e.g. not bringing casual part- sexual curfews, for example, not engaging
ners into the couples home or not using the with the same partner twice, avoiding
couples bed with them (Adam, 2006). Such extended periods of contact, not engaging
boundaries served to uphold partner primacy socially with a sexual partner and always
and to preclude the formation of emotional returning home rather than spending the
bonds with others, a key factor for non-exclu- night away (Adam, 2006; LaSala, 2004b).
sive couples. The overall theme emergent from the
Most men in these studies reported synthesised studies suggests that whilst many
outside sex to have positive effects on the gay men prized sexual variety, emotional
primary relationship with particular refer- commitment was a fundamental principle of
ence made to the couples sex life (Blasband their relationships which they valued and
& Peplau, 1985; LaSala, 2004b). For affirmed. For most men, the need for sexual
example: openness was not about unhappiness with
Generally, its always been a pretty their primary relationship but a preference
positive area, simply because after every for sexual experimentation with others.
person we have sex [with] outside the Notwithstanding, these findings should not
relationship, we always realise the sex we be assumed to apply to all male couples who
have with each other is better we seek outside sex. As reported earlier, some
always come to the conclusion that the men merely acquiesced to their partners
sex between us is always better. request for an open relationship. Some men
(LaSala, 2004b, p.14) may be reluctant to oppose their partners,
I meet lots of new people. I learn a lot in order to avoid conflict or relationship
about myself. I get fantasy material so dissolution, and therefore such reticence
when I have sex with my lover, it is better could be a sign of power imbalances within
than it would otherwise be. the couple (cf. Case-Study by Greenan,
(Blasband & Peplau, 1985, p.403) 2003). Furthermore, outside sex may also be

58 Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009


Gay monogamy and extra-dyadic sex

a manifest symptom of emotional disconnec- tionship satisfaction (Blasband & Peplau,


tion or a fear of intimacy, by one or both 1985) and psychological adjustment (Blas-
partners, i.e. a defence. Such defensive band & Peplau, 1985; Kurdek & Schmitt,
behaviour, although not overtly evident in 1986b) between open and closed male
the reviewed theoretical and empirical couples, or between monogamous gay
evidence, has, however, been documented in couples and heterosexual married couples
clinical populations (Greenan, 2003). (Kurdek & Schmitt, 1986a). However, intra-
couple comparisons from one study
Psychological adjustment, relationship suggested that those men who originally
satisfaction and longevity pledged monogamy but subsequently broke
Comparing monogamous and non-monogamous their agreements were less adjusted and
The shortage of published empirical satisfied than the couples who remained
research comparing monogamous and non- monogamous or who had discussed outside
monogamous couples renders a discussion sex (LaSala, 2004a, p.405). Such findings,
on differential variables between these based on very small sub-sample sizes (N=4),
groups limited. To date, the available should, however, be interpreted with caution
evidence reviewed in this paper has despite that instruments with sound psycho-
suggested no significant differences between metric properties were reportedly used
open and closed male couples on demo- (LaSala, 2004a). Furthermore, Yip (1997)
graphic characteristics (Blasband & Peplau, concluded that couples who were expecta-
1985; Kurdek & Schmitt, 1986b; LaSala, tionally and behaviourally monogamous
2006a). Such findings demonstrated that showed higher levels of relationship satisfac-
demographic factors (e.g. age, income, and tion than those who were sexually non-exclu-
education) do not prescribe the type of rela- sive. Such conclusions were drawn from a
tionship into which gay men enter, whether self-report measure requiring participants to
open or closed. However, recurrent across rate their levels of satisfaction. Absent,
the studies was a lack of important demo- however, was any discussion of the reliability
graphic information reported (e.g. ethnicity, and validity of this measure and, therefore,
religious affiliation and religiosity), the these results remain inconclusive.
implications of which are discussed shortly. All studies reported on relationship dura-
Research conducted in the 1980s found tion in the demographic information they
comparable levels of relationship quality provided. Furthermore, three of the four
between sexually exclusive and non-exclusive empirical studies carried out statistical
male couples (Blasband & Peplau, 1985) but analyses that compared relationship dura-
Kurdek and Schmitt (1986b) concluded that tions between monogamous and non-
monogamous couples reported higher levels monogamous couples. One study reported
of relationship quality than their open coun- that open couples lived together for longer
terparts. When gay couples were compared than those who were in sexually exclusive
to lesbian couples and to heterosexual unions (Kurdek & Schmitt, 1986b). In Blas-
married couples no differences were found band & Peplaus (1985) study, no significant
on relationship quality (Kurdek & Schmitt, differences in length of relationships were
1986a). However, the latter study failed to found between these two groups. However,
include non-monogamous gay couples in its as the latter authors themselves acknowl-
sample. Furthermore, empirical evidence, 20 edged, the men in monogamous relation-
years on, suggested male couples in both ships were more likely to engage in
relationship types to enjoy comparable levels extra-dyadic sex the longer their relation-
of relationship quality (LaSala, 2004a). ships lasted. Unfortunately, no statistical
Empirical research has not demonstrated analysis controlling for this subgroup was
any significant differences in overall rela- provided and, therefore, it remains unclear

Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009 59


Kristoff Bonello

whether their findings of no differences are couples in wider LGB populations. All
as accurate as purported. LaSalas (2004a) studies, however, addressed the ages of their
findings were consistent with those of Blas- participants by reporting a mean or a
band & Peplau (1985) thus concluding that median. Two, also provided standard devia-
no significant differences were found in rela- tions (LaSala, 2004a/b). Sample sizes across
tionship duration between open and closed the empirical studies were generally very
couples. Again, since broken agreement small. This, therefore, restricted further
couples were not statistically controlled for analyses when different sub-groups were
on measures of relationship duration, it is identified within the main sample pool. For
unclear whether exclusive couples are truly example, when analyses of monogamous
comparable to open couples in this regard. couples revealed that some men broke their
In light of these divergent findings and the agreements by engaging in extra-dyadic sex
lack of robust empirical research, a clear (LaSala, 2004a), these men comprised very
picture of how relationship duration is small sub-sample sizes (e.g. N=4). As a result,
related to sexual exclusivity remains elusive. these small groups rendered further analysis
a difficult feat and their results inconclusive
Summary of studies samples, methods or possibly unreliable.
and measures Although the generalisability of findings
This section discusses the quality of the empir- from the synthesised studies is of limited
ical and theoretical studies reviewed in terms utility, they may still be of scientific and
of their samples, methods and measures. clinical value to those researching or
assisting gay men.
Summary of studies samples
Some of the studies authors have argued Summary of studies methods
that obtaining genuine random samples of All theoretical studies utilised structured or
gay men is virtually impossible. This argu- semi-structured interviews as a data collec-
ment has been put forward by many scholars tion method. A discussion of the interview
in the past particularly since this group protocols used was mostly omitted except for
represents a partially hidden community two studies which briefly mentioned the
(Flowers, et al., 1999; Morin, 1977). Notwith- topics addressed (e.g. LaSala, 2004b; Yip,
standing, the issue of sample bias merits 1997). Moreover, further methodological
attention. None of the reviewed studies limitations were identified owing to a lack of
made use of randomised samples which was transparency over the procedures used in
generally acknowledged by investigators. It the studies analytic strategies. Of the four
should be noted that most studies attracted theoretical papers, only one discussed how
relatively small homogenous samples which the data were analysed and the steps taken to
were predominantly composed of affluent counteract researcher bias, for example, the
white men who may, therefore, not be repre- use of validation methods (LaSala, 2004b).
sentative of the diversity amongst gay men. The remainder either failed to address data
Unfortunately, only half of the studies analysis altogether or provided very limited
reported on ethnicity, by providing percent- information. For example, Adam (2006)
ages or numbers, and just two studies merely stated that transcribed data were
reported on the religious denominations of organised using QSR N6 software. When
their participants. Ethnicity may impact on conducting qualitative research it is essential
how gay men value openness and exclusivity that authors provide sufficient information
in their relationships (Wagner et al., 2000) about their data analytic procedures in order
and, therefore, the absence of such demo- for their readers to evaluate the quality of
graphic information precludes any broader their findings. In the absence of this infor-
interpretation of those findings to gay mation it is impossible to discern how

60 Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009


Gay monogamy and extra-dyadic sex

authors have moved from data to interpreta- Discussion and implications for
tion (Popay et al., 1998). Furthermore, when psychotherapeutic practice and
considering that researcher bias is a widely research
held criticism of qualitative research Perhaps one of the most striking conclusions
(Henwood & Pidgeon, 1992) the impor- that can be drawn from the reviewed studies
tance of investigator reflexivity is paramount. is the rich variety of relationship configura-
Only one study (LaSala, 2004b) included a tions that gay men construct. Although, at
reflexive statement that allowed its reader- the surface, outside sex in committed rela-
ship to frame the authors position in rela- tionships might generate doubt as to
tion to the study. whether such unions are sustainable,
reviewing the scientific evidence reveals the
Summary of studies measures complex processes that operate within the
The reviewed empirical studies assessed infrastructures of some gay relationships.
participants on several variables including Irrespective of relationship type, whether
relationship quality, relationship satisfaction open or closed, a common goal for these
and psychological adjustment. Statistical men was to maintain functional and
analyses were carried out to compare open committed unions over time rather than
and closed couples on these variables. All the entering multiple cursory relationships. It
studies generally made use of standardised would therefore be premature to assume
measures that had sound psychometric prop- that sexually non-monogamous couples
erties whilst some also used instruments that maintain dysfunctional or unsatisfactory
were constructed by the investigators them- relationships, as has been reported (Bell &
selves (e.g. Blasband & Peplau, 1985; Kurdek Weinberg, 1978; Saghir & Robins, 1973).
& Schmitt, 1986b). LaSala (2004a) assessed Contrary to what Bowenian family theory
participants on relationship quality using the purports about couple conflict and resultant
Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS), developed emotional triangles (Kerr & Bowen, 1988),
by Spanier (1976), and reported on its the open couples in these studies prized
psychometric integrity, for example, excel- partner primacy and upheld emotional
lent internal consistency (=.91). However, commitment. Several studies have shown that
no mention of this instruments validity was the presence of outside sex does not auto-
made. The DAS was also used in other matically result in depleted relationships but
studies, e.g. Kurdek & Schmitt (1986a/b). reinforces dyadic commitment in some
Furthermore, Blasband & Peplau (1985) (LaSala, 2004a/b). As discussed in the intro-
used Rubins (1973) Love Scale, which has duction of this review, men are able to cogni-
well established reliability, and also used a tively separate sex from love (LaSala, 2004b,
self-rating scale. Again, no discussion of the p.3). Recurrent across all the synthesised
validity of either of these measures was studies was the distinction made between
presented. recreational sex and emotional fidelity by
One theoretical paper, reported meas- open couples. In this respect, it appears
uring differences in relationship satisfaction evident that extra-dyadic sex seems to
between closed and open couples (Yip, comprise a fundamental constituent of some
1997). As aforementioned, a self-report successful unions. Furthermore, the theoret-
measure was administered to the sample in ical studies reporting directly from gay
order to assess relationship satisfaction. This couples experiences may lend important
instrument asked participants to rate their information to those assisting gay couples.
level of satisfaction on a scale of 1 to 10. For example, an understanding of the intri-
Unfortunately, he does not report on the cate processes these men adopt in managing
reliability and validity of this instrument and outside sex effectively, such as boundary affir-
a very small sample size was used (N=30). mation and emotional monogamy, could be

Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009 61


Kristoff Bonello

of valuable help. Whilst the findings in this rance, that may propagate poor practice in
review might challenge heteronormative therapies with gay couples. These findings
relationship conventions, it would be may, therefore, point to the importance for
prudent to consider that the core values therapists to facilitate dyadic communication
which these men espouse often differ from when male couples present with problems
most heterosexuals beliefs about what a rela- around monogamy. However, notwith-
tionship or marriage should, or should not, standing this evidence, gay men are not
constitute. As a result, mental health asymptomatic of, or immune to, underlying
providers might need to consider and assimi- emotional difficulties. It is clear, from
late such findings in their work with this clinical populations, that for some men the
group. Furthermore, empirical evidence, difficulty with staying sexually exclusive may
although mostly limited and dated, has be related to deeper self-issues such as a fear
suggested comparable relationship satisfac- of intimacy or abandonment (Greenan,
tion and psychological adjustment between 2003). The exploration of such possibilities
open and closed gay couples and also in a helping setting should, therefore,
between male couples and their heterosexual neither be ignored nor hastily labelled as
counterparts (Kurdek & Schmitt, 1986a/b; heterosexist bias. Rather, what seems
LaSala, 2004a). Again, these findings suggest important, owing to gay mens variety of rela-
no evidence that sexually non-exclusive gay tionship styles, is practitioners flexibility and
relationships are unsustainable. openness to both monogamy and non-
Although it is clear that some couples monogamy as a therapeutic option.
adopt and manage monogamy scripts There are limitations to this review. The
successfully, it is also evident that others generalisability of the empirical evidence is
experience difficulty with sexual exclusive- largely limited due to the non-random
ness over time. Despite that the evidence sampling procedures adopted by the studies.
reviewed herein contradicts earlier reported Although obtaining this type of sample when
trends of natural progression from exclu- studying gay populations is difficult, some-
sivity to openness, some couples demon- times prohibitively so, several measures can
strably become unhappy and unsatisfied with be taken by future studies to increase the
their original agreements (Worth, Reid & generalisability of their findings. Firstly, the
McMillan, 2002). Through the synthesis of collection of more encompassing demo-
several theoretical studies, we can learn of graphic information would be of help in
the micro-dynamics that furnish such diffi- extending interpretation to other LGB
culties rather than drawing blanket conclu- populations. None of the reviewed studies
sions of dysfunction. As the studies have directly investigated how ethnicity and/or
shown, communication barriers were largely religious affiliation affects the type of rela-
responsible for these mens difficulties whilst tionship into which gay men enter. Research
their emotional commitment was not in this area, both empirical and theoretical,
afflicted by the desire for outside sex is needed and may shed light on the cultural
(LaSala, 2004b). Interpreting the aforemen- contexts in which these men function.
tioned difficulty of staying exclusive through Secondly, the use of significantly larger
heterosexual models of relating may serve to sample sizes would improve the quality of
diagnose underlying dysfunction or, at worst, results and would also be more permissive of
even pathology. However, when drawing on sub-sample analyses when these are
the available evidence from their non-exclu- required. Furthermore, most theoretical
sive counterparts it is evident that non- papers, which explored important aspects of
monogamy does not automatically conflate male couples, were methodologically flawed
with dysfunction. It is this confluence, often owing to a lack of transparency in the
prompted by heterosexist bias and igno- reporting of their analytic procedures.

62 Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009


Gay monogamy and extra-dyadic sex

Qualitative studies with optimum designs relationships in rural areas to those in


and robust methodologies are much urbanised settings where sexual opportuni-
needed. Despite these limitations, the ties are comparatively plentiful and easily
authors of these studies should be accessible (Bonello & Cross, in press).
applauded for their groundbreaking work in Finally, studies that investigate psychologists
this under-researched area of gay male attitudes, training and therapeutic
coupledom. approaches to LGB clients in the UK remain
The shortage of empirical and theoret- mostly absent.
ical studies investigating gay relationship The psychology profession espouses to
patterns and processes indicates examine human behaviour and experience
Psychologys need to address this area across all of its diversity. However, the sparse
further than it has. Most of the available coverage of LGB issues in non-specialist
empirical research originated from the US journals continues to neglect the gaps in
and is mostly dated. On the other hand, knowledge about this group. Such a paucity
research carried out in the UK has been in the psychological literature therefore
largely focused on the sexual health and questions the commitment of the profession
safe-sex practices of gay men (Flowers, in this regard.
1999). It has been clear for many years that
the question of functional vs. dysfunctional Acknowledgement
has been adequately answered and, there- Sincere thanks to Malcolm C. Cross, PhD
fore, research that transcends both singular (Reader in Psychology, City University), for
and dichotomous representations of gay supervising this work.
experience is overdue. To date, many impor-
tant questions remain unanswered. We still The author
do not know much of how gay couples in the Kristoff Bonello, DPsych, is a Chartered
UK establish relationships and how these Counselling Psychologist working in the
advance over time. Empirical and theoretical NHS.
evidence in this regard is both necessary and
timely owing to the current political climate Correspondence
of civil partnerships and adoption laws. Also, Dr Kristoff Bonello
cross-cultural studies that look at gay mens Department of Psychology,
relationship patterns are largely unavailable City University,
or otherwise limited to the demographic Northampton Square,
information embedded within other studies. London EC1V 0HB.
Furthermore, it would also be interesting to E-mail: fotsirk@btinternet.com
see sound research designs that compare gay

Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009 63


Kristoff Bonello

References
Adam, B.D. (2006). Relationship innovation in male Flowers, P., Hart, G. & Marriott, C. (1999).
couples. Sexualities, 9(1), 526. Constructing sexual health: Gay men and risk
Attree, P. (2004). Growing up in disadvantage. in the context of a public sex environment.
A systematic review of the qualitative evidence. Journal of Health Psychology, 4, 483495.
Child: Care, Health and Development, 30(6), Garnets, L., Hancock, K.A., Cochran, S.D.,
679689. Goodchilds, J. & Peplau, L.A. (1991). Issues in
The Advocate Sex Poll. (2002, 20 August). psychotherapy with lesbians and gay men:
The Advocate, 869/870, 2843. A survey of psychologists. American Psychologist,
Banfield, S. & McCabe, M.P. (2001). Extra relation- 46, 964974.
ship involvement among women: Are they Gonsiorek, J. (1991). The empirical basis for the
different from men? Archives of Sexual Behaviour, demise of the illness model of homosexuality.
30, 110142. In J. Gonsiorek & J. Weinrich (Eds.), Homo-
Bell, A.P. & Weinberg, M.S. (1978). Homosexualities: sexuality: Research implications for public policy
A study of diversity among men and women. London: (pp.15136). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Mitchell Beazley. Greenan, D. (2003). Do open relationships work?
Berger, R.M. (1990). Men together: Understanding Gay couples and the question of monogamy.
the gay couple. Journal of Homosexuality, 19(3), Psychotherapy Networker: Washington. Retrieved
3049. 10 May 2007, from:
Blasband, D. & Peplau, L.A. (1985). Sexual www.psychotherapynetworker.org
exclusivity versus openness in gay male couples. Halkitis, T., Zade, D., Shrem, M. & Marmor, M.
Archives of Sexual Behaviour, 14, 395412. (2004). Beliefs about HIV non-infection and
Bonello, K. & Cross, M.C. (in press). Gay monogamy: risky sexual behaviour among MSM. AIDS
I love you but I cant have sex with only you. Education and Prevention, 16, 448458.
Journal of Homosexuality. Harry, J. & DeVall, W.B. (1978). The social organisation
Brown, L. (1995). Therapy with same-sex couples: of gay males. New York: Praeger.
An introduction. In N.S. Jacobson & A.S. Henwood, K.L. & Pidgeon, N.F. (1992). Qualitative
Gurman (Eds.), Clinical handbook of couple therapy research and psychological theorising. British
(pp.274291). New York: Guilford Press. Journal of Psychology, 83(1), 97112.
Coyle, A. (1993). A study of psychological well-being Kerr, M. & Bowen, M. (1988). Family evaluation:
among gay men using the GHQ-30. British Journal An approach based on Bowen theory. New York:
of Clinical Psychology, 32, 218220. Norton.
Coyle, A. & Kitzinger, C. (1995). Lesbian and gay Kilgore, H., Sideman, L., Amin, K., Baca, L. &
couples: Speaking of difference. The Psychologist, Bohanske, B. (2005). Psychologists attitudes and
8(2), 6469. therapeutic approaches toward gay, lesbian and
Davidovich, U., de Witt, J., Albrecht, N., Geskus, R., bisexual issues continue to improve: An update.
Stroebe, W. & Coutinho, R. (2001). Increase in Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training,
the share of steady partners as a source of HIV 42(3), 395400.
infection: A 17-year study of seroconversion Kurdek, L.A. & Schmitt, J.P. (1986a). Relationship
among younger and older gay men. AIDS, 15, quality of partners in heterosexual married,
13031308. heterosexual cohabiting, and gay and lesbian
Derogatis, L.R. (1977). SCL: Administration, scoring, relationships. Journal of Personality and Social
and procedures manual for the revised version. Psychology, 51, 711720.
Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University School Kurdek, L.A. & Schmitt, J.P. (1986b). Relationship
of Medicine. quality of gay men in closed or open
Division 44/Committee on Lesbian, Gay, and relationships. Journal of Homosexuality, 12, 8599.
Bisexual Concerns Joint Task Force on LaSala, M.C. (2004a). Extra-dyadic sex and gay male
Guidelines for Psychotherapy with Lesbian, Gay, couples: Comparing monogamous and non-
and Bisexual Clients (2000). Guidelines for monogamous relationships. Families in Society:
psychotherapy with lesbian, gay, and bisexual The Journal of Contemporary Human Services, 85,
clients. American Psychologist, 55, 14401451. 405412.
Eidelson, R.J. & Epstein, N. (1982). Cognition and LaSala, M.C. (2004b). Monogamy of the heart: Extra-
relationship maladjustment: Development of a dyadic sex and gay male couples. Journal of Gay &
measure of dysfunctional relationship beliefs. Lesbian Social Services, 15, 124.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, Liddle, B.J. (1997). Gay and lesbian clients selection
715720. of therapists and utilisation of therapy.
Psychotherapy, 34, 1118.

64 Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009


Gay monogamy and extra-dyadic sex

Mays, N. & Pope, C. (2000). Assessing quality in Rubin, Z. (1970). Measurement of romantic love.
qualitative research. British Medical Journal, 330, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45,
5052. 104117.
McWhirter, D.P. & Mattison, A.M. (1984). The male Rubin, Z. (1973). Liking and loving: An invitation to
couple: How relationships develop. Englewood Cliffs, social psychology. New York: Holt, Reinhart &
NJ: Prentice-Hall. Winston.
Morgan, K.S. (1992). Caucasian lesbians use of Saghir, M.T. & Robins, E. (1973). Male and female
psychotherapy: A matter of attitude? Psychology of homosexuality: A comprehensive investigation.
Women Quarterly, 16, 127130. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Morin, S.F. (1977). Heterosexual bias in psycho- Sandelowski, M., Docherty, S. & Emden, C. (1997).
logical research on lesbianism and male Qualitative metasynthesis: Issues and techniques.
homosexuality. American Psychologist, 32, 629637. Research in Nursing and Health, 20, 365371.
Murphy, J.A., Rawlings, E.I. & Howe, SR. (2002). Sigma Research (2004). The boys in the backroom:
A survey of clinical psychologists treating lesbian, Anonymous sex among gay and bisexual men.
gay and bisexual clients. Professional Psychology: Silverstein, C. (1981). Man to man: Gay couples in
Research and Practice, 33(2), 183189. America. New York: Morrow.
NHS CRD (Centre for Reviews and Dissemination) Soldan, K. & Sinka, K. (2003). Evaluation of the de-
(2001). Undertaking systematic reviews of research selection of men who have had sex with men
effectiveness: CRDs guidance for those carrying out or from blood donation in England. Vox Sanguinis,
commissioning reviews. NHS Centre for Reviews 84(4), 265273.
and Dissemination. York University. Spanier, G.B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment:
Noblit, G.W. & Hare, R.D. (1988). Meta-ethnography: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage
Synthesising qualitative studies. London: Sage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the
Publications. Family, 38, 1528.
Peplau, L.A. & Gordon, S.L. (1983). The intimate Spanier, G.B. (1989). Manual for the Dyadic Adjustment
relationships of lesbian and gay men. In E.R. Scale. North Tonowanda, NY: Multi-Health
Allegier & N.B. McCormick (Eds.), Changing systems.
boundaries: Gender roles and sexual behaviour. Townsend, J.M. (1995). Sex without emotional
Palo Alto, CA: Mayfield. involvement: An evolutionary interpretation of
Phillips, J.C. (2000). Training issues and sex differences. Archives of Sexual Behaviour, 24,
considerations. In R.M. Perez, K.A. DeBord & 173206.
K.J. Bieschke (Eds.), Handbook of counselling and Wagner, G.J., Remien, R.H. & Carballo-Dieguez, A.
psychotherapy with lesbian, gay, and bisexual clients (2000). Prevalence of extra-dyadic sex in male
(pp.337358). Washington, DC: American couples of mixed HIV status and its relationship
Psychological Association. to psychological distress and relationship quality.
Pillard, R. (1988). Sexual orientation and mental Journal of Homosexuality, 39, 3146.
disorder. Psychiatric Annals, 18, 5156. Warren, C.A.B. (1974). Identity and community in the
Popay, J., Rogers, A. & Williams, G. (1998). Rationale gay world. New York: John Wiley.
and standards for the systematic review of Worth, H., Reid, A. & McMillan, K. (2002).
qualitative literature in health services research. Somewhere over the rainbow: Love, trust, and
Qualitative Health Research, 8, 341351. monogamy in gay relationships. Journal of
Reiss, I.L., Anderson, R.E. & Sponaugle, G.C. (1980). Sociology, 38(3), 237253.
A multivariate model of determinants of extra- Yip, A.K.T. (1997). Gay male christian couples and
marital permissiveness. Journal of Marriage and the sexual exclusivity. Sociology, 31(2), 289306.
Family, 42, 395411.

Counselling Psychology Review, Vol. 24, Nos. 3 & 4, November 2009 65


Copyright of Counselling Psychology Review is the property of British Psychological Society
and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without
the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or
email articles for individual use.

You might also like