You are on page 1of 15

J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812

DOI 10.1007/s10802-006-9079-y

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

The Interactive Role of Anxiety Sensitivity and Pubertal Status


in Predicting Anxious Responding to Bodily Sensations among
Adolescents
Ellen W. Leen-Feldner Laura E. Reardon
Laura G. McKee Matthew T. Feldner
Kimberly A. Babson Michael J. J. Zvolensky

Published online: 18 November 2006


C Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2006

Abstract The present study examined the interaction be- Keywords adolescent . hyperventilation . pubertal status .
tween pubertal status and anxiety sensitivity (AS) in predict- anxiety sensitivity . panic vulnerability
ing anxious and fearful responding to a three-minute vol-
untary hyperventilation challenge among 124 (57 females) Puberty is a key developmental process characterized by pro-
adolescents between the ages of 12 and 17 years (Mage = found biopsychosocial changes that occur in a relatively short
15.04; SD = 1.49). As predicted, after controlling for base- period of time (Rogol, Roemmich, & Clark, 2002; Sheehy,
line anxiety, age, and gender, there was a significant inter- Gasser, Molinari, & Largo, 1999). These hormonallydriven
action between pubertal status and AS in predicting anx- changes typically co-occur with unexpected and/or undesir-
ious responding to bodily sensations to the hyperventilation able bodily events (e.g., Costos, Ackerman, & Paradis, 2002;
challenge. Specifically, adolescents reporting more advanced Omar, McElderry, & Zakharia, 2003), increased susceptibil-
pubertal status and higher levels of AS reported the great- ity to negative affect (Brooks-Gunn, Graber, & Paikoff, 1994;
est post-challenge self-reported anxiety focused on bodily Susman, Dorn, & Chrousos, 1991), and enhanced emotional
sensations, whereas pubertal status had relatively less of an lability (Buchanan, Eccles, & Becker, 1992; Spear, 2003).
effect on low AS adolescents. A test of specificity also was In addition, empirical evidence suggests an association be-
conducted; as expected, the interaction between AS and pu- tween pubertal stage and psychopathology, including eat-
bertal status was unrelated to generalized negative affectivity, ing disorders (Killen et al., 1992), substance use problems
suggesting the predictor variables interact to confer specific (Patton et al., 2004), and depression (Angold, Costello &
risk for anxious responding to bodily sensations. Finally, ex- Worthman, 1998; Patton et al., 1996). Overall, available data
ploratory analyses of psychophysiological reactivity to the indicate puberty may be important for better understanding
challenge indicated AS, but not pubertal status, moderated affective vulnerability and psychopathology. However, rela-
the relation between challenge-related change in heart-rate tively little is known about the association between puberty
and post-challenge anxiety such that high AS youth who and anxiety psychopathology.
had experienced a relatively greater heart-rate change re- Although in its relative infancy, evidence is emerging that
ported the most anxious reactivity to the challenge. Results links puberty to panic attacks. In a groundbreaking study,
are discussed in relation to theory regarding vulnerability to Hayward and colleagues (1992) administered structured clin-
anxious responding to bodily sensations among adolescents. ical interviews to determine panic attack history to an un-
screened sample (n = 754) of 6th and 7th grade females, who
self-reported pubertal status using the Tanner staging system.
E. W. Leen-Feldner () L. E. Reardon M. T. Feldner K. A.
Babson Findings from this cross-sectional analysis indicated a two-
University of Arkansas, Department of Psychology, fold increase in the likelihood of having had a panic attack for
216 Memorial Hall, Fayetteville, AR, 72701 each one-point change in Tanner stage. In a follow-up study,
e-mail: eleenfe@uark.edu Hayward and colleagues (1997) examined pubertal timing in
L. G. McKee M. J. Zvolensky relation to panic attack status among 758 adolescent females
University of Vermont, Department of Psychology, across a six-year period. Compared to their on-time coun-
John Dewey Hall, Burlington, VT 05405 terparts, early maturing girls were significantly more likely

Springer
800 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812

to have experienced a panic attack. In addition, an interac- Anxiety sensitivity (AS) is a well established panic-relevant
tion was observed such that, relative to the rest of the sam- individual difference variable reflecting a cognitive vulner-
ple, early maturing females with a history of internalizing ability to experience fear in the context of bodily arousal
symptoms had the highest overall risk of developing a clin- (Barlow, 2002; McNally, 1990; Reiss & McNally, 1985).
ical disorder (including panic disorder). These data suggest At least four lines of research support the conceptualiza-
that puberty is related to panic attacks and that individual dif- tion of AS as a panic-relevant variable risk factor (Kraemer,
ference factors may interact with pubertal status to increase Lowe, & Kupfer, 2005; Zvolensky, Schmidt, Bernstein, &
panic-relevant vulnerability. Keogh, 2006). First, AS predicts anxious and fearful re-
Although promising, the extant work on puberty and panic sponding to biological challenge procedures among youth,
vulnerability currently is limited in at least two regards. First, even when conceptually-relevant variables such as trait anxi-
existing work has thus far focused on psychologically un- ety and negative affectivity are accounted for (Leen-Feldner,
screened adolescents (i.e., youth with psychopathology have Feldner, Bernstein, McCormick, & Zvolensky, 2005; Rabian,
not been excluded). As a result, it is not clear whether pre- Embry, & McIntyre, 1999; Unnewehr, Schneider, Margraf,
existing psychopathology played a role in the documented Jenkins, & Florin, 1996). Second, AS is associated with panic
puberty-panic association. For example, given the link be- symptomatology, even after controlling for symptoms of
tween puberty and other mental health problems (e.g., Patton general anxiety and depression (Deacon, Valentiner, Gutier-
et al., 2004), it may be that psychopathology (e.g., substance rez, & Blacker, 2002), and is elevated among adolescents
abuse/dependence, clinical levels of depression), rather than with panic attacks (Ginsburg & Drake, 2002; Lau, Calamari,
pubertal status per se, may at least partially explain the & Waraczynski, 1996) and panic disorder (Kearney, Albano,
puberty-panic linkage. To address this methodological lim- Eisen, Allan, & Barlow, 1997) compared to those without
itation of previous studies, investigations need to focus on such histories. Third, work with youth (Ollendick, 1995) and
adolescents who have not yet developed psychopathology; adults (Telch et al., 1993; Smits, Powers, Cho, & Telch, 2004)
in this way, observed findings can be more confidently at- indicates AS is sensitive to change following clinical inter-
tributed to the predictor variables of interest rather than vention for panic-related problems. Finally, work completed
factors related to psychopathology (Forsyth & Zvolensky, with adults (Li & Zinbarg, in press; Maller & Reiss, 1992;
2002; Zvolensky, Lejeuz, Stuart, & Curtin, 2001). Overall, Schmidt, Lerew, & Jackson, 1997; 1999) and adolescents
this methodological approach is clinically-relevant and the- (Hayward, Killen, Kraemer, & Taylor, 2000) indicates AS
oretically useful, as the potential confound associated with prospectively predicts the development of panic attacks and
pre-existing psychopathology is delimited, thereby allowing related anxiety disorders. For instance, among non-clinical
researchers to interpret findings in terms of risk-factor (eti- adults with no history of panic, AS scores in the upper half
ological) processes. Such data inform our understanding of of the sample distribution were associated with a two-fold
vulnerability processes among youth who have not yet devel- increase in the likelihood of developing panic attacks, even
oped panic psychopathology and help to specify for whom after controlling for trait anxiety (Schmidt, Zvolensky, &
or under what conditions panic problems develop (Mrazek & Maner, 2006). Collectively, evidence across methodological
Haggerty, 1994). approaches indicates elevations in AS are reliably associated
Second, little is known about the variables that may mod- with an increased likelihood of developing panic problems.
erate the association between puberty and panic vulnerabil- In the context of puberty, AS may represent a key vul-
ity. The relative neglect of this empirical question is impor- nerability factor that may interact with the biopsychoso-
tant, particularly since puberty is a normative (Rosenfeld & cial changes that occur during puberty to potentiate panic-
Nicodemus, 2003) developmental process. Thus, it is not relevant learning. That is, compared to low AS youth, adoles-
likely that changes associated with puberty alone are cents with elevated AS may perceive the bodily events that
pathogenic in their own right, but rather, increase risk for occur during puberty as personally threatening and anxiety-
psychopathology only under certain conditions (e.g., among provoking and thereby be exposed to more frequent and in-
psychologically vulnerable adolescents; Buchanan et al., tense learning trials, wherein they are learning to fear bodily
1992). This conceptualization is consistent with extant the- sensations, thereby setting the stage for panic development.
ory and research on affective vulnerability (e.g., accentuation Over time, the repeated pairing of bodily sensations and anx-
hypothesis; Caspi & Moffit, 1991; 1993); specific individual iety about such sensations may result in a learned association
differences may be critical to understanding why puberty is between these two phenomena, such that bodily sensations
characterized by heightened vulnerability for psychopathol- ultimately come to elicit fear-relevant affective states. This
ogy among some adolescents. conceptualization is drawn from contemporary theoretical
In light of these limitations, we sought to examine the models of panic development, where bodily sensations be-
moderating role of anxiety sensitivity on the puberty-panic come associated with fearful responding, ultimately resulting
association among psychologically healthy adolescents. in the development of anxious reactivity to such sensations

Springer
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812 801

(Barlow, 2002; Bouton, Mineka, & Barlow, 2001). Although new research in this domain, we conducted some exploratory
theoretically promising, empirical tests of this perspective analyses of physiological reactivity to the hyperventilation
are scant. challenge (i.e., changes in heart-rate).
The objective of this investigation was to examine the
interaction between pubertal status and AS in predicting
anxious responding to a voluntary hyperventilation chal- Method
lenge among psychologically healthy adolescents. A bio-
logical challenge procedure was utilized because it allows Participants
for a controlled, real-time assessment of multimodal (e.g.,
self-report; physiological) responding. This approach elim- Adolescents between the ages of 12 and 17 years (M =
inates exclusive reliance on self-report, which, particularly 15.04; SD = 1.49) were recruited from the general commu-
in regard to the reporting of emotional states/processes, is nity. The primary means of advertising was flyers that alerted
subject to recall biases and other types of reporting er- adolescents to a study being conducted at a northeastern
rors (McNally, 1999). In addition, this procedure: 1) has university that involved a breathing exercise, an interview,
been safely used with youth in the past (Unnewehr et al., completion of questionnaires, and compensation for partic-
1996), 2) is a non-invasive challenge controlled voluntar- ipation. These fliers were placed in the community (e.g., in
ily by participants, 3) produces responses that remit imme- arcades, primary care offices, adolescent-oriented commu-
diately upon return to normal breathing, and 4) is a rec- nity centers) and advertisement booths also were set up in a
ommended treatment strategy for children and adolescents local well-traveled marketplace on two separate occasions,
with PD (Bouchard, Mendlowitz, Coles, & Franklin, 2004). approximately two months apart.
Moreover, prospective research indicates respiratory-based One hundred twenty four adolescents (57 females) par-
challenges do not increase risk of panic-type problems (e.g., ticipated in the present study.1 The racial distribution of
the sample reflected that of the local population (State of
Harrington, Schmidt, & Telch, 1996; Perna, Cocchi, Politi, &
Vermont Department of Health, 2000): 90.3% (n = 112)
Bellodi, 1997; Prenoveau, Forsyth, Kelly, & Barrios, in
Caucasian; 3.2% (n = 4) African American; 0.8% (n = 1)
press). Overall, the potential gains in knowledge far out-
Native Hawaiian; 0.8% (n = 1) Asian; 0.8% (n = 1) Amer-
weigh the temporary discomfort that may result from study
ican Indian; 0.8% (n = 1) other; and 3.2% (n = 4) did
procedures.
not specify race. On average, participants had completed 9.91
Three hypotheses guided the investigation. First, in terms
years of education (SD = 1.56). Parents (Mage = 46.44 [SD:
of predicting post-challenge self-reported anxiety, a main
5.99]; 91 females) provided additional demographic data
effect of AS was expected. A main effect of pubertal status
for 113 participants; 97 respondents were currently married
on post-challenge anxiety was not predicted because the-
or living with someone; 1 was widowed, 12 were divorced
oretically, the changes associated with puberty would not
or separated, and 3 were never married. In terms of total
be expected to potentiate panic responding among healthy
family income, 20 reported less than $50,000/year; 52 be-
adolescents per se, but rather only among those already at
tween $50,000 and $100,000/year; 21 between $101,000 and
risk by virtue of elevated levels of AS. Therefore, it was
expected that pubertal status would predict post-challenge
1
anxiety among adolescents high in AS, whereas pubertal In light of the sensitive nature of questions on the Tanner stage ques-
status would demonstrate less of a relation to such anxiety tionnaire, participants were reminded before completing the question-
naire battery that they were free to skip any questions that made them
among individuals low in AS. feel uncomfortable. Of the 151 adolescents recruited, 124 completed
Second, we conducted a discriminative test of our primary the Tanner stage questionnaire. Analyses indicated that there were no
hypothesis. Specifically, we examined the specificity of the significant differences between adolescents who completed the Tan-
AS x pubertal stage interaction for predicting panic-type ner stage questionnaire and those who did not with respect to age,
gender, anxiety sensitivity, or challenge responding.To delimit unnec-
responding versus general levels of negative affectivity. This essary participant burden, adolescents were apprised of exclusionary
analysis aimed to provide an initial test of whether, given criteria upon contacting the laboratory. Specifically, it was explained
support for our first hypothesis, the interaction between AS that participants would be excluded for any of the six conditions de-
and pubertal status is predictive of panic-type responding scribed in the narrative (e.g., if a doctor ever diagnosed you with a
mental health problem or gave you medication for it or if you have
specifically, or whether pubertal youth high in AS are at an a bad cold right now). Thereafter, the participant was asked whether
increased risk for negative affect in general. This test seemed he/she was still interested in the study. Thirty-three adolescents (17 fe-
particularly relevant in light of established relations between males) who contacted the laboratory reported that they did not meet the
pubertal status and negative affectivity (e.g., Brooks-Gunn inclusionary criteria; specific reasons for ineligibility were not recorded
and no additional information (e. g., age) was gathered. An additional
et al., 1994). It was not expected that the AS x pubertal nine participants (5 females) were excluded on the basis of ADIS-C
status interaction term would relate to self-reported levels of responses (primary diagnoses: 6 past or current depression; 2 social
negative affectivity. Finally, for the purposes of generating phobia; 1 specific phobia).

Springer
802 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812

$150,000/year; 10 more than $150,000/year; and 10 did not Tanner stages provide a reliable index of secondary sexual
specify. characteristics and, as such, reflect the two major hormonal
Participants were screened for psychopathology using the events associated with pubertal onset; adrenarche and go-
Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for the Diagnostic and nadarche (Fechner, 2002). This approach is standard in pu-
Statistical Manual Fourth Edition (DSM-IV): Child Ver- berty assessment and is more reliable than other methods
sion (ADIS-C; Silverman & Albano, 1996). This standard- (e.g., retrospective recall of menarche). Broader develop-
ized assessment strategy allows for the clinical evaluation of mental indices (e.g., age) were not used because they do not
Axis I psychopathology related to anxiety and mood states necessarily reflect pubertal status (e.g., age at pubertal onset
and ensures a conservative evaluation of the exclusionary cri- varies across youth, for instance, female breast development
teria. Research personnel were trained to mastery in the use typically begins anywhere from ages 8 to 13; Tanner, 1962)
of the instrument; training involved intensive didactic ses- and hence, are not necessarily valid tools in the assessment
sions, direct observation of administrations, and diagnostic of puberty (Dorn, Dahl, Woodward, & Biro, 2006). Sub-
comparison until 100% diagnostic reliability was reached. jects provided ratings of pubertal stage based on the Tan-
In addition, ongoing supervision (e.g., to resolve diagnostic ner staging system (Morris & Udry, 1980). Each participant
questions) was provided throughout the study. In addition, was shown sex-appropriate drawings depicting specific sec-
medical history was assessed with a brief semi-structured ondary sexual characteristics across the course of puberty
screening interview (e.g., Do you have asthma?) that we (i.e., breasts and pubic hair in girls; genitalia and pubic hair
developed and used successfully in previous work with ado- in boys). Participants were asked to choose the drawing that
lescents (Leen-Feldner et al., 2005). Based upon their re- most looks like you from each set of five drawings. Self-
sponse to the ADIS-C, none of the participants had current ratings were averaged to generate a single score ranging
or past Axis I psychopathology. Additional exclusionary cri- from Tanner stage I (immature) to Tanner stage V (mature)
teria for the present study included: (a) current or past car- for each participant. This instrument is commonly used in
diopulmonary (chronic) illness (e.g., chronic obstructive pul- puberty research (e.g., Hayward et al., 1992; Wilson et al.,
monary disease; asthma); (b) current acute respiratory illness 1991) and data support the validity of this assessment strat-
(e.g., bronchitis); (c) current or past psychotropic medication egy. For instance, Duke, Litt, & Gross (1980) reported good
use; (d) suicidality; and (e) limited mental competency and convergent validity between this measure and physician rat-
the inability to give informed, written consent/assent. Such ings of pubertal stage, with kappa coefficients ranging from
exclusionary criteria have been used successfully in past bi- 0.81 to 0.88. In terms of construct validity, self-reported
ological challenge work with adults (Feldner et al., 2006), Tanner stage is positively correlated with physiological pro-
and aside from avoiding medical complications and risks that cesses known to occur during puberty, such as increases in
might arise from the procedure, they serve to reduce potential sex steroid concentrations (reflected by increased levels of
confounds introduced by individual difference factors known Insulin-Like Growth Factor I; Wilson et al., 1994) . Cron-
to affect responding to the hyperventilation procedure. bachs alphas for the current sample are: Female Drawings =
.74; Male Drawings = .71.
Measures The negative affectivity subscale of the Positive and
Negative Affect Schedule for Children (PANAS-C; Wilson,
Pre-challenge assessment Gullone, & Moss, 1998) was used to assess a generalized
propensity to experience negative affectivity. For each of 15
The 18-item Childhood Anxiety Sensitivity Index (CASI; descriptors on the PANAS-C, participants were instructed
Silverman, Fleisig, Rabian, & Peterson, 1991), a modifi- to indicate to what extent you usually feel this way, that
cation of the adult 16-item Anxiety Sensitivity Index (Reiss, is, how you feel on average on a 5-point Likert-type scale
Peterson, Gursky, & McNally, 1986), was used to assess AS. (1 = very slightly to 5 = extremely). The PANAS-C
Participants rated their perceptions of the aversive nature was selected as the outcome variable for the discriminant
of anxiety symptoms by endorsing 1 (none), 2 (some), or test in the current study because it is nonspecific; it covers a
3 (a lot) in response to questions such as it scares me when wide range of negative affective states (e.g., anxiety; sadness;
my heart beats fast. This scale was adapted for, and validated anger). As such, it represents a particularly conservative test
with, a population of children and adolescents. The scale has of the study hypothesis that AS and pubertal status will re-
good psychometric properties (e.g., Cronbachs alpha for the late specifically to panic-type states and not to general levels
total scale in present sample = 0.82) and satisfactory valid- of negative affectivity. In addition, the PANAS-C is psycho-
ity estimates (Silverman et al., 1991). metrically sound; it demonstrates good internal consistency
The Tanner staging system was selected as the index of pu- (e.g., = .80 for the negative affectivity scale in the present
bertal status. Although there is no gold standard in terms sample), as well as adequate discriminant and convergent
of measuring pubertal development (Hayward, 2003), the validity (Joiner, Catanzaro, & Laurent, 1996).

Springer
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812 803

Challenge assessment tory. As part of the informed consent/assent process, partic-


ipants and parental guardians were fully informed about the
Self-reported anxiety was indexed using a Subjective Units side effects of the challenge procedure, including the pos-
of Distress Scale (SUDS; Wolpe, 1958), which ranged from sibility of having a panic attack, and that participants had
0 (no anxiety) to 100 (extreme anxiety), and was ad- the right to discontinue participation at any time without
ministered during a baseline period and immediately post- penalty (no participant withdrew from the study). Next, a
challenge. This is a well-established assessment of self- doctoral level graduate student, trained in administration of
reported anxiety in studies with adolescents (e.g., Pine et al., the ADIS-C and directly supervised by Dr. Leen-Feldner,
2000). Psychophysiological responding was indexed using a conducted the medical exclusionary screenings and psychi-
J & J Engineering I-330-C2 system, which recorded phys- atric interviews. Exclusionary criteria were rigorously as-
iological data on-line at a sample rate of 1024 samples per sessed, with any ambiguous responses (e.g., I dont know)
second across all channels using J & J Engineering Physiolab coded as a positive endorsement. Biological challenge pro-
Software. Raw electrocardiogram data were collected with cedures were conducted in accordance with comprehensive
disposable Ag/AgCl electrodes placed in a standard bilateral and strict laboratory policies, which include in-depth and on-
configuration on the palmar side of each wrist. Respiration going training in the safe use of the procedure. In addition,
rate (breaths-per-minute) was sampled using a Pnemograph challenge administrators were carefully trained in the man-
sensor cable with two elastic PS-2 sensors filled with con- agement of adverse reactions. Specifically, participants were
ductive fluid. Stretching across the sensors causes a voltage told they may give a simple indicator (e.g., wave) if they
change, thereby providing a gross measure of excursion of wish to stop the procedure. In this event, or if the adminis-
the chest during respiration. Respiration rate was examined trator has any reason to believe the participant is becoming
to validate the efficacy of the breathing rate manipulation. upset (each participant is monitored throughout the proce-
As is standard in our laboratory, physiological data were dure via a closed-caption television system), the procedure is
screened for outliers prior to data analysis. stopped immediately. The administrator is trained in the use
of cognitive-behavioral methods of anxiety reduction (e.g.,
Laboratory Challenge relaxation training) to help participants relax if they are upset
and participants do not leave the laboratory until they have
A well-established voluntary hyperventilation procedure was returned to a relaxed state. In addition, licensed child clin-
selected as the challenge induction because this laboratory- ical psychologists, housed in the University Psychological
based procedure allows for the modeling of panic-like expe- Clinic across the hall from the laboratory, were on-hand to
riences (e.g., abrupt increases in bodily arousal) in a safe and offer consultation services if necessary. However, it is im-
well-controlled manner (see Zvolensky & Eifert, 2000, for portant to note that we did not have a single instance where
a discussion). Specifically, this procedure has been shown an adverse reaction occurred or consultation was necessary.
to be reliable and safe when used with children and adoles- Eligible participants first completed the pre-experimental
cents (Leen-Feldner et al., 2005; Unnewehr et al., 1996), its assessments. Thereafter, participants were seated in the sub-
parametric properties are well studied, and it reliably pro- ject testing room; an 8ft 12ft sound attenuated experimen-
duces bodily arousal that mimics anxiety and panic attack tal room. Electrodes were attached, the experimenter left the
symptoms (Hornsveld, Garssen, Dop, & van Spiegel, 1990). subject testing room, and the participant sat quietly for a
The challenge involved a three-minute hyperventilation with five-minute adaptation/baseline period, which is consistent
a breathing rate of 30 respiratory cycles/min; this rate has with prior studies utilizing the hyperventilation procedure
been shown to produce a decrease in pCO2 , an elevation in with adolescents (e.g., Unnewehr et al., 1996), so that he/she
blood pH, and increases in plasma epinephrine and lactate could adjust to the laboratory context. Audiotaped directions
levels (Fried & Grimaldi, 1993). To ensure consistency of were then played for participants that described the hyper-
breathing rates across the duration of the interval and across ventilation procedure as follows:
subjects, audiotaped instructions were used to standardize
participant breathing tempo (see below for details). You will now be guided through a deep breathing ex-
ercise. In this exercise, you will be asked to breathe in
General Laboratory Layout and Procedure and breathe out very deeply. The instructions will tell
you when you should breathe in and when you should
The current study was approved by the University Insti- breathe out. Simply inhale when asked to breathe in,
tutional Review Board and ethical guidelines for research and exhale when asked to breathe out making each
with children and adolescents were adhered to throughout breath in as deep as possible and each breath out as
all stages of the project. Specifically, informed written con- forceful as possible. It is important that you follow these
sent (for child participation) from parents and written assent instructions as best as you can, and continue the exercise
from adolescents was obtained upon arrival at the labora- until you are asked to stop and rest.
Springer
804 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812

Post-challenge SUDS (anxiety) ratings were collected individual differences in pre-challenge levels of anxiety as
directly following the hyperventilation challenge and theoretically important2 .
physiological data were gathered continuously throughout
the adaptation and challenge periods. The subject and exper-
Results
imenter rooms were equipped with a closed circuit television
so the experimenter could monitor participant behavior and
Means and standard deviations for predictor and criterion
ensure that the depth of breathing was comparable for all
variables are reported in Table 1. CASI scores for the current
participants. Supporting the efficacy of the procedure, results
sample are consistent with previous findings (e.g., Kearney
indicated that the hyperventilation challenge significantly in-
et al., 1997; Silverman et al., 1991). For instance, scores ob-
creased breaths per minute (Mbaseline = 18.31; SD = 2.15;
served in the current study (M = 23.98; SD = 4.49) were
Mchallenge = 25.71; SD = 3.31; t = 20.20, p < . 01) from
lower compared to youth with panic disorder (M = 34.58;
baseline to challenge. At the end of the study, the partic-
SD = 8.06), other non-panic anxiety disorders (M = 28.32;
ipant and his or her parental guardian were compensated
SD = 9.73), and various psychiatric diagnoses (e.g., M =
$50 for their efforts. In addition, all participants took part
28.07; SD = 7.01; Silverman et al., 1991), and similar to
in our comprehensive debriefing procedure, which included
unscreened, non-clinical samples (M = 26.53; SD = 5.81).
defining anxiety sensitivity, psychoeducation about panic
Post-challenge anxiety ratings and mean challenge heart rate
and bodily sensations, and a newsletter describing specific
also were comparable to those observed in studies of youth
study findings and details about the scientific research pro-
using similar methodologies (e.g., Unnewehr et al., 1996).
cess (mailed to participants at the conclusion of data collec-
In terms of zero-order correlations among the predictor and
tion; the newsletter is available from the first author upon
demographic variables (see Table 1), AS and Tanner stage
request).
related positively to age, and Tanner stage related positively
to female gender. No other significant associations emerged
General Analytic Approach
among predictor and demographic variables. In regard to
challenge responding, as predicted, AS related positively to
Separate hierarchical multiple regression analyses were con-
post-challenge anxiety ratings and Tanner stage was not sig-
ducted to examine the study hypotheses. The model was
nificantly correlated with this outcome variable. Also, there
divided into three levels: (1) gender, age, and baseline levels
were no significant relations among AS, Tanner stage, and
of anxiety were entered at level one; (2) main effects (e.g., to-
physiological responsivity to the challenge. Finally, in line
tal CASI scores and Tanner stage) were entered at level two,
with expectations, pubertal status correlated positively with
and (3) the interaction term was entered at level three. Each
negative affectivity (r = .32); AS also was related to this
variable that met criteria for entry into the model (p = .05 to
variable. Notably, CASI scores and Tanner stage shared little
enter) was retained. Squared semi-partial correlations (sr2 )
variance with one another (i.e., 3.6%), supporting the con-
were used as indices of effect size for those variables retained
ceptualization that they are each tapping distinct processes.
in the equation (Cohen, 1988). Main effect variables were
mean-centered prior to computing product terms (Aiken &
West, 1991). Overall, this analytic approach provides a test
2
of incremental validity and ensures that any observed asso- The current study is the first direct test of the relation between pu-
ciations between the interaction term and criterion variables berty and panic-relevant responding among both male and female ado-
lescents. Although the specific events of puberty differ across gen-
are above and beyond the variance accounted for by fac-
ders (e.g., differences in the amount and type of hormones released;
tors at levels one or two in the equation (Cohen & Cohen, Fechner, 2002), and the risk for internalizing-type problems is higher
1983; Sechrest, 1963). In the case of significant interactions, for post-pubertal girls (Hayward & Sanborn, 2002), the current model
the form of the interaction was graphically depicted in ac- is predicated on the notion that the co-occurrence of bodily perturba-
tion and fearful reactivity to such sensations may potentiate panic-type
cordance with the recommendations of Cohen and Cohen responding. These general features are characteristic of puberty across
(1983). Finally, based on recommendations of Holmbeck the sexes (Buchanan et al., 1992), and therefore, we contend that the
(2002), post-hoc probing analyses were conducted on the role of puberty in panic attack development may be similar for males
data to examine significant moderational effects. and females. Indeed, Craske (2003) points out that the frequency of
prodromal panic symptomatology such as panic attacks does not dif-
In each model, age, gender, and pre-experimental anxiety fer as a function of gender; rather, girls are more likely to engage in
were selected as covariates on an a priori basis. Specifi- avoidance-type behavior, increasing the likelihood of panic disorder
cally, in line with our conceptualization and the approach development. Nonetheless, the role of gender is an important element
taken in previous reports (Graber, Seeley, Brooks-Gunn, & of the model that merits empirical attention. In the present study, we
were underpowered to test hypotheses in separate genders (a test of
Lewinsohn, 2004; Hayward et al., 1992; Rabian et al., 1999), the triple interaction was non-significant). Instead, we elected to treat
we viewed the isolation of the effects of the predictor vari- gender as a co-variate in this initial test of the model and highlight the
ables relative to chronological age, participant gender, and issue of gender as paramount for future research in the area.

Springer
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812 805

Table 1 Descriptive Data for Predictor and Criterion Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 M (SD)

Predictor Variables
1. Age (in years) .10 .03 .14 .28 .53 .09 .22 .35 15.04 (1.49)
2. Gender .10 .08 .05 .30 .15 .11 .10 57
3. Baseline anxiety (SUDS) ratings .12 .17 .02 .41 .01 .19 9.01 (16.47)
4. Baseline mean heart-rate .18 .12 .10 .56 .06 74.70 (9.36)
5. CASI total score .19 .25 .15 .40 23.98 (4.49)
6. Tanner stage .14 .07 .32 4.21 (.72)
Criterion Variables
7. Post-challenge anxiety (SUDS) ratings .08 .29 22.87 (20.42)
8. Mean challenge heart-rate .10 90.82 (14.02)
9. PANAS-C: negative affect subscale score 12.87 (4.15)

Note. N = 124. Gender was dummy coded (0 = male; 1 = female); the number listed for gender reflects number of girls in the sample; SUDS:
Subjective Units of Distress Scale (Wolpe, 1958); CASI: Child Anxiety Sensitivity Index (Silverman et al., 1991); Tanner stage: self-reported
Tanner stages of pubertal development (Morris & Udry, 1980; Tanner, 1962); mean challenge responding scores were derived by averaging heart-
rate across the three-minute challenge period; PANAS-C: Positive and Negative Affect Scale for Children; (Wilson et al., 1998). = p < .05,

= p < .01.

To assess the degree to which the hyperventilation chal- no main effect of Tanner stage. At level three, the interac-
lenge elicited self-reported anxiety and physiological arousal tion term accounted for additional variance (R2 = .03,
we examined changes in SUDS ratings and heart rate. = .19, t = 2.27, p < .05, sr2 = .03). As can be seen in
Paired samples t-tests suggested baseline self-reported anx- Figure 1, the nature of the interaction was consistent with
iety (M = 9.01, SD = 16.47) and heart rate (M = 74.70, prediction. Specifically, among individuals of advanced pu-
SD = 9.36) significantly increased in response to the hy- bertal stage, greater AS was associated with increased anx-
perventilation challenge (M = 22.87, SD = 20.42; M = ious responding to the challenge, whereas pubertal status did
90.82, SD = 14.02, respectively) period [t (self-reported not appear as strongly related to anxiety ratings among in-
anxiety) = 7.56, p < .01; t (heart rate) = 16.12, p < .01]. dividuals low in AS (see Figure 1). Post hoc probing of this
These data suggest the hyperventilation procedure effec- moderational effect indicated the simple slope for the low
tively elicited a panic-relevant response. AS regression line was not significantly different from zero
Results of the regression analyses are presented in Table 2. (t = .04, ns), whereas the simple slope for the high AS re-
With respect to post-challenge SUDS (anxiety) ratings, the gression line was significant (t = 2.78, p < .01; Holmbeck,
predictor set at the first level in the model accounted for a 2002). These analyses support hypotheses, suggesting post-
significant amount of variance (R2 = .19, p < .01). At this challenge anxiety ratings are higher among adolescents at
level only pre-experimental anxiety was significantly associ- more advanced pubertal stage when AS is also high.
ated with post-challenge anxiety ratings ( = .40, p < .01) Next, in regard to negative affectivity, predictors at level
such that higher levels of pre-experimental anxiety predicted one of the model accounted for a significant amount of vari-
higher levels of post-challenge anxiety. After controlling for ance (R2 = .13, p < .01). Here, only age related positively to
age3 , gender, and pre-experimental anxiety, the predictor set negative affectivity ( = .34, p < .01) such that increased
at the second level in the model accounted for additional age predicted elevated levels of negative affectivity. Level
variance (R2 = .05, p < .05). As predicted, there was a two of the model also was significant (R2 = .12, p <
main effect of AS on post-challenge anxiety ( = .18, p < .05); there was a main effect of AS ( = .34, p < .01) such
.05) such that increased CASI scores predicted increased that increased CASI scores predicted increased negative af-
anxiety following the hyperventilation procedure. There was fectivity. There was not a main effect of pubertal status4 .
Finally, as predicted, the interaction did not predict negative
3 affectivity (see Table 2).
A reviewer made the interesting suggestion that observed findings
may be due to the effects of age, rather than pubertal status. Specif-
ically, compared to younger youth, older adolescents may evidence
greater awareness of, and ability to report, interoceptive states and/or 4
Post hoc exploration of the association between pubertal status and
be more sensitive to the demand characteristics of the testing situation. negative affectivity indicated that when pubertal status was entered by
To explore this alternative explanation, we examined the interaction itself at level two of the model, it predicted negative affectivity above
between anxiety sensitivity (AS) and age in predicting post-challenge and beyond variance accounted for by age and gender. However, as
anxiety. After controlling for gender, pre-experimental anxiety, and the can be seen in Table 2, when CASI scores were entered simultaneously
main effects of AS and age, the interaction was not predictive of SUDs with pubertal status at level two, pubertal status no longer predicted
anxiety ratings (R2 = .00, = .77, t = .62, p = .56). negative affectivity.

Springer
806 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812

Table 2 Anxiety Sensitivity,


Pubertal Status, and Heart-Rate R2 t (each predictor) sr2 p (F)
Change Predicting
Dependent Variable: Post-Challenge Anxiety Ratings
Post-Challenge Anxiety Ratings
and Negative Affectivity Step 1 .19 < .01
Age (in years) 1.33 .11 .01 ns
Gender 1.24 .11 .01 ns
Baseline Anxiety (SUDS) 4.72 .40 .16 < .01
Step 2 .05 < .05
CASI-total score 2.09 .18 .03 < .05
Tanner stage 1.62 .16 .02 ns
Step 3 .03 < .05
CASI-total score x Tanner stage 2.27 .19 .03 < .05
Dependent Variable: PANAS-C negative affectivity subscale total score
Step 1 .13 < .01
Age (in years) 3.89 .34 .11 < .01
Gender .85 .07 .00 ns
Step 2 .12 < .01
CASI-total score 4.00 .34 .11 < .01
Tanner stage 1.31 .13 .01 < .01
Step 3 .02 ns
CASI-total score x Tanner stage 1.92 .16 .02 ns
Dependent Variable: Post-Challenge Anxiety Ratings
Step 1 .18 < .01
Age (in years) 1.48 .12 .01 ns
Gender 1.08 .09 .00 ns
Baseline Anxiety (SUDS) 4.53 .39 .15 < .01
Step 2 .08 < .01
CASI-total score 1.95 .17 .02 < .05
Heart Rate Change 2.87 .24 .05 < .01
Note: N = 124. = Step 3 .03 < .05
standardized beta weight.
CASI-total score x Heart Rate Change 2.28 1.19 .03 < .05
Gender was dummy coded (0 =
male; 1 = female); SUDS: Dependent Variable: Post-Challenge Anxiety Ratings
Subjective Units of Distress Step 1 .18 < .01
Scale (Wolpe, 1958); CASI: Age (in years) 1.24 .10 .00 ns
Child Anxiety Sensitivity Index Gender 1.22 .10 .00 ns
(Silverman et al., 1991); Tanner Baseline Anxiety (SUDS) 4.45 .38 .14 < .01
stage: self-reported Tanner Step 2 .07 < .01
stages of pubertal development Tanner stage 1.81 .18 .02 ns
(Morris & Udry, 1980; Tanner, Heart Rate Change 2.52 .21 .04 < .05
1962); PANAS-C: Positive and
Step 3 .03 ns
Negative Affect Scale for
Tanner Stage x Heart Rate Change 1.55 .73 .01 ns
Children; (Wilson et al., 1998).

Theoretically-Derived Exploratory Analyses the puberty by heart-rate change interaction term was non-
significant (see Table 2). However, in the AS model, the
Finally, we explored psychophysiological reactivity to the predictor set at the first level accounted for a significant
challenge. Specifically, we examined AS and pubertal status amount of variance (R2 = .18, p < .01). At this level only
as independent moderators of the relation between heart- pre-experimental anxiety predicted post-challenge anxiety
rate change5 and post-challenge anxiety. The model with ratings ( = .39, p < .01). After controlling for age, gen-
der, and pre-experimental anxiety, the predictor set at the
second level in the model accounted for additional variance
5
We also examined the study hypotheses in terms of heart rate vari- (R2 = .08, p < .05). There was a main effect of AS ( =
ability (HRV). Specifically, the J & J Engineering software we use .17, p < .05) and heart-rate change ( = .24, p < .01);
provides power values for the high frequency (HF) and low frequency these were qualified by a significant interaction at level three
(LF) components of the power spectra; consistent with extant research
(e.g., Friedman & Thayer, 1998), HF power and the LF/HF ratio (as
an index of cardiac autonomic balance) were employed as the index tween challenge-related changes in HRV and anxious reactivity to the
of HRV. Neither AS nor pubertal status moderated the association be- voluntary hyperventilation.

Springer
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812 807

40

35.75
35

30
Post-Challenge SUDS Rating

26
25

20 Low AS
High AS
17 16.58
15

10

0
Low Tanner High Tanner
Tanner Stage
Fig. 1. Post-challenge anxiety ratings, as a function of pubertal status man et al., 1991) among participants one-half of a standard deviation
(measured using the Tanner staging system; Morris & Udry, 1980) and above and/or below the mean for each predictor.
anxiety sensitivity (indexed via the Anxiety Sensitivity Index; Silver-

(R2 = .03, = 1.19, t = 2.28, p < .05, sr2 = .03). A high AS adolescents endorsing more advanced pubertal sta-
graphical depiction of the interaction is presented in Figure 2; tus reported the greatest overall post-challenge anxiety. In
among youth who experienced a greater change in heart-rate contrast, pubertal status had relatively less of an effect on
in response to the challenge, greater AS was associated with low AS adolescents (see Figure 1). These findings support the
increased anxious responding to the challenge, whereas heart conceptualization that AS and pubertal status interact to in-
rate change did not appear as strongly related to anxiety rat- crease vulnerability to anxious responding to bodily arousal.
ings among individuals low in AS (see Figure 2). Post hoc Thus, bodily changes related to puberty among youth with
probing of this effect supports this interpretation; the simple pre-existing panic vulnerability (i.e., high AS), may provide
slope for the low AS regression line was not significantly dif- a critical context in which panic-relevant learning occurs
ferent from zero (t = .33, ns), whereas the simple slope (Bouton et al., 2001). Interestingly, recent data suggest AS
for the high AS regression line was significant (t = 3.47, relates positively to catastrophic misinterpretation of internal
p < .01; Holmbeck, 2002). somatic cues among youth in grades 3, 6, and 9 (Mattis &
Ollendick, 1997). Findings from the current study coalesce
well with these data; among adolescents high in AS, the
Discussion events of puberty may provide a fecund context for expe-
riencing fear in the context of somatic arousal, ultimately
To date, no study has examined the interactive role of pu- setting the stage for a panic attack. Future research may
bertal status and AS in predicting panic vulnerability among benefit by examining other features of puberty that may po-
adolescents. The present study sought to address this gap tentiate panic development (e.g., timing; Graber, Lewinsohn,
in the literature. As predicted, and consistent with the ex- Seeley, & Brooks-Gunn, 1997). Importantly, post-challenge
tant literature linking AS to a number of anxiety-relevant anxiety results cannot be attributed to Axis I psychopathol-
outcomes (Silverman & Weems, 1999), total CASI scores ogy or individual difference factors, including state anxiety,
related positively to post-challenge anxiety ratings. Also age, and gender. Although the size of the observed effect
consistent with hypotheses, there was no main effect of pu- was small in overall magnitude at 3% of uniquely explained
bertal status on post-challenge anxiety. This supports our variance, it is important to consider the larger context when
contention that puberty per se does not increase panic re- considering the potential significance of this finding. Specif-
activity. Rather, pubertal stage interacted with AS such that ically, a relatively large amount of variance was explained

Springer
808 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812

60

55

50
Post-Challenge SUDS Ratings

40

Low AS
30
High AS

20 19.75

10 9.5

0 0
Low Heart Rate Change High Heart Rate Change
Change in Heart Rate
Fig. 2. Post-challenge anxiety ratings, as a function of heart rate 1991) among participants one-half of a standard deviation above and/or
change (baseline to mean within-challenge heart rate) and anxiety sen- below the mean for each predictor.
sitivity (indexed via the Anxiety Sensitivity Index; Silverman et al.,

by predictor variables, including the covariates and the main strategies. In terms of main effects, pubertal status corre-
effect of AS, at earlier levels in the model (i.e., 24%); thus, lated positively with negative affectivity, although there was
there was relatively less variance that could be accounted no main effect in regression analyses. This latter finding
for by the interaction term. In this sense, it is noteworthy was surprising in light of theory and research suggesting pu-
that the interaction term enhanced the models predictive berty is marked by increased negative affect (Brooks-Gunn
power (Abelson, 1985). In addition, we employed a vol- et al., 1994). However, post-hoc analyses (see Footnote #4)
untary hyperventilation challenge among psychologically suggested the absence of a main effect in regression analyses
healthy adolescents, which likely truncated upper-end vari- was due to the fact that when AS was entered simultaneously
ability in panic-relevant responding. Utilization of prospec- with pubertal status at level two of the model, it accounted
tive time-sampling methodologies to examine the interactive for much of the associated variance. Prior to drawing con-
roles of pubertal status, AS, and panic-relevant responding in clusions, this pattern of findings should be replicated among
real-world contexts would further develop empirical un- a sample of adolescents with less stringent selection crite-
derstanding of how these factors relate across time during ria, which would delimit the impact of constrained variance.
puberty-related biopsychosocial development. In any case, these two sets of findings provide promising
Next, as a test of specificity, we examined the predictor initial results suggesting pubertal development and AS may
variables in relation to self-reported negative affectivity. As interact to specifically influence panic onset among youth.
expected, the AS by pubertal status interaction was unrelated In exploratory analyses focused on psychophysiological
to negative affectivity, suggesting these factors do not com- reactivity to the challenge, the AS by heart-rate change inter-
bine to increase a general propensity to experience negative action predicted post-challenge anxiety after accounting for
affect, but rather are specifically related to anxious respond- age, gender, baseline anxiety, and the respective main effects.
ing to bodily sensations. Such specificity tests are critical for This analytic approach uniquely extends available work ex-
building sophisticated, developmentally-sensitive risk factor amining the main effect of AS on psychophysiological reac-
models of panic psychopathology (Kraemer et al., 1997). tivity, wherein null findings have been observed among both
Of note, negative affectivity was indexed via self-report youth (Leen-Feldner et al., 2005; Rabian et al., 1999) and
and therefore future studies would benefit from measuring adults (Zvolensky, Feldner, Eifert, & Brown, 2001). The find-
this construct via multi-modal, multi-informant assessment ing that anxious responding was elevated among youth who

Springer
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812 809

were both high in AS and had experienced relatively greater From a clinical perspective, the current study is note-
change in heart-rate from baseline to challenge is consistent worthy for two primary reasons. First, there are direct im-
with AS theory (e.g., Reiss, 1991), which suggests high AS plications for panic prevention, an important area receiving
individuals find bodily perturbation to be fear-relevant. Con- burgeoning empirical attention (Craske & Zucker, 2002).
sistent with the interactionist perspective that puberty is un- The value of well-controlled (e.g., laboratory-based) anxiety-
likely to be pathogenic in its own right (Graber, Petersen, & related risk factor research has been articulated within a
Brooks-Gunn, 1996), the interaction between pubertal status translational framework where risk factor research informs
and heart-rate change did not predict post-challenge anxiety. prevention efforts and vice versa (Zvolensky et al., 2006).
In this sense, the two sets of findings are complementary; This stage model of progress highlights studies of the inter-
only the theoretically-relevant individual difference (AS), play among risk factors as a critical step toward designing
and not pubertal status, combined with heart-rate reactivity effective preventative interventions for youth at risk for anx-
to predict challenge response. Extending these findings to iety. As an illustrative example, given compelling (e.g., in-
other indices of physiological arousal (e.g., cortisol levels) dependently replicated and extended) evidence for the roles
would further bolster these conclusions. of AS and pubertal development in the onset of panic psy-
A number of issues warrant further consideration. First, chopathology, investigators could identify high AS adoles-
the study was cross-sectional. While useful at this early stage cents in a primary care setting who are just entering puberty
of research, this design does not permit causal inferences, and design a brief intervention aimed at reducing AS. Al-
nor does it speak to changes in vulnerability processes across though in its relative infancy, research with adults suggests
time. Moreover, specific pubertal processes were not directly such an approach may be effective (Schmidt et al., in press).
investigated; much has yet to be learned regarding the par- Here, a key research objective will be to empirically estab-
ticular events that occur during puberty, which may promote lish cut-offs that indicate at what level of AS the risk for
panic-relevant learning. Second, this sample is limited as it panic development meaningfully increases (Bernstein, Zv-
was a relatively homogenous (e.g., psychologically healthy, olensky, Stewart, Comeau, & Leen-Feldner, 2006). This type
primarily Caucasian) group of adolescents who volunteered of criterion would enhance prevention-related efforts by de-
to participate for monetary reward and whose parents were lineating specific subpopulations at risk for developing panic
available/willing to transport them to the laboratory. To rule (Feldner, Zvolensky, & Schmidt, 2004). Second, there are in-
out the potential role of self-selection bias among this sam- direct implications of the current findings for work with clin-
ple and increase the generalizability of these findings, future ical populations of adolescents suffering from panic prob-
studies would benefit by examining a more diverse sam- lems. Specifically, researchers are now situated to extend
ple and utilizing alternative recruitment efforts. Third, the these findings to clinical samples of adolescents with panic
present study used a controlled laboratory-based induction disorder to begin to understand if AS and pubertal status may
of abrupt bodily sensations to allow for a multimodal (e.g., be involved in the maintenance or exacerbation of such prob-
self-report; physiological) observation of panic-relevant re- lems. Overall, the present study sets the stage for additional
sponding in real-time. This approach represents a useful research targeted at disentangling the specific mechanisms
laboratory model of panic-relevant responses, which allows that impact panic problems among youth.
for insight into panic vulnerability, but does not reflect ac-
tual PD development. Accordingly, future research that inte- Acknowledgements This project was supported by a grant from
grates both laboratory and naturalistic (prospective) methods Child & Adolescent Research and Training, Inc. awarded to the
first author and National Institute on Drug Abuse research grants
will ultimately provide the most comprehensive account of (1 R01 DA018734-01A1, R03 DA16307-01, and 1 R21 DA016227-
the role of AS and puberty in panic development. Finally, 01) awarded to the sixth author. The authors thank Kate Follansbee,
although an inspection of the non-significant interaction be- Marc Hartigan, Justin McCormick, Amanda ODell, Stephanie Sinisi,
tween AS and age predicting challenge response (see foot- and Lindsay Van Zanten for their assistance with this project.
note #3) increased our confidence that findings from the
current study were not due to the effects of age, the specific
role of puberty (versus other age-related variables, such as References
cognitive development) is a critical issue for future investiga-
tors to elucidate. Here, it will be important to obtain samples Abelson, R. P. (1985). A variance explanation paradox: When a little is
that evidence variability in both age and Tanner stage (e.g., a lot. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 129133.
groups of youth ages 9, 10, and 11 at each of the five Tanner Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
stages; Hayward, 2003) and to directly examine theoretically American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statisti-
relevant cognitive variables such as anxiety-relevant memory cal manual of mental disorders (4th Edition). Washington, DC:
biases (e.g., Teachman, 2005). Author.

Springer
810 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812

Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Worthman, C. M. (1998). Puberty and Forsyth, J. P., & Zvolensky, M. J. (2002). Experimental psychopathol-
depression: The roles of age, pubertal status, and pubertal timing. ogy, clinical science, and practice: An irrelevant or indispensable
Psychological Medicine, 28, 661666. alliance? Applied and Preventive Psychology: Current Scientific
Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treat- Perspectives, 10, 243264.
ment of anxiety and panic (2nd Edition). New York: The Guilford Fried, R., & Grimaldi, J. (1993). The psychology and physiology of
Press. breathing: In behavioral medicine, clinical psychology, and psy-
Bernstein, A., Zvolensky, M. J., Stewart, S. H., Comeau, M. N., & Leen- chiatry. New York: Plenum press.
Feldner, E. W. (2006). Anxiety sensitivity taxonicity across gender Friedman, B. H., & Thayer, J. F. (1998). Anxiety and autonomic flexibil-
among youth. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 679698. ity: A cardiovascular approach. Biological Psychology, 47, 243
Bouchard, S., Mendlowitz, S. L., Coles, M. E., & Franklin, M. (2004). 263.
Considerations in the use of exposure with children. Cognitive and Ginsburg, G. S., & Drake, K. L. (2002). Anxiety sensitivity and panic at-
Behavioral Practice, 11, 5665. tack symptomatology among low-income African-American ado-
Bouton, M.E., Mineka, S., & Barlow, D.H. (2001). A modern learning lescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16, 8396.
theory perspective on the etiology of panic disorder. Psychological Graber, J. A., Petersen, A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1996). Pubertal pro-
Review, 108, 432. cesses: Methods, measures, and models. In J. A. Graber, J. Brooks-
Brooks-Gunn, J., Graber, J., & Paikoff, R. L. (1994). Studying links Gunn, & A. Petersen (Eds.), Transitions Through Adolescence:
between hormones and negative affect: Models and measures. Interpersonal Domains and Context(pp. 2353). Mahwah, NJ:
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 4, 469486. Lawrence Erlbaum Associated
Buchanan, C. M., Eccles, J. S., & Becker, J. B. (1992). Are adolescents Graber, J. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley, J. R., & Brooks-Gunn, J.
the victims of raging hormones? Evidence for activational effects (1997). Is psychopathology associated with the timing of pubertal
of hormones on moods and behaviors in adolescence. Psycholog- development? Journal of the American Academy of Child and
ical Bulletin, 111, 62107. Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 17681776.
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (1991). Individual differences are accentu- Graber, J. A., Seeley, J. R., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewinsohn, P. M.
ated during periods of social change: The sample case of girls at (2004). Is pubertal timing associated with psychopathology in
puberty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 157 young adulthood? Joural of the American Academy of Child and
168. Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 718726.
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (1993). When do individual differences mat- Harrington, P. J., Schmidt, N. B., & Telch, M. J. (1996). Prospective
ter? A paradoxical theory of personality coherence. Psychological evaluation of panic potentiation following 35% CO2 challenge in
Inquiry, 4, 247271. nonclinical subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 823
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences 825.
(2nd. ed.). New York: Academic Press. Hayward, C. (2003). Methodological concerns in puberty-related re-
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation search. In C. Haywards (Ed.) Gender Differences at Puberty.
analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press (pp.
Costos, D., Ackerman, R., & Paradis, L. (2002). Recollections of menar- 114).
che: Communication between mothers and daughters regarding Hayward, C., Killen, J. D., Hammer, L. D., Litt., I. F., Wilson, D. M.,
menstruation. Sex Roles, 46, 4959. Simmonds, B., & Taylor, C. B. (1992) Pubertal stage and panic
Craske, M. G. (2003). Origins of phobias and anxiety disorders: Why attack history in sixth- and seventh-grade girls. American Journal
more women than men? New York, NY: Elsevier. of Psychiatry, 149, 12391243.
Craske, M. G., & Zucker, B. G. (2002). Prevention of anxiety disorders. Hayward, C., Killen, J. D., Kraemer, H. C., Blair-Greimer, A.,
Applied and Preventive Psychology, 10, 155175. Strachowski, D., Cuning, D., & Taylor, S. (1997). Assessment
Davidson, R. J. (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: Per- and phenomenology of nonclinical panic attacks in adolescent
spectives from affective neuroscience. Cognition and Emotion, girls. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 11, 1732.
12, 307320. Hayward, C., Killen, J. D., Kraemer, H. C., & Taylor, C. B. (2000).
Deacon, B.J., Valentiner, D.P., Gutierrez, P.M., & Blacker, D. (2002). Predictors of panic attacks in adolescents. Journal of the American
The Anxiety Sensitivity Index for Children: Factor structure and Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 207214.
relation to panic symptoms in an adolescent sample. Behavior Hayward, C., & Sanborn, K. (2002). Puberty and the emergence of gen-
Research & Therapy, 40, 839852. der differences in psychopathology. Journal of Adolescent Health,
Dorn, L. D., Dahl, R. E., Woodward, H. R., & Biro, F. (2006). Defining 30S, 4958.
the boundaries of early adolescence: A users guide to assessing Holmbeck, G. N. (2002). Post-hoc probing of significant moderational
pubertal status and pubertal timing in research with adolescents. and mediational effects in studies of pediatric populations. Journal
Applied Developmental Science, 10, 3056. of Pediatric Psychology, 27, 8796.
Duke, P. M., Litt, I. F., & Gross, R. T. (1980). Adolescents self- Hornsveld, H., Garssen, B., Dop, M. F., & van Spiegel, P. (1990). Symp-
assessment of sexual maturation. Pediatrics, 66, 918920. tom reporting during voluntary hyperventilation and mental load:
Fechner, P. Y. (2002). Gender differences at puberty. Journal of Ado- Implications for diagnosing hyperventilation syndrome. Journal
lescent Health, 30, 4448 of Psychosomatic Research, 34, 687697.
Feldner, M. T., Zvolensky, M. J., & Schmidt, N. B. (2004). Prevention Joiner, T. E., Catanzaro, S. J., & Laurent, J. (1996). Tripartite struc-
of anxiety psychopathology: A critical review of the empirical ture of positive and negative affect, depression, and anxiety in
literature. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 405 child and adolescent psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Abnormal
424. Psychology, 105, 401409.
Feldner, M. T., Zvolensky, M. J., Stickle, T. R., Bonn-Miller, M. O., & Kearney, C. A., Albano, A. M., Eisen, A. R., Allan, W. D., & Barlow,
Leen-Feldner, E. W. (2006). Anxiety sensitivity as a modera- D. H. (1997). The phenomenology of panic disorder in young-
tor of the emotional consequences of emotion suppression dur- sters: An empirical study of a clinical sample. Journal of Anxiety
ing biological challenge: An experimental test using individual Disorders, 11, 4962.
growth curve analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 249 Killen, J. C., Hayward, C., Litt, I. F., Hammer, L. D., Wilson, D. M.,
272. Milner, B., Taylor, C. B., Varady, A., & Shisslak, C. (1992). Is

Springer
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812 811

puberty a risk factor for eating disorders? American Journal of Reiss, S., Peterson, R. A., Gursky, D. M., & McNally, R. J. (1986).
Childhood Disorders, 146, 323325. Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency, and the prediction of fear-
King, N. J., Gullone, E., Tonge, B. J., & Ollendick, T. H.,(1993). Self- fulness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 18.
reports of panic attacks and manifest anxiety in adolescents. Be- Rogol, A. D., Roemmich, J. N., & Clark, P. A. (2002). Growth at
havior Research and Therapy, 31, 111116. puberty. Journal of Adolescent Health, 31, 192200.
Kraemer, H., Kazdin, A., Offord, D., Kessler, R., Jensen, P., & Kupfer, Rosenfeld, R. G., & Nicodemus, B. C. (2003). The transition from
D. (1997). Coming to terms with the terms of risk. Archives of adolescence to adult life: Physiology of the transition phase and
General Psychiatry, 54, 337343. its evolutionary basis. Hormone Research, 60, 7477.
Kraemer, H. C., Lowe, K. K., & Kupfer, D. J. (2005). To your health: Schmidt, N. B., Eggleston, M., Woolaway-Bickel, K., Fitzpatrick,
How to understand what research tells us about risk. New York, K. K., Vasey, M. W., & Richey. J. A. (in press). Anxiety Sen-
NY: Oxford University Press. sitivity Amelioration Training (ASAT): A longitudinal primary
Lau, J. J., Calamari, J. E., & Waraczynski, M. (1996). Panic attack prevention program targeting cognitive vulnerability. Journal of
symptomatology and anxiety sensitivity in adolescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders.
Anxiety Disorders, 10, 355364. Schmidt, N., Lerew, D., & Jackson, R. (1997). Anxiety sensitivity in
Leen-Feldner, E. W., Feldner, M. T., Bernstein, A., McCormick, the pathogenesis of panic: A prospective evaluation of spontaneous
J. T., & Zvolensky, M. J. (2005). Anxiety sensitivity and panic attacks during acute stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
anxious responding to bodily sensations: A test among adolescents 106, 355364.
using a voluntary hyperventilation challenge. Cognitive Therapy Schmidt, N. B., Lerew, D. R., & Jackson, R. J. (1999). Prospective
and Research, 29, 593609. evaluation of anxiety sensitivity in the pathogenesis of panic:
Li, W., & Zinbarg, R. E. (in press). Anxiety sensitivity and panic attacks: Replication and extension. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108,
A one-year longitudinal study. Behavior Modification. 532537.
Mattis, S. G., & Ollendick, T. H. (1997). Childrens cognitive responses Schmidt, N. B., Zvolensky, M. J., & Maner, J. K. (2006). Anxiety sensi-
to the somatic symptoms of panic. Journal of Abnormal Child tivity: Prospective prediction of panic attacks and Axis I pathology.
Psychology, 24, 4757. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40, 691699.
McNally, R. J. (1990). Psychological approaches to panic disorder: A Sechrest, L. (1963). Incremental validity: A recommendation. Educa-
review. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 403419. tional & Psychological Measurement, 23, 153158.
McNally, R. J. (1999). Panic induction: A critical appraisal. Behavior Sheehy, A., Gasser, T., Molinari, L., & Largo, R. H. (1999). An analysis
Therapy, 30, 245261. of variance of the pubertal and mid-growth spurts for length and
Morris, N. M., & Udry, J. R. (1980). Validation of a self-administered width. Annals of Human Biology, 26, 309331.
instrument to assess stage Of adolescent development. Journal of Silverman, W. K., & Albano, A. M. (1996). The Anxiety Disorders
Youth and Adolescence, 9, 271280. Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: Child and Parent Versions. San
Mrazek, P. G., & Haggerty, R. J. (1994). Reducing risk for mental disor- Antonio, TX: Physiological Corporation.
ders: Frontiers for preventive intervention research. Washington, Silverman, W. K., Fleisig, W., Rabian, B., & Peterson, R. A. (1991).
DC: National Academy Press. Childhood Anxiety Sensitivity Index. Journal of Clinical Child
Ollendick, T.H. (1995). Cognitive behavioral treatment of panic disor- Psychology, 20, 162168.
ders with agoraphobia in adolescents: A multiple baseline design Silverman, W. K., & Weems, C. F. (1999). Anxiety sensitivity in chil-
analysis. Behavior Therapy, 26, 517531. dren. In Taylor, S. (Ed.), Anxiety sensitivity: Theory, research,
Omar, H., McElderry, D., & Zakharia, R. (2003). Educating adolescents and treatment of the fear of anxiety (pp. 239268). Mahwah, NJ:
about puberty: What are we missing? International Journal of Erlbaum.
Adolescent Health and Medicine, 15, 7983. Smits, J. A., Powers, M. B., Cho, Y., & Telch, M. J. (2004). Mechanisms
Patton, G. C., Hibbert, M. E., Carlin, J., Shao, Q., Rosier, M., Caust, of change in cognitive-behavioral treatment of panic disorder:
J., & Bowes, G. (1996). Menarche and the onset of depression Evidence for the fear of fear mediational hypothesis. Journal of
and anxiety in Victoria, Australia. Journal of Epidemiology and Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 646652.
Community Health, 50, 661666. Spear, L. P. (2003). Neurodevelopment during adolescence. In D.
Patton G. C., McMorris, B. J., Trombourou, J. W., Hemphill, S. A., Cicchetti & E. Walker. (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental mechanisms in
Donath, S., & Catalano, R. F. (2004). Puberty and the onset of psychopathology (pp. 6283). New York: Cambridge University
substance use and abuse. Pediatrics, 114, 300306. Press.
Perna, G., Cocchi, S., Politi, E., Bellodi, L. (1997). CO2 challenge in State of Vermont Department of Health. (2000). Retrieved September
nonclinical subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 886 3, 2002, from http://www.healthyvermonters.info/.
887 Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2001). Adolescent development. Annual
Pine, D., Klein, R. G., Coplan, J. D., Papp, L. A., Hoven, C. W., Review of Psychology, 52, 83110.
Martinez, J., et al. (2000). Differential carbon dioxide sensitiv- Susman, E. J., Dorn, L. D., & Chrousos, G. P. (1991). Negative affect
ity in childhood anxiety disorders and nonill comparison group. and hormone levels in young adolescents: Concurrent and pre-
Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 960967. dictive perspectives. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 167
Prenoveau J. M., Forsyth, J., Kelly, M. M., & Barrios, V. (in press). 190.
Repeated exposure to 20% CO2 challenge and risk for develop- Tanner, J. M. (1962). Growth at Adolescence: With a General Con-
ing panic attacks: A controlled 6- and 12-month follow-up in a sideration of the Effects of Hereditary and Environmental Factors
nonclinical sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders upon Growth and Maturation from Birth to Maturity. Blackwell
Rabian, B., Embry, L., MacIntyre, D. (1999). Behavioral validation of Scientific Publications: Oxford.
the Childhood Anxiety Sensitivity Index in children. Journal of Teachman, B. A. (2005). Information processing and anxiety sensi-
Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 105112. tivity: Cognitive vulnerability to panic reflected in interpretation
Reiss, S. (1991). An expectancy model of fear, anxiety, and panic. and memory biases. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 479
Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 141153. 499.
Reiss, S., & McNally, R. J. (1985). Expectancy model of fear. In S. Reiss Telch, M. J., Lucas, J. A., Schmidt, N. B., Hanna, H. H., Jaimez, T. L., &
& R. R. Bootzin (Eds.), Theoretical Issues in Behavior Therapy Lucas, R. A. (1993). Group cognitive behavioral treatment of panic
(pp.107121). San Diego: Academic Press. disorder. Behavior Research and Therapy, 31, 279287.

Springer
812 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2006) 34:799812

Unnewehr, S., Schneider, S., Margraf, J., Jenkins, M., & Florin, I. Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Stanford,
(1996). Exposure to internal and external stimuli: Reactions in CA: Stanford University Press.
children of patients with panic and animal phobia. Journal of Zvolensky, M. J., & Eifert, G. H. (2000). A review of psychological
Anxiety Disorder, 10, 489508. factors/processes affecting anxious responding during voluntary
Vasey M. W., Dadds, M. R. (2001). The Developmental Psychopathol- hyperventilation and inhalations of carbon dioxide-enriched air.
ogy of Anxiety. New York: Oxford. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 375400.
Wilson, K., Gullone, E., & Moss, S. (1998). The youth version of the Zvolensky, M. J., Feldner, M. T., Eifert, G. H., & Brown, R. A. (2001).
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule: A psychometric validation. Affective style among smokers: understanding anxiety sensitiv-
Behaviour Change, 15, 187193. ity, emotional reactivity, and distress tolerance using biological
Wilson, D.M., Killen, J. D., Hayward, C., Robinson, T. N., Hammer, challenge. Addictive Behaviors, 26, 901915.
L. D., Kraemer, H. C., Varady, A., & Taylor, C. B. (1994). Timing Zvolensky, M. J., Lejuez, C. W., Stuart, G. L., & Curtin, J. J. (2001).
and rate of sexual maturation and the onset of cigarette and alcohol Experimental psychopathology in psychological science. Review
use among teenage girls. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent of General Psychology, 5, 371381.
Medicine, 148, 789795. Zvolensky, M. J., Schmidt, N. B., Bernstein, A., & Keough, M. E.
Wilson, D. M., Killen, J. D., Hammer, L. D., Litt, C. F., Vosti, C., Miner, (2006). Risk factor research and prevention programs for anxi-
B., Hayward, C., & Taylor, C. B. (1991). Insulin-like growth factor- ety disorders: A translational research framework. Behaviour Re-
1 as a reflection of body composition, nutrition, and puberty in search and Therapy, 44, 12191239.
sixth and seventh grade girls. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology
and Metabolism, 73, 907912.

Springer
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

You might also like