You are on page 1of 18

A Biopsychosocial Perspective

of Adolescent Health and Disease

Jessica M. Sales and Charles E. Irwin Jr.

Adolescence is a developmental stage characterized endanger adolescents current and future health
by dramatic physical, cognitive, social and emo- and well-being (Sales & Irwin, 2009).
tional changes. For most adolescents, it is a period Risk is defined as a chance of loss and risk-
marked by rapid physiological change, increased taking is often defined as engaging in behaviors
independence, a change in family relationships that may have harmful consequences, but simul-
that is more interdependent, prioritizing peer taneously provide an outcome that can also be
affiliations, initiation of intimate partner relation- perceived as positive (Beyth-Marom & Fischoff,
ships, identity formation, increased awareness of 1997). For example, driving too fast is considered
morals and values, and cognitive and emotional a risk-taking behavior because it can result in a
maturation. Despite the rapid change, the major- positive feeling (i.e., rush of adrenaline) while
ity of adolescents cope successfully with the the action is occurring, but it also can result
demands of physical, cognitive, and emotional in serious harm to self or others (i.e., health-
development during this time period (Cicchetti & endangering). Examples of frequent health-
Rogosch, 2002). In addition to the positive endangering behaviors adolescents engage in
growth seen during adolescence, it is also a include the use of tobacco and alcohol, experi-
developmental stage recognized for turmoil and mentation with illicit and/or prescription drugs,
challenges, partly due to increased exploration unsafe sexual activities, poor eating habits, as
and risk-taking typical of adolescence. Although well as delinquent actions (Centers for Disease
considered to be a normative part of adolescence, Control and Prevention, 2010).
risk-taking behaviors are nonetheless concerning Adolescent risk-taking is concerning because
to parents, peers, teachers, clinicians, research- of the immediate danger it poses, but also because
ers, and society because these actions often research has demonstrated that the behaviors
established during adolescence often persist into
adulthood (Park, Mulye, Adams, Brindis, &
J.M. Sales, Ph.D. (*) Irwin, 2006). The potential long-term conse-
Department of Behavioral Sciences and Health
quences of engaging in the most prevalent ado-
Education, Rollins School of Public Health,
Emory University, 1518 Clifton Road Room 570, lescent risk-taking behaviors include substance
Atlanta, GA 30322, USA abuse, cancers associated with tobacco use,
e-mail: jmcderm@emory.edu unwanted pregnancies, sexually transmitted
C.E. Irwin Jr., M.D. infections (STIs) including HIV, obesity or other
Division of Adolescent Medicine, Department of health problems caused by problem eating
Pediatrics, School of Medicine, University of California,
(i.e., eating disorders), and serious criminal
San Francisco, Box 0503, LH 245, San Francisco,
CA 94143, USA activity (Sales & Irwin, 2009). For instance, in
e-mail: irwinch@peds.ucsf.edu 2010 the Monitoring the Future Study found that

W.T. ODonohue et al. (eds.), Handbook of Adolescent Health Psychology, 13


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-6633-8_2, Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013
14 J.M. Sales and C.E. Irwin Jr.

41 % of high school seniors had consumed alcohol take into account the role of biology, psychosocial
in the past 30 days, and over 35 % had used mari- influences, and the environment. In line with this
juana in the past year, with 6.1 % using marijuana ideology, the National Institute of Health (NIH)
daily, and that the use of non-prescribed prescrip- issued a report explicitly stating the need for a
tion-type drugs was high (21.6 % lifetime use) better understanding of the interaction between
(Johnston et al., 2011). Specific to sexual behav- biology, environment, and behavior and stressed
ior, after a decade of declining adolescent the utility of such an approach for advancing our
pregnancy rates, in 2006 the numbers inclined understanding of behavior, particularly behaviors
(Centers for Disease Control and Prevention which place one at risk (Working Group of the
(CDC), 2008), and in 2007 a report by the CDC NIH Advisory Committee, 2004). The biopsy-
indicated that one in four adolescent females chosocial model of risk-taking (Irwin &
have had an STI, with rates doubling in certain Millstein, 1986) provides a framework in which
subgroups (Forhan et al., 2008). Finally, juveniles social environmental factors as well as existing
accounted for 17 percent of all arrests and 16 biological and psychological predispositions
percent of all violent crime arrests in 1999 interact to influence risk-taking behavior.
(Snyder, 2000). Adolescent female detainees are In the remainder of this chapter, we (1) briefly
currently the fastest growing population of incar- review the studies or theories of risk-taking that
cerated adolescents. In 2008, female adolescents focus predominately on one factor (i.e., biologi-
represented 30 % of total juvenile arrests. While cal, psychological, and environmental) as it
the overall crime rate has declined in the past related to risk-taking behavior; (2) describe the
decade, the incarceration rate for girls, relative to multi-dimensional biopsychosocial model of
boys, decreased less than males in most cate- risk-taking; (3) provide empirical evidence sup-
gories of crimes, including violent offenses porting the utility of the biopsychosocial model
(Puzzanchera, 2009). for better understanding various adolescent
Adolescent death is the ultimate consequence health-endangering behaviors; and (4) suggest
of risk-taking, with most cases of mortality in the future directions for the utility of the biopsycho-
USA during the adolescent period (approxi- social model of risk-taking for advancing our
mately 75 %) resulting from preventable causes understanding of adolescent health.
like motor vehicle crashes, unintended injuries,
homicide and suicide (Fingerhut & Anderson,
2008). Thus, due to the immediate and long-term The Biology of Adolescent Risk-Taking
health consequences associated with adolescent
risk-taking, identifying and understanding fac- Biological factors are thought to influence risk-
tors associated with adolescent risk-taking have taking behaviors through four sources: (1) genetic
become a public health priority. A variety of fac- predispositions, (2) direct hormonal influences,
tors, including biological, psychosocial, and (3) the influence of asynchronous pubertal timing
environmental, have been found to be associated (i.e., earlier or later timing than peers), and (4)
with various adolescent risk-taking behaviors brain/central nervous system development.
(Irwin & Millstein, 1986; Sales & Irwin, 2009). Examples of each type of biological influence are
However, many studies, as well as theories, of presented in turn.
risk-taking behavior are uni-dimensional and
predominately focus on either biological, psy-
chological, or environmental factors indepen- Genetic Predispositions
dently as they affect risk-taking.
Given the complexity of behavior, to provide Long before the mapping of the human genome,
a complete framework for examining the range of the familial nature of health risk behaviors led
factors found to influence adolescent risk-taking some to speculate about the role of genetic pre-
behavior a theory or model must simultaneously dispositions in risk-taking behaviors. Early evidence
A Biopsychosocial Perspective of Adolescent Health and Disease 15

from family studies demonstrated that risk-taking becomes more feasible, identifying other genetic
behaviors tended to cluster within families. markers that predispose adolescents towards
Examples include substance abuse, criminal other risk-taking behaviors is, certainly on the
activity, and injury-related behavior. Specific to horizons.
injury, Schor (1987) found that a small number of
families accounted for a disproportionately large
number of injury-related health care visits, and Direct Hormonal Inuences
individual members of these high injury fami-
lies had similar rates of unintentional injuries, Adolescence is a period also marked hormonal
with injury rates being stable over time. Moreover, changes (increased reproductive/sex hormones,
many studies have demonstrated that children of including estrogen, testosterone, etc.) (Spear, 2000).
alcoholics are nearly four times more likely than Puberty is associated with increases in gonadal
children of nonalcoholics to abuse alcohol as sex hormones (Spear, 2000), and hormones have
adults (Adger, 1991; Windle, 1997). Eloquently been postulated to play a role in brain develop-
designed twin-adoption studies have been able to ment and in adolescent risk-taking (Irwin &
disentangle the effects of shared environment or Millstein, 1986; Shirtcliff, 2009; Steinberg,
learned behaviors from genetic predispositions, 2008). Hormones influence brain maturation of
as children of alcoholic biological parents show a white matter (Asato, Terwilliger, Woo, & Luna,
greater predisposition toward alcohol abuse even 2010; Perrin et al., 2008), and sex hormones con-
when raised by nonalcoholic adoptive parents tribute to the reorganization of dopaminergic
(Cloninger, 1987). neurons in the motivational system (Sisk & Zehr,
Recent advances in genotyping techniques 2005; Steinberg, 2008), which can then lead to
now allow the ability to further explore the role behavioral manifestations in puberty (Shirtcliff,
of genetic predispositions on adolescent risk-tak- 2009; Sisk & Zehr, 2005; Steinberg, 2008).
ing behaviors. For example, in regard to alcohol Hormones are necessary to activate neural cir-
use, genetic studies support the Al allele of the cuitry dedicated to sexuality (Shirtcliff, 2009;
D2 dopamine receptor gene (DRD2) as a risk Sisk & Foster, 2004), but are no longer necessary
marker for alcoholism and substance use disor- once individuals become sexually experienced
ders. Conner and colleagues (2005) found that (Shirtcliff, 2009). Stronger findings of the effects
male adolescents with the A1 (+) allele tried and of sex hormones such as testosterone on sexual
got intoxicated on alcohol more often than boys behavior are more commonly reported among
without this genetic marker providing support for females than males since small changes in testos-
the DRD2 A1 allele as a marker identifying a terone are able to stimulate females sex drive.
subgroup of adolescent males at high risk for According to Udry et al. (1986), the effects of
developing substance use problems. androgens, such as testosterone, on sexual behav-
Further, a genetic vulnerability factor in the ior of adolescent girls work directly on motiva-
promoter region of the serotonin transporter tion for sexuality, libido, and possibly also
gene (5-HTTLPR) has been associated with both personality. All these factors make adolescence
substance use and risky sexual behavior. One and puberty particularly vulnerable periods, espe-
study conducted with adolescents found that cially for young females in regard to sexual deci-
substance use had a significant impact on the sion-making.
sexual risk behavior of youth with one or two
copies of the short allele (the genetic variant
associated with risk) at this site, but substance Inuence of Asynchronous Pubertal
use had little effect on sexual behavior for youth Maturation
without the short allele (Kogan et al., 2010).
These are only two examples of the current The timing of pubertal maturation is related to
work in this emerging field. As the ability to both genetics and hormonal fluctuations. For exam-
affordably collect and analyze genetic data ple, menarcheal age of mothers and daughters are
16 J.M. Sales and C.E. Irwin Jr.

usually significantly correlated, and physical (Steinberg, 2004, 2007). The cognitive-control
pubertal development is preceded by elevations system, which mainly consists of outer regions of
in respective sex steroid levels. There is recent the brain such as the lateral prefrontal and parietal
evidence that pubertal timing in females is cortices and portions of the anterior cingulated
impacted by environmental factors as well, such cortex, is involved in executive function tasks like
as presence or absence of the father during child- planning, thinking ahead, impulse control, and
hood (Belsky, 2011; Deardorff et al., 2011). self-regulation (Giedd, 2008).
Asynchronous pubertal maturation is matura- In addition, according to a recent review by
tion that occurs earlier or later than peers, and it Steinberg (2008), puberty is associated with the
has been hypothesized to be a factor in risk-taking remodeling of dopaminergic pathways in the
(Irwin & Millstein, 1986). Physically mature- socioemotional brain system that influence
appearing adolescents, because of their older reward salience and reward sensitivity, especially
appearance, may be more apt to associate with in social situations. This neural transformation is
older peers and engage in adult behaviors such also accompanied by a significant increase in
as smoking, drinking, and sexual intercourse oxytocin receptors located within the socioemo-
(Brooks-Gunn, 1988), and engaging in such behav- tional system, which is critical as oxytocin
iors may then be supported and imitated by peers. heightens adolescents attentiveness to, and
Research indicates that early maturing females memory for, social information. Steinberg (2008)
are more likely to initiate sexual intercourse at argues that the increase in oxytocin leads to an
younger ages (Phinney, Jensen, Olsen, & Cundick, increase in the salience of peers, and this increase
1990). Younger age at sexual debut is associated in the importance of peers and peer relations
with less consistent contraception and increased plays a role in encouraging risky behaviors. In
numbers of lifetime sex partners, resulting in an sum, in comparison to younger youth, adoles-
increased risk for pregnancy and STIs (Ford cents who have gone through puberty are more
et al., 2005; Kaestle, Halpern, Miller, & Ford, inclined to take risks in order to gain rewards, and
2005; Manning, Longmore, & Giordano, 2000). this tendency is exacerbated by the presence of
peers. Recent research from behavioral science is
consistent with Steinbergs position. For instance,
Brain and Central Nervous System his laboratory-based research found that the pres-
Development ence of peers more than doubled the number of
risks teenagers took in a video driving game and
Recent advances in developmental neuroscience increased risk-taking by 50 % in college students,
indicate that the brain continues to develop into but had no effect among adults (Gardner &
adulthood. In fact, the cognitive-control system in Steinberg, 2005).
the brain, which regulates impulse control, is a
slow maturing system and makes adolescence a
time of heightened vulnerability for risk-taking Beyond Biology
behavior (Steinberg, 2004). According to Steinberg
(2007), adolescent risk-taking is the product of Biological development during adolescence is
both logical reasoning and psychosocial factors. accompanied by physiological changes in the
Logical reasoning abilities are mostly fully devel- ways in which adolescents perceive both them-
oped by the age of 15. However, the psychosocial selves and the world around them. Cognitive
capacities (i.e., impulse control, emotion regula- development may occur in concert or asynchro-
tion, delay of gratification, and resistance to peer nously with physical development. When physi-
influence) that facilitate decision-making and cal development precedes cognitive development
moderate risk-taking are guided by the cognitive- (as often is the case with females experiencing
control systems in the brain and this system early maturation) adolescents are at increased
continues to mature well into young adulthood risk for engaging in health-endangering behaviors.
A Biopsychosocial Perspective of Adolescent Health and Disease 17

Further, although developmental neuroscience about the seriousness of consequences resulting


research has found that the brain is still develop- from risky behavior (Beyth-Marom, Austin,
ing into adulthood, the social world may have Fischoff, Palmgren, & Jacobs-Quadrel, 1993).
unrealistic e or unhealthy expectations of adoles- Research has also examined the role of deci-
cents, especially those whom physically appear sion-making on adolescent risk-taking. Fischoff
as adults. Thus, it is imperative to include the (1992) identified five salient components of deci-
social environment in models exploring adoles- sion-making: (1) identify alternative options,
cent risk-taking. Indeed, when biological models (2) identify possible consequences, (3) evaluate the
are expanded to include psychological vari- desirability of the potential consequences, (4) assess
ables the combined effects of biological and psy- the likelihood of those consequences, and (5) com-
chological factors explains more of the variation bine the information to make a decision.
in health-endangering behaviors (e.g., used ciga- According to Keating (1990), by middle adoles-
rettes or marijuana) than either of these factors cence (i.e., 14 or 15 years of age), most adoles-
alone. cents make decisions in a similar manner to
adults. Although the decision-making process
may be similar, the content of the aforementioned
The Psychology of Adolescent components may differ substantially between
Risk-Taking adults and adolescents. For instance, Beyth-
Marom et al. (1993) found that adolescent and
Psychologically based studies of adolescent risk- adult patterns of responses regarding risk-taking
taking behavior examine the roles of cognition, were similar, with both producing more negative
personality traits, and dispositional characteristics, consequences than positive ones. The difference
such as self-esteem and depression, in risk-taking was that adults reported more consequences over-
behavior. Examples of each are briefly presented all than adolescents.
and discussed in turn. Another cognitive theory of adolescent is
called fuzzy-trace theory (Reyna & Farley, 2006).
Fuzzy-trace theory proposes that that adolescent
The Role of Cognition decision-making is based on simple, gist mental
representations of choices (i.e., fuzzy memory
Cognitive theories of risk-taking behavior explore traces) as opposed to more detailed, verbatim
how people perceive risk and make decisions representations or traces. Because of this, when
about risk-taking. Specific to adolescents, risk adolescents attempt to rationally weigh costs and
perception theory has been guided by the premise benefits (via the use of verbatim memory traces),
that adolescents are optimistically biased or risk-taking increases. However, risk-taking
that they believe themselves invincible. The con- decreases when the core gist of a decision is pro-
cept of invincibility has been frequently employed cessed and a decision is based on this gist infor-
to explain or justify adolescent risk-taking behav- mation (Rivers, Reyna, & Mills, 2008).
ior, although little evidence supports this asser-
tion as people of all ages, not just adolescents,
tend to underestimate the likelihood of experi- The Role of Personality
ence adverse consequences of their actions
(Reyna & Farley, 2006). For instance, Millstein Some evidence indicates that adolescent deci-
and Halpern-Felsher (2002) demonstrated that sion-making may reflect an overall tendency
increasing the salience of the risks associated toward unconventional behavior. Jessors Problem
with making a potentially dangerous decision had Behavior Theory links unconventionality in
the same effect on adolescents and adults. Further, personality, among other things, with an increased
few age differences have been found in regard to likelihood of participating in problem behaviors
individuals evaluations of the risks inherent in a such as risky sexual activity, substance use, and
variety of dangerous behaviors, or in judgments delinquency (Jessor, 2008; Jessor & Jessor, 1977).
18 J.M. Sales and C.E. Irwin Jr.

Problem Behavior Theory has attempted to pro- who endorsed high levels of sexual sensation
vide a theoretical framework for the connection seeking attitudes and behaviors also reported
observed between multiple problem behaviors. high levels of sexual risk-taking behaviors such
This theory suggests that multiple factors con- as frequency of vaginal intercourse, number of
tribute to problem behaviors, defined as socially sexual partners, and inconsistent condom use
problematic, concerning, or undesirable behav- (Spitalnick et al., 2007). Not surprisingly per-
iors usually eliciting some form of social or per- haps, sensation seeking has also been linked to
sonal consequence (e.g., disapproval from others, various biological markers including testosterone
incarceration, health compromise). The theoreti- levels, electrodermal and heart rate responses,
cal framework includes three major systems of and cortical evoked potentials (Zuckerman,
explanatory variables: (1) the perceived-environ- 1990).
ment system, involving social controls, models, The impulsivity seen among sensation-seekers
and support; (2) the personality system, involving may be seen in psychopathologic states that have
values, expectations, beliefs, attitudes, and orien- been linked to an increased likelihood of risk-
tations toward self and society; and (3) the behav- taking behaviors, primarily in male adolescents.
ior system, encompassing both problem and Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
conventional behaviors. Considering these in males has been associated with an increased
explanatory systems, it is possible that engaging risk for delinquency. One study found that male
in health-compromising behaviors (i.e., smoking, youths with ADHD had arrest rates more than
sexual risk behaviors) may be related due to fac- twice those of controls (Farrington, Loeber, &
tors such as social support, depressive symptoms, Van Kammen, 1990). Similarly, male youth with
self-esteem, and other factors underlying these conduct disorders are at increased risk for alco-
risk behaviors and potentially accounting for the hol and substance abuse (Kazdin, 1989).
relationship among them.
The personality trait sensation-seeking has
been used to explain adolescent risk-taking The Role of Dispositional Characteristics
behavior. For example, Zuckerman (1979) states
that sensation seeking is a trait defined by the Self-esteem, depression and locus of control are
need for varied, novel and complex sensations often cited as theoretical or empirically sup-
and experiences and willingness to take physical ported predictors of risk-taking behavior. Lower
and social risks for the sake of such experiences. self-esteem has been associated with age of sex-
Zuckerman developed a Sensation Seeking Scale ual debut in adolescent females (Orr, Wilbrandt,
to assess individual differences in optimal levels Brack, Rauch, & Ingersoll, 1989). Depressive
of arousal. Often high-sensation seekers tend to symptoms and stress have been related to the ini-
perceive less risk in many activities than low- tiation and intensity of use of tobacco among
sensation seekers. Even when the evaluation of adolescents (Covey & Tam, 1990), and more
the risk involved is equal between the two groups, recently, to a various risky sexual behaviors in
high-sensation seekers are significantly more both adolescent males and females (Crepaz &
likely to anticipate more positive potential out- Marks, 2001, Sales, Spitalnick, Crittenden, &
comes than low-sensation seekers. Sensation DiClemente, 2009). Further, depression and
seeking has been associated with a variety of external locus of control have been associated
risk-taking behaviors including substance abuse, with substance use (Baumrind, 1987; Dielman
reckless driving, delinquent actions, and risky et al., 1987). Kohler (1996) examined the rela-
sexual behavior (Andrucci, Archer, Pancoast, & tionship between locus of control, sensation-
Gordon, 1989; Kalichman & Rompa, 1995; seeking, critical-thinking skills, and risk-taking
Newcomb & McGee, 1991; Tonkin, 1987). among adolescents and found a significant cor-
Further, an adolescent-specific sexual sensation- relation between risk-taking and gender, critical
seeking scales has been recently developed thinking, and locus of control. However, it
(DiClemente et al., 2010), and adolescent females should be noted that research has not supported
A Biopsychosocial Perspective of Adolescent Health and Disease 19

a consistent role for any of these psychological in risk-taking behaviors. These studies examine
factors in various risk-taking behaviors (Dryfoos, how the social/environmental context provides
1990; McCord, 1990; Sales, Spitalnick, Crittenden, models, opportunities, and/or reinforcements for
& DiClemente, 2009). adolescent participation in risk-taking behaviors.
The role of family, peers, and society in risk-tak-
ing behaviors are presented in turn.
Beyond the Psychological

In summary, cognitive factors such as risk The Role of Family


perception and decision-making contribute to
adolescent risk-taking. Although adolescents Although adolescence is a time of emerging
decision-making processes appear to differ little autonomy and individuation from the family,
from adults, adolescents lack adult experience most adolescents maintain close relationships
interacting with the social/environmental world with their parents (Steinberg, 1993) and parents
in general, and engaging in decision-making continue to influence their childrens behavior
specifically. As articulated in fuzzy-trace theory, throughout adolescence. Parents play an impor-
their judgments cannot reflect the influence of tant role in determining adolescent involvement
these experiences. Further, adolescent risk-related in risk behaviors. Parental modeling of and per-
decision-making may reflect young peoples ten- missive attitudes toward substance use have been
dencies toward unconventionality and/or sensa- implicated in the initiation of substance use in
tion seeking. Sensation seeking is clearly related early adolescence (Hawkins & Fitzgibbon, 1993;
to increased rates of adolescent risk-taking behav- Werner, 1991). Further, adolescents are less
iors, but not all risk-taking behavior can be con- likely to abuse substances or to initiate sexual
strued as sensation seeking. Some clinical activity when parents provide emotional support
psychological difficulties such as excessive and acceptance, and have a close relationship
aggression, impulsivity, and attention deficit and with their children (Turner, Irwin, Tschann, &
conduct disorders increase the likelihood of ado- Millstein, 1993).
lescents engaging in risk-taking behavior. The In addition to modeling, parental monitoring
role of depression, a highly prevalent issue facing has been widely studied as an important correlate
adolescents, has been linked to substance abuse of adolescent risk-taking behavior (Jaccard &
and risky sexual behaviors, but role of depressive Dittus, 1991). Monitoring and supervision incor-
mood in other types of risk behavior has yet to be porates both communication between parent and
established. Further, the evidence for a causal child, and supervision of the youth. Borawski and
role for self-esteem and locus of control on risk- colleagues (2003) found that perceived parental
taking is unclear. Thus, biological and psycho- monitoring, combined with trust, served as a
logical factors are themselves important significant protective factor against sexual activity
determinants of risk-taking behavior. They also for both males and females, and tobacco and mari-
are the personal filters through which social and juana use in females, and alcohol use in male ado-
environmental stimuli are interpreted and trans- lescents. Less perceived parental monitoring has
lated into action. been associated with increased participation in
antisocial activities, sexual risk-taking, and
increased substance abuse or use (Chilcoat et al.,
The Environment of Adolescent 1995; Smith & Rosenthal, 1995; Steinberg, 1993).
Risk-Taking Parental influence on adolescent behavior var-
ies with the quality of the relationship between the
Environmental models of adolescent risk-taking adolescent and the parent (Bijur et al., 1991; Turner
behavior look at the roles of peers, family, and et al., 1993). Related to this, parental approaches
institutions (school, church, and/or social media) to child rearing have also been associated with
20 J.M. Sales and C.E. Irwin Jr.

adolescents engaging in risk-taking behaviors. For drug use (Erickson & Jensen, 1977) to rape and
instance, Baumrind (1991) found an association homicide (Zimring, 1998). Further, social net-
between adolescent substance use and parenting work analysis of smoking and drug-use behav-
styles. Adolescents whose parents were authori- iors among adolescents has found that both
tative (i.e., demanding and responsive) were less risk-taking and non-risk-taking behaviors (i.e.,
likely to use substances than either those with smoking and drug-use) are learned in the context
authoritarian (i.e., demanding but unresponsive) of peer clusters, and risk-taking peer clusters exe-
or those with permissive (i.e., nondemanding crate greater influence on those peers on the
but responsive) parents. Adolescent with neglect- periphery of clusters than do non-risk-taking peer
ing and rejecting parents were the most likely to clusters (Pearson & Michell, 2000).
engage in substance abuse. Traditionally peer pressure has been viewed
In summary, family approval and modeling of as an etiologic factor in adolescent risk-taking
risk behavior has been linked to adolescent risk- behavior. If remains unclear if risk behaviors are
taking behavior. Parentchild relationships char- initiated in order to conform to an existing peer
acterized by conflict, increased emotional group or if those prone to engage in risk-taking
distance, and non-responsiveness increase the behaviors are drawn to those who are similarly
likelihood of adolescents engaging in health inclined. Lashbrook (2000) provides one possible
endangering behaviors (see Chapter Determinants explanation for how peers exert their impact on
of Health-Related Behaviors in Adolescence by adolescent risk-taking. Specifically, he demon-
DiClemente, Brown, and Davis for a more thor- strated that older adolescents may attempt to
ough review). avoid negative emotions, such as feelings of iso-
lation and inadequacy, by participating in risky
behaviors with peers. Recent findings suggest
Peers that the answer is not straightforward. Brady,
Dolcini, Harper, and Pollack (2009) found that
According to Jessor and Jessor (1977) the relatively adolescents with low social support from peers
greater influence of peers compared to parents is maybe prone to engaging in sexual risk-taking as
associated with a greater tendency (or proneness) a response to stress, whereas adolescents with
toward problem behaviors. As a result, parental high peer support may engage in sexual risk-taking
impact on risk-taking behavior may wane as peer due to peer socialization of risk.
influences increase throughout adolescence.
Consistent with this assertion, Jessor and col-
leagues (1980, 1983) found that peer influence Society
more so than parental influence predicted mari-
juana use, problem drinking, and precocious sex- Societal influences such as mass media and com-
ual debut. munity norms may also influence risk-taking
Peer influence has been a significant factor behavior. Role models and/or images portrayed in
associated with adolescent substance use (Jessor, the media are regularly presented by the media
1976; Kandel, 1985; Newcomb & Bentler, 1989), engaging in various health-endangering behav-
alcohol use (Urberg, Degirmencioglu, & Pilgram, iors (including unprotected sexual behavior and
1997), delinquency (McCord, 1990) and sexual alcohol/substance use), though evidence for the
behaviors. Accordingly, peers have been identified influence of these models/images on actual behav-
as an important influence on the sexual behavior ior is lacking. A recent study conducted among
of adolescents in a wide range of populations adolescent African American girls found that
(Buhi & Goodson, 2007; Pedlow & Carey, 2004). those who watched rap music videos and who
Also, adolescents are usually accompanied by perceived more sexual stereotypes in the videos
one or more persons when committing crimes were more likely to engage in binge drinking, test
that range in seriousness from vandalism and positive for recent marijuana, have multiple sexual
A Biopsychosocial Perspective of Adolescent Health and Disease 21

partners, and have a negative body image a framework in which environmental factors are
(Peterson, Wingood, DiClemente, Harrington, & brought to bear on existing biological and psycho-
Davis, 2007). logical predispositions to influence risk-taking
In addition to media, different communities behavior, and ultimately adolescent health.
and neighborhoods provide adolescents with
opportunities and motivations to engage in risk-
taking behavior. Peer norms reflected in local The Biopsychosocial Model
rates of substance use and teen pregnancy create of Risk-Taking
expectancies of typical adolescent behavior
(Crockett & Petersen, 1993). Local ordinances The biopsychosocial model integrates two areas
allowing cigarette vending machines, legaliza- of research that have often been considered sepa-
tion of marijuana for medical purposes for ado- rately: (1) the relationship of biological develop-
lescents, or lower ages to purchase alcohol ment to psychosocial processing during
provide opportunities for engaging in risk-taking adolescence, and (2) the relationship of risk-taking
behavior. Johnston and colleagues (1993) have behaviors to psychosocial correlates of these
reported that the perceived availability of mari- behaviors (Sales & Irwin, 2009). According to
juana in a community is not necessarily related to this model, biological, psychological, and social
prevalence of use by teens. In fact, declines in or environmental factors influence adolescent
marijuana use by high school seniors have been risk-taking behaviors (Irwin & Millstein, 1986).
accompanied by unchanged or even increased Specifically the timing of biological maturation
perceived availability in recent years. directly influences psychosocial functioning
Cultural expectations may also influence ado- (i.e., cognition, perceptions of self and the social
lescent risk-taking behavior. For instance, despite environment, and personal values). According to
similar ages of sexual debut, the USA has the the model, biological, psychological, and social
highest rates of adolescent childbearing and abor- or environmental variables, mediated by percep-
tion in the developed world (Martin et al., 2006). tions of risk and peer-group characteristics, best
This is speculated to be related to differing cul- predict adolescent risk-taking. Biological vari-
tural attitudes toward adolescent sexuality and ables influencing adolescent risk-taking behavior
contraception (Geronimus, 2003). When looking include pubertal timing, hormonal effects, genetic
within the USA only, contraception rates vary predispositions, and brain maturation.
significantly by ethnicity and religious affiliation Psychological variables associated with risk-tak-
(Brewster, Cooksey, Guilkey, & Rindfuss, 1998; ing include self-esteem, sensation seeking, and
Santelli, Morrow, & Carter, 2004). Substance use cognitive and affective states. Social influences
and age of sexual debut also differ among differ- on adolescent risk-taking include peers, parents,
ent ethnic groups in the USA (Cavanagh, 2004; and school (see Fig. 1).
Guerra, Romano, Samuels, & Kass, 2000). (Note: Given the framework of the biopsychosocial
Ethnicity associated differences may be con- perspective, Irwin and colleagues (Irwin, 1990;
founded by factors related to socioeconomic status; Irwin & Millstein, 1986; Irwin & Ryan, 1989),
See Chapter Determinants of Health-Related have elaborated on the theory to include condi-
Behaviors in Adolescence by DiClemente, tions that may increase the probability that a
Brown, and Davis for further discussion.) given adolescent will engage in risk-taking
behaviors (see Fig. 2). Because of advances in
our understanding of developmental neurosci-
Beyond Environment ence, a fourth biological factor has been added
to the model. Now, the biological factors thought
The studies presented demonstrate the complexity to predispose adolescents to risk-taking behav-
of the interaction between adolescents and their iors include male gender, genetic predisposi-
environment. The biopsychosocial model provides tions, hormonal influences, and prolonged brain
22 J.M. Sales and C.E. Irwin Jr.

Biological Maturation
Hormonal Effects
Pubertal Timing
Genetic Predispositions
Brain Maturation

Cognitive Scope Self-Perceptions Social/Environmental Personal Values


Egocentrism Self-Esteem Perceptions Independence
Future Time- Body Image Parental/Peer Influence Achievement
Perspective Identity Parental/Peer Control
Parental/Peer Support

Risk Perceptions Peer Group


Costs/Benefits Characteristics
Optimistic Bias Peer Age
Controllability Peer Values
Peer Behavior

Risk-taking Behavior

Fig. 1 Model based on the biopsychosocial causal model of risk-taking behavior (Irwin & Millstein, 1986)

maturation. Psychological predisposing factors early maturational timing was associated with a
include sensation seeking, risk perception, more negative self-image, and with earlier onset
depression and low self-esteem. Social environ- of sexual activity. For both males and females,
mental predisposing factors include maladaptive early maturation is a risk factor for the initiation
parenting styles, parental modeling of risk behav- of substance use in adolescents (Tschann et al.,
iors, peer behaviors and socioeconomic status. 1994). Seminal work by Jessor and Jessor (1977)
Finally, adolescent vulnerability to risk-taking supports the roles of environment and personal
behaviors may be increased situationally by fam- values (i.e., psychosocial factors) in the onset of
ily disruption, school transitions, and substance adolescent risk-taking behavior. Specifically, the
use and peer initiation of risk-taking behaviors. predominance of peer influence over parental
influence, along with adolescents placing a
greater personal value on independence versus
Research Supporting the Utility achievement resulted in an increased likelihood
of the Biopsychosocial Model of adolescents engaging in risk-taking behavior.
Moreover, Hughes et al. (1991) conducted a
A variety of studies provide support for the util- study with urban delinquent youth and concluded
ity of the biopsychosocial model for examining that alcohol/substance abuse during adolescence
adolescent risk-taking. For instance, Brooks- further added to biological predispositions,
Gunn (1988) found that among female adolescents, educational difficulties, and coercive family
A Biopsychosocial Perspective of Adolescent Health and Disease 23

PREDISPOSING FACTORS PREDISPOSING FACTORS

Endogenous Factors

Cognitive Immaturity Normal Affective States


Depression Intact Self-Esteem
Low Self-Esteem Value on Achievement
Male Gender Religiosity
Genetic Predisposition Cognitive Maturity
Sensation Seeking
Value on Dependence
Asynchronous Development
Hormonal Effects
Immature Brain Development

Exogenous Factors

Peer Approval of Risk Behaviors Academic Achievement


Lack of Parental Supervision Intact Family
School Failure Close Supportive Parental
Poverty Relationships
Parental Involvement in Risk Church Attendance
Behaviors Involvement in School Activities
Authoritative Parenting

PRECIPITATING
FACTORS

Peer Initiation of Risk


Behaviors
Social Pressure
School Transition
Family Disruption
Initiation of Sexual
Activity or Substance Use
Vulnerability

RISK-TAKING BEHAVIOR

Fig. 2 Factors contributing to the onset of risk-taking behaviors during adolescence (Irwin & Millstein, 1986; Irwin &
Ryan, 1989; Sales & Irwin, 2009)

environments, all of which contribute to their analysis, found that egocentrism contributes
delinquent behavior. While most evidence sup- directly to health-endangering behaviors while
porting the biopsychosocial model stems from influences of self-esteem and perceived social
research in the USA, a study conducted with norms are mediated by risk perception (Omori &
Japanese students, utilizing structural equation Ingersoll, 2005).
24 J.M. Sales and C.E. Irwin Jr.

Often it is difficult for a single study to collect skills of youth, can attenuate risk initiation
data for each area emphasized in the biopsycho- among adolescents.
social model. Thus, articles which can overview Thus, across various behavioral domains,
and synthesize studies provide additional support research supports the utility of the biopsychoso-
for the utility of the biopsychosocial model. For cial model for explaining adolescent risk-taking.
example, a review by Ricciardelli and McCabe
(2004), synthesized the literature and reported
that among adolescent males, disordered eating Future Directions for the
and the pursuit of muscularity are consistently Biopsychosocial Model
associated biological factors such as body mass
index (BMI), psychological factors such as nega- Give the complexity of human behavior, a model
tive affect and self-esteem, and sociocultural fac- must encompass a variety of constructs to more
tors such as perceived pressure to lose weight by fully explain and understand why some people opt
parents and peers (Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2004). to participate in health-endangering activities.
Further, Dodge and Pettit (2003) reviewed of the Because the biopsychosocial approach includes
empirical literature pertaining to the development various constructs empirically linked to adolescent,
of chronic conduct problems in adolescence and it is a more complex model, and until recently it has
conclude that reciprocal influences among bio- been incredibly difficult to empirically examine all
logical dispositions, environments, and life expe- of the factors comprising the model in one study.
riences lead to recursive iterations across time However, with recent advances in technology and a
which either worsen or diminish antisocial devel- concerted effort by researchers (and funding agen-
opment. Additionally, their findings indicate that cies) to engage in interdisciplinary collaborations
adolescents cognitive and emotional processes to more thoroughly examine health-compromising
mediate the relationship between life experiences decisions and behaviors, future research may be
and conduct problems. Finally, specific to aggres- able to do so more frequently.
sion and delinquency in adolescent girls, a review For instance, the recent mapping of the human
by Celio, Karnik, and Steiner (2006) found that genome has allowed us to explore the biological
early maturation is a risk factor for aggression underpinnings of behavior and cognition in ways
and delinquent behavior. However, the way in not possible even a decade ago. Advances in gene
which early physical maturation is perceived and mapping have lead to findings implicating par-
treated by others (family, peers, and society) also ticular genes in alcoholism and substance use
determines how adolescent girls behave. disorders (Conner et al., 2005). Also, genetic
Additional support for the biopsychosocial markers for impulsivity (e.g., DRD4) and depres-
model comes from interventions designed to sive symptomatology (e.g., 5HTT and MAOA)
reduce adolescent risk-taking. For example, are currently being explored, and identifying a
Brody et al. (2009) found that youth in the con- multitude of other genetic markers that predis-
trol group of their Strong African American pose adolescents towards various risk-taking
Families (SAAF) intervention program with the behaviors is on the horizon.
polymorphism in the SCL6A4(5HTT) gene at Advances in brain imaging science have
5-HTTLPR showed significant increases in risk allowed researchers to examine the brain across
behavior initiation (particularly substance use development and while engaging in problem
initiation) across 29 months of follow-up in solving. Many now believe, based upon neuroim-
comparison to youth in the SAAF program with aging studies, that mature decision making is
the same genetic risk. These findings demon- composed of two networks: a highly interconnected
strate that despite genetic predispositions, pro- cognitive-control network that biases decisions
grams that work to intervene at the family level in favor of rational outcomes and a socioemo-
to build supportive family environments, along tional network that biases decision making
with increasing self-esteem and improving life toward reward-based demands (Chein, 2008).
A Biopsychosocial Perspective of Adolescent Health and Disease 25

It is postulated that, in adults, the cognitive-control iPhones, iPads, Netbooks) among adolescent
network can regulate the behavior of the socioe- populations in the USA and around the globe pro-
motional network, allowing for people to make vide teens with nearly unlimited access to infor-
rational, utilitarian decisions. However, neither mation (health promoting and health endangering)
of these systems is fully matured during adoles- and social networks. These advances allow
cence, and each one develops along different unique opportunities to access and assess adoles-
timetables (Giedd, 2008). Thus, these two under- cents, especially as it relates to decision-making,
developed networks and their differing rates of risk-taking, and health outcomes.
development pave the way for heightened risk- In the coming years it will be possible to
taking during adolescence, which, as demon- explore biological influence on behavior and
strated by Gardner and Steinbergs (2005) work the interaction between biology, psychology,
with teen drivers, may be further compounded by environment, adolescent risk-taking behavior,
social and environmental factors, particularly the and health outcomes in ways never possible
presence of peers. In the past decade great before. Thus, just as our society is becoming
scientific advances have been made through neu- more and more complex, the utility of complex
roimaging studies, but understanding the rela- models of adolescent risk-taking like the biop-
tionship between neuroimaging findings and sychosocial model will prove invaluable in
behavior is still in its infancy. Although this is an guiding the next generation of adolescent health
area of great academic interest and active research.
research, demonstrating straight-forward rela-
tionships between the size of, neural activity in,
or connectedness between particular brain regions References
and a specific behavior or ability has, to date,
been challenging at best (DiFranza, 2011; Giedd, Adger, H. (1991). Problems of alcohol and other drug
2008; Rubinstein et al., 2011; Steinberg, 2008). abuse in adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health,
12, 606613.
Technological advances have also bettered Andrucci, G. L., Archer, R. P., Pancoast, D. L., & Gordon,
researchers ability to assess adolescent risk-tak- R. A. (1989). The relationship of MMPI and sensation
ing, as well as psychological and environmental seeking scales to adolescent drug use. Journal of
influences on risk-taking. For example, it is now Personality Assessment, 53, 253266.
Asato, M. R., Terwilliger, R., Woo, J., & Luna, B. (2010).
possible to detect through self-collected vaginal White matter development in adolescence: A DTI
swab specimens, the presence of semen in vagi- study. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 21222131.
nal fluid (Yc PCR). This can then be used as a Baumrind, D. (1987). A developmental perspective on
nondisease marker of unprotected vaginal inter- adolescent risk taking in contemporary America. In C.
E. Irwin (Ed.), Adolescent social behavior and health.
course (Zenilman, Yuenger, Galai, Turner, & New directions for child development (Social and
Rogers, 2005). Also, novel techniques, such as Behavioral Sciences Series, Vol. 37, pp. 93125). San
GeoCoding, allow researchers to spatially place Francisco: Jossey Bass. Fall.
participants in their physical neighborhoods, Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on
adolescent competence and substance abuse. Journal
which are then mapped onto Census data to establish of Early Adolescence, 11, 5695.
neighborhood profiles (Sales & Irwin, 2009). Belsky, J. (2011). Family experience and pubertal devel-
This provides an objective measure of neighborhood opment in evolutionary perspective. Journal of
level social conditions including socioeconomic Adolescent Health, 48(5), 425426.
Beyth-Marom, R., Austin, L., Fischoff, B., Palmgren, C.,
status, racial makeup, population density, as well & Jacobs-Quadrel, M. (1993). Perceived consequences
as access to potentially health-compromising of risky behaviors: Adults and adolescents.
venues (such as fast-food restaurants and liquor Developmental Psychology, 29, 549563.
stores) or health-promoting venues (such as grocery Beyth-Marom, R., & Fischoff, B. (1997). Adolescents
decisions about risks: A cognitive perspective. In J.
stories and green spaces). Further, the explosion of Schulenberg & J. L. Maggs (Eds.), Health risks and
social media (e.g., Facebook, MySpace, YouTube) developmental transitions during adolescence
and mobile communication (e.g., cell-phones, (pp. 110135). New York: Cambridge University Press.
26 J.M. Sales and C.E. Irwin Jr.

Bijur, P. E., Kurzon, M., Hamelsky, V., & Power, C. and substance use in adolescent children of alcoholics.
(1991). Parent-adolescent conflict and adolescent inju- Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 79, 379387.
ries. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Covey, L. S., & Tam, D. (1990). Depressive mood, the
Pediatrics, 12, 9297. single-parent home and adolescent cigarette smok-
Brady, S. S., Dolcini, M. M., Harper, G. W., & Pollack, L. ing. American Journal of Public Health, 80(11),
M. (2009). Supportive friendships and community 13301333.
involvement moderate the association between stress- Crepaz, N., & Marks, G. (2001). Are negative affective
ful life events and sexual risk taking among African states associated with HIV sexual risk behaviors?
American adolescents. Health Psychology, 28(2), A meta-analytic review. Health Psychology, 20(4),
238248. 291299.
Brewster, K. L., Cooksey, E. C., Guilkey, D. K., & Crockett, L. J., & Petersen, A. C. (1993). Adolescent
Rindfuss, R. R. (1998). The changing impact of reli- development: Health risks and opportunities for health
gion on the sexual and contraceptive behavior of ado- promotion. In S. G. Millstein, A. C. Petersen, & E. O.
lescent women in the United States. Journal of Nightingale (Eds.), Promoting the health of adoles-
Marriage and the Family, 60, 493504. cents: New directions for the twenty-first century (pp.
Broawski, E., Iefers-Landis, C. E., Lovegreen, L. D., & 1337). New York: Oxford University Press.
Trapl, E. S. (2003). Parental monitoring, negotiated Deardorff, J., Ekwaru, J. P., Kushi, L. H., et al. (2011).
unsupervised time, and parental trust: The role of per- Father absence, body mass index, and pubertal timing
ceived parenting practices in adolescent health risk in girls: Differential effects by family income and eth-
behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 33, 6070. nicity. Journal of Adolescent Health, 48, 441447.
Brody, G. H., Beach, S. R., Philibert, R. A., Chen, Y., & DiClemente, R. J., Milhausen, R. R., Salazar, L. F.,
McBride Murray, V. (2009). Prevention effects moder- Spitalnick, J., Sales, J. M., Crosby, R. A., et al. (2010).
ate the association of 5-HTTLPR and youth risk Development of the sexual sensation seeking scale for
behavior initiation: Gene x environment hypotheses African American adolescent women. International
tested via a randomized prevention design. Child Journal of Sexual Health, 22, 248261.
Development, 80, 645661. Dielman, T. E., Campanelli, P. C., Shope, J. T., & Butchart,
Brooks-Gunn, J. (1988). Antecedents and consequences A. T. (1987). Susceptibility to peer pressure, self-
of variations in girls maturational timing. Journal of esteem, and health locus of control as correlates of
Adolescent Health Care, 9, 19. adolescent substance abuse. Health Education
Buhi, E. R., & Goodson, P. (2007). Predictors of ado- Quarterly, 14, 207221.
lescent sexual behavior and intention: A theory- DiFranza, J. R. (2011). Who are you going to believe?
guided systematic review. Journal of Adolescent Adolescents and nicotine addiction. Journal of
Health, 1, 427. Adolescent Health, 48(1), 12.
Cavanagh, S. E. (2004). The sexual debut of girls in early Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. (2003). A biopsychosocial
adolescence: The intersection of race, pubertal timing, model of the development of chronic conduct prob-
and friendship group characteristics. Journal of lems in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 39,
Research on Adolescence, 14, 285. 349371.
Celio, M., Karnik, N. S., & Steiner, H. (2006). Early mat- Dryfoos, J. G. (1990). Adolescents at risk. Oxford: Oxford
uration as a risk factor for aggression and delinquency University Press.
in adolescent girls: A review. International Journal of Erickson, M., & Jensen, G. F. (1977). Delinquency is still
Clinical Practice, 60, 12541262. a group behavior: Toward revitalizing the group prem-
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2008). ise of the sociology of deviance. The Journal of
Births: Preliminary Data for 2006. National Vital Criminal Law and Criminology, 68, 262273.
Statistics Reports, 56, 118. Farrington, D. P., Loeber, R., & Van Kammen, W. (1990).
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2010). Youth Cited in U.S. Congress Office of Technology Assessment
risk behavior surveillanceUnited States, 2009. Adolescent health-volume II: Background and the
MMWR, 59(5), 1142. effectiveness of selected prevention and treatment ser-
Chein, J. (2008). Linking risk-taking behavior and peer vices, OTA-H-466. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
influence in adolescents. Neuropsychiatry Reviews, 9, 1. Printing Office. November 1991.
Chilcoat, H. D., Dishion, T. J., & Anthony, J. C. (1995). Fingerhut, L. A., & Anderson, R. N. (2008). The three
Parent monitoring and the incidence of drug sampling leading causes of injury mortality in the United States,
in urban elementary school children. American Journal 19992005. http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/products/pubs/
of Epidemiology, 141, 2531. pubd/hestats/injury99-05/injury99-05.htm#other_
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2002). A developmental ages. Retrieved May 16, 2008, from http://www.cdc.
psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal gov/nchs/health-data-for-all-ages.htm.
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 620. Fischoff, B. (1992). Risk taking: A developmental
Cloninger, C. R. (1987). Neurogenetic adaptive mecha- perspective. In J. F. Yates (Ed.), Risk-taking behavior
nisms in alcoholism. Science, 236, 410416. (pp. 132162). New York: Wiley.
Connor, B. T., Noble, E. P., Berman, S. M., Ozkaragoz, T., Ford, C. A., Pence, B. W., Miller, W. C., Resnick, M. D.,
Ritchie, T., Antolin, T., et al. (2005). DRD2 genotypes Bearinger, L. H., Pettingell, S., et al. (2005). Predicting
A Biopsychosocial Perspective of Adolescent Health and Disease 27

adolescents longitudinal risk for sexually transmitted Jessor, R., Costa, F., Jessor, L., & Donovan, J. E. (1983).
infection: Results from the National Longitudinal Time of first intercourse: A prospective study. Journal
Study of Adolescent Health. Archives of Pediatric and of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 608626.
Adolescent Medicine, 159(7), 657664. Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. L. (1977). Problem behavior and
Forhan, S. E., Gottlieb, S. L., Sternberg, M. R., Xu, F., psychological development: a longitudinal study of
Datta, S. D., Berman, S., & Markowitz, L. E. (2008). youth. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Prevalence of sexually transmitted infections and bac- Johnston, L. D., OMalley, P. M., & Bachman J. G. (1993).
terial vaginosis among female adolescents in the National survey results on drug use from the
United States: Data from the National Health and Monitoring the Future Study, 19751992. Rockville,
Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 20032004. MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse. U.S.
Paper presented at the National STD Prevention Department of Health and Human Services, Public
Conference, Chicago, IL. Health Service, National Institutes of Health.
Gardner, M., & Steinberg, L. (2005). Peer influence on Washington, DC (NIH publication #93-3597).
risk-taking, risk preference, and risky decision Johnston, L. D., OMalley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., &
making in adolescence and adulthood: An experi- Schulenberg, J. E. (2011). Monitoring the future
mental study. Developmental Psychology, 41, national results on adolescent drug use: Overview of
625635. key findings, 2010. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social
Geronimus, A. T. (2003). Damned if you do: Culture, iden- Research, The University of Michigan.
tity, privilege, and teenage childbearing in the United Kaestle, C. E., Halpern, C. T., Miller, W. C., & Ford, C. A.
States. Social Science & Medicine, 57, 881893. (2005). Young age at first sexual intercourse and sexu-
Giedd, J. N. (2008). The teen brain: Insights from neu- ally transmitted infections in adolescents and young
roimaging. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42, adults. American Journal of Epidemiology, 161(8),
335343. 774780.
Guerra, L. M., Romano, P. S., Samuels, S. J., & Kass, P. Kalichman, S. C., & Rompa, D. (1995). Sexual sensation
H. (2000). Ethnic differences in adolescent substance seeking and sexual compulsivity scales: Reliability,
initiation sequences. Archives of Pediatric Adolescent validity, and predicting HIV risk behaviors. Journal of
Medicine, 154, 10891095. Personality Assessment, 65, 586602.
Hawkins, J. D., & Fitzgibbon, J. J. (1993). Risk factors Kandel, D. B. (1985). On processes of peer influences in
and risk behaviors in prevention of adolescent sub- adolescent drug use: A developmental perspective.
stance abuse. In M. Schydlower & P. D. Rogers Advances in Alcohol & Substance Abuse, 4(34),
(Eds.), Adolescent medicine state of the art reviews: 139163.
Adolescent substance abuse and addictions. 4(2), Kazdin, A. E. (1989). Developmental psychopathology:
249262. Current research, issues and directions. American
Hughes, J. R., Zagar, R., Sylvies, R. B., Arbit, J., Busch, Psychologist, 44, 180187.
K. G., & Bowers, N. D. (1991). Medical, family, and Keating, D. P. (1990). Adolescent thinking. In S. S.
scholastic conditions in urban delinquents. Journal of Feldman & G. R. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The
Clinical Psychology, 47, 448464. developing adolescent (pp. 5489). Cambridge:
Irwin, C. E., Jr. (1990). The theoretical concept of at-risk Harvard University Press.
adolescents. Adolescent Medicine: State of the Art Kogan, S. M., Beach, S. R., Philibert, R. A., Brody, G. H.,
Reviews, 1, 114. Chen, Y. F., & Lei, M. K. (2010). 5-HTTLPR status
Irwin, C. E., Jr., & Millstein, S. G. (1986). Biopsychosocial moderates the effect of early adolescent substance use
correlates of risk-taking behaviors during adolescence. on risky sexual behavior. Health Psychology, 29(5),
Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 7, 82S96S. 471476.
Irwin, C. E., Jr., & Ryan, S. A. (1989). Problem behaviors Kohler, M. P. (1996). Risk-taking behavior: A cognitive
of adolescence. Pediatrics in Review, 10, 235246. approach. Psychological Reports, 78, 489490.
Jaccard, J., & Dittus, P. (1991). Parent-teen communica- Lashbrook, J. T. (2000). Fitting in: Exploring the emo-
tion: Toward the prevention of unintended pregnan- tional dimension of adolescent peer pressure.
cies. New York: Springer. Adolescence, 35, 277294.
Jessor, R. (1976). Predicting time of onset of marijuana Manning, W. D., Longmore, M. A., & Giordano, P. C.
use: A developmental study of high school youth. (2000). The relationship context of contraceptive use
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, at first intercourse. Family Planning Perspectives,
125134. 32(2), 104110.
Jessor, R. (2008). Description versus explanation in cross- Martin, J. A., Hamilton, B. E., Sutton, P. D., et al. (2006).
national research on adolescence. Journal of Births: Final data for 2004. National Vital Statistics
Adolescent Health, 43(6), 527528. Reports, 55(1), 1101.
Jessor, R., Chase, C. A., & Donovan, J. E. (1980). McCord, J. (1990). Problem behaviors. In S. S. Feldman
Psychosocial correlates of marijuana use and problem & G. R. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing
drinking in a national sample of adolescents. American adolescent (pp. 414429). Cambridge: Harvard
Journal of Public Health, 70, 604613. University Press.
28 J.M. Sales and C.E. Irwin Jr.

Millstein, S. G., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2002). emotion in adolescence. Developmental Review, 28,
Perceptions of risk and vulnerability. Journal of 107144.
Adolescent Health, 31S, 1027. Rubinstein, M. L., Luks, T. L., Moscicki, A.-B., et al.
Newcomb, M. D., & Bentler, P. M. (1989). Substance use (2011). Smoking-related cue-induced brain activation
and abuse among children and teenagers. American in adolescent light smokers. Journal of Adolescent
Psychologist, 44, 242248. Health, 48, 712.
Newcomb, M. D., & McGee, L. (1991). Influence of sen- Sales, J. M., & Irwin, C. E., Jr. (2009). Theories of adoles-
sation seeking on general deviance and specific prob- cent risk-taking: A biopsychosocial model. In R.
lem behaviors from adolescence to adulthood. Journal DiClemente & R. Crosby (Eds.), Adolescent health:
of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 614628. Understanding and preventing risk behaviors and
Omori, M., & Ingersoll, G. M. (2005). Health-endangering adverse health outcomes (pp. 3150). San Francisco,
behaviors among Japanese college students: A test of CA: Jossey-Bass.
psychological model of risk-taking behaviors. Journal Sales, J. M., Spitalnick, J., Crittenden, C. P., & DiClemente,
of Adolescence, 28, 1733. R. J. (2009). Depression and sexual risk-taking in ado-
Orr, D. P., Wilbrandt, M. L., Brack, C. J., Rauch, S. P., & lescents. In F. Columbus (Ed.), Sexual risk-taking.
Ingersoll, G. M. (1989). Reported sexual behaviors Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
and self-esteem among young adolescents. American Santelli, J. S., Morrow, B., & Carter, M. (2004). Trends in
Journal of Diseases of Children, 143, 8690. contraceptive use among US high school students in
Park, M. J., Mulye, T. P., Adams, S. H., Brindis, C. D., & the 1990s. Journal of Adolescent Health, 34, 140.
Irwin, C. E., Jr. (2006). The health status of young Schor, E. L. (1987). Unintentional injuries: Patterns within
adults in the US. Journal of Adolescent Health, 29, families. American Journal of Disease of Children,
305317. 141, 12801284.
Pearson, M. A., & Michell, L. (2000). Smoke Rings: Shirtcliff, E. (2009). Biological underpinnings of adoles-
Social network analysis of friendship groups, smoking cent development. In R. DiClemente, J. S. Santelli, &
and drug-taking. Drugs: Education, Prevention and R. A. Crosby (Eds.), Adolescent health:
Policy, 7(1), 2137. Understanding and preventing risk behaviors.
Pedlow, C. T., & Carey, M. P. (2004). Developmentally Hoboken: Jossey-Bass.
appropriate sexual risk reduction interventions for Sisk, C. L., & Foster, D. L. (2004). The neural basis of
adolescents: Rationale, review of interventions, and puberty and adolescence. Nature Neuroscience, 7,
recommendations for research and practice. Annals of 10401047.
Behavioral Medicine, 27(3), 172184. Sisk, C. L., & Zehr, J. L. (2005). Pubertal hormones orga-
Perrin, J. S., Herve, P., Leonard, G., Perron, M., Pike, G. B., nize the adolescent brain and behavior. Frontiers in
& Pitiot, A., et al. (2008). Growth of white matter in Neuroendocrinology, 26, 163174.
the adolescent brain: Role of testosterone and andro- Smith, A. M. A., & Rosenthal, D. A. (1995). Adolescents
gen receptor. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(38), perceptions of their risk environment. Journal of
95199524. Adolescence, 18, 229245.
Peterson, S. H., Wingood, G. M., DiClemente, R. J., Snyder, H.N. (2000). Juvenile arrests, 1999. Washington,
Harrington, K., & Davies, S. (2007). Images of sexual DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency
stereotypes in rap videos and the health of African Prevention, 1.
American female adolescents. Journal of Womens Spear, L. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related
Health, 16(8), 11571164. behavioral manifestations. Neuroscience and
Phinney, V. G., Jensen, L. C., Olsen, J. A., & Cundick, B. Behavioral Reviews, 24, 417463.
(1990). The relationship between early development Spitalnick, J. S., DiClemente, R. J., Wingood, G. M.,
and psychosexual behaviors in adolescent females. Crosby, R. A., Milhausen, R. R., Sales, J. M., et al.
Adolescence, 98, 321332. (2007). Brief report: Sexual sensation seeking and its
Puzzanchera, C. (2009). Juvenile arrests 2008. U.S. relationship to risky sexual behavior among African
Department of Justice, Office of Juvenile Justice and American adolescent females. Journal of Adolescence,
Delinquency Prevention. Retrieved from http://www. 30, 165173.
ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/ojjdp/228479.pdf. Steinberg, L. (1993). Autonomy conflict and harmony in
Reyna, V., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk and rationality in ado- the family relationship. In S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliot
lescent decision-making: Implications for theory, (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent
practice, and public policy. Psychological Science in (pp. 255275). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
the Public Interest, 7, 144. Steinberg, L. (2004). Risk-taking in adolescence: What
Ricciardelli, L. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2004). A biopsy- changes, and why? Annals of the New York Academy
chosocial model of disordered eating and the pursuit of Sciences, 1021, 5158.
of muscularity in adolescent boys. Psychological Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence: New
Bulletin, 30, 179205. perspectives from brain and behavioral science.
Rivers, S. E., Reyna, V. F., & Mills, B. A. (2008). Risk Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16,
taking under the influence: A fuzzy-trace theory of 5559.
A Biopsychosocial Perspective of Adolescent Health and Disease 29

Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective Bulletin. Cincinnati: National Center for Education in
on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, Maternal and Child Health.
28(1), 78106. Windle, M. (1997). Concepts and Issues in COA Research.
Tonkin, R. S. (1987). Adolescent risk-taking behavior. Alcohol Health and ResearchWorld, 21(3), 185191.
Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 8, 213220. Working Group of the NIH Advisory Committee. (2004).
Tschann, J. M., Adler, N. E., Irwin, C. E., Jr., Millstein, S. Report of the working group of the NIH advisory
G., Turner, R. A., & Kegeles, S. M. (1994). Initiation committee to the director on research opportunities in the
of substance use in early adolescence: The roles of basic behavioral and social sciences. http://obssr.od.nih.
pubertal timing and emotional distress. Health gov/pdf/Basic%20Beh%20Report_complete.pdf.
Psychology, 13, 326333. Zenilman, J. M., Yuenger, J., Galai, N., Turner, C. F., &
Turner, R. A., Irwin, C. E., Jr., Tschann, J. M., & Millstein, Rogers, S. M. (2005). Polymerase chain reaction
S. G. (1993). Health. Psychology, 12(3), 200208. detection of Y chromosome sequences in vaginal fluid:
Udry, J. R., Talbert, L. M., & Morris, N. M. (1986). Preliminary studies of a potential biomarker for sexual
Biosocial foundations for adolescent female sexuality. behavior. Sexually Transmitted Diseases, 32(2),
Demography, 23, 217227. 9094.
Urberg, K. A., Degirmencioglu, S. M., & Pilgram, C. Zimring, F. E. (1998). American youth violence. Oxford,
(1997). Close friends and group influence on adoles- England: Oxford University Press.
cent cigarette smoking and alcohol use. Developmental Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the
Psychology, 33(5), 834844. optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Werner, M. J. (1991). Adolescent substance abuse. Zuckerman, M. (1990). The psychophysiology of sensa-
Maternal and Child Health Technical Information tion seeking. Journal of Personality, 58, 313341.
http://www.springer.com/978-1-4614-6632-1

You might also like