You are on page 1of 4

ARTICLE

less complex ones, but minor irrelevant

If it’s easy to read, it’s easy features can easily lead us astray in our
effort estimates.
For example, consider the identical

to do, pretty, good, and true exercise instructions shown (in part) in
Figure 1. When they were presented in
an easy-to-read print font (Arial), readers
assumed that the exercise would take 8.2
Hyunjin Song and Norbert Schwarz describe some fascinating findings on how minutes to complete; but when they were
fluency affects judgement, choice and processing style presented in a difficult-to-read print font,
readers assumed it would take nearly twice
as long, a full 15.1 minutes (Song &
Thinking can feel easy or difficult. uppose you ponder whether a new Schwarz, 2008b). They also thought that
But what effect does the ease or
difficulty of reading a text have on
information processing? Can
S exercise routine is suitable for you
or whether a statement like ‘Orsono
is a city in Chile’ is true or false. What
the exercise would flow quite naturally
when the font was easy to read, but feared
that it would drag on when it was difficult
something as seemingly irrelevant would your decision be based on? Most to read. Given these impressions, they
as the print font in which psychological theories suggest that you were more willing to incorporate the
information is presented influence would consider the nature of the exercise exercise into their daily routine when
how information is evaluated, or or draw on your knowledge about it was presented in an easy-to-read font.
even whether it is accepted as true geography to arrive at an informed Quite clearly, people misread the difficulty
or false? What are the practical decision. Surely, you wouldn’t base your of reading the exercise instructions as
implications for everyday life? judgement on the print font in which the indicative of the difficulty involved in
material is presented – or would you? doing the exercise. If we want people
Surprisingly, recent experimental to adopt a new behaviour, it is therefore
research shows that the print font can important that instructions are not only
What is the likely role of metacognitive exert a profound influence on such semantically clear and easy to follow, but
questions

feelings of ease and difficulty in your decisions. This is the case because print also visually easy to read – or else the
own field of psychology? fonts and related variables influence how behaviour may seem unduly demanding.
fluently new information can be processed. Similar results were obtained when
What are the implications for teaching,
The resulting feeling of ease or difficulty, in people read a recipe for a Japanese lunch
counselling, advertising, health
turn, informs a wide variety of judgements, roll (Song & Schwarz, 2008b). When the
education, and political communication?
from judgements of effort to judgements identical recipe was presented in the
What do these influences imply for the of familiarity, truth, risk and beauty (for elegant but difficult-to-read Mistral font,
rationality of human judgement? a review see Schwarz et al., 2009). We they assumed that it would require more
illustrate some of these effects, discuss time and more skill than when it was
their applied implications, and note presented in the easy-to-read Arial font.
parallels between people’s reliance on Hence, it may be advantageous for
resources

Schwarz, N. (2004). Metacognitive


experiences in judgments and the metacognitive feelings of ease and restaurants to describe their dishes in a
decision making. Journal of Consumer difficulty and their reliance on moods difficult-to-read font, which conveys that
Psychology, 14, 332–348. and emotions as sources of information their preparation requires considerable
Schwarz, N., Song, H., & Xu, J. (2009). (Schwarz & Clore, 2007). skill and effort – but the same font may
When thinking is difficult: discourage the hobby cook from trying
Metacognitive experiences as the recipe at home.
information. In M. Wänke (Ed.) The
social psychology of consumer behavior. Effort and choice Other research showed that the print
New York: Psychology Press. When we consider adopting new font can influence whether people make
www.philosophypress.co.uk/?p=876 behaviours, we often try to assess how any decision at all or defer the decision to
www.time.com/time/health/article/0,8599, much effort they will require. Will this a later time. Not surprisingly, people are
1881325,00.html new exercise routine be a pain? Will this more likely to postpone a decision the
www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm? recipe be easy to prepare? Not harder it is to make (for a review see
id=a-recipe-for-motivation surprisingly, complex exercise routines Novemsky et al., 2007). In most cases, the
and recipes will seem more effortful than difficulty arises from characteristics of the
references

Alter, A.L. & Oppenheimer, D.M. (2006). Marketing Research, 44, 347–356. (2004). Processing fluency and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.
Predicting short-term stock Reber, R., Brun, M. & Mittendorfer, K. aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the Schwarz, N. & Clore, G.L. (2007).
fluctuations by using processing (2009). The use of heuristics in perceiver's processing experience? Feelings and phenomenal
fluency. Proceedings of the National intuitive mathematical judgment. Personality and Social Psychology experiences. In A. Kruglanski & E.T.
Academy of Science, 103, 9369–9372. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, Review, 8, 364–382. Higgins (Eds.) Social psychology:
McGlone, M.S. & Tofighbakhsh, J. (2000). 15(6), 1174–1178. Schwarz, N. (2006). On judgments of Handbook of basic principles (2nd edn)
Birds of a feather flock conjointly. Reber, R. & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of truth and beauty. Daedalus, 135, (pp.385–407). New York: Guilford.
Psychological Science, 11, 424–428. perceptual fluency on judgments of 136–138. Schwarz, N., Sanna, L., Skurnik, I. &
Novemsky, N., Dhar, R., Schwarz, N. & truth. Consciousness and Cognition, 8, Schwarz, N. & Clore, G.L. (1983). Mood, Yoon, C. (2007). Metacognitive
Simonson, I. (2007). Preference 338–342. misattribution, and judgments of experiences and the intricacies of
fluency in choice. Journal of Reber, R., Schwarz, N. & Winkielman, P. well-being. Journal of Personality and setting people straight. Advances in

108 vol 23 no 2 february 2010


processing fluency

the experimenter stated unless their attention is drawn to the


Tuck your chin into your chest, and the obvious: ‘This may be incidental nature of their current feelings
then lift your chin upward as far difficult to read because of (e.g. Schwarz & Clore, 1983).
the print font.’ In this case,
as possible. 6–10 repetitions deferral dropped from 41
Lower your left ear toward your left per cent to 16 per cent, Familiarity and risk
shoulder and then your right ear wiping out the difference In addition to providing information
toward your right shoulder. 6–10 between the two fonts. about effort, the fluency with which a
In combination, these stimulus can be processed also provides
repetitions findings highlight that information about the familiarity of the
people are sensitive to their stimulus. Familiar stimuli are indeed
Tuck your chin into your chest, and then lift your feelings of ease or difficulty, easier to process, recognise and remember
but insensitive to where than unfamiliar stimuli. But not
chin upward as far as possible. 6–10 these feelings come from. everything that is easy to process is also
repetitions As a result, they familiar – in some cases, it is only easy to
Lower your left ear toward your left shoulder and misattribute the process because it is presented in an easy-
then your right ear toward your right shoulder. experienced ease or to-read print font or with good
difficulty to whatever is in figure–ground contrast. As already seen,
6–10 repetitions the focus of their attention. however, people are more sensitive to
Hence, they decide to defer their feelings of ease or difficulty than
choice, or to avoid an to where those feelings come from and
Figure 1. People mistakenly interpret the difficulty of exercise routine, simply hence infer familiarity whenever a
reading exercise instructions as indicative of the difficulty because the print font makes stimulus is easy to process. This
involved in doing the exercise the information difficult to fluency–familiarity link is at the heart
process. Once their attention of many fluency effects, including the
is drawn to the print font, influence of fluency on judgements of
choice situation, like difficult trade-offs facilitating a correct attribution, these risk.
between price and quality or the sheer effects are no longer observed. This finding It is not surprising that familiar options
number of similar choice alternatives. parallels the observation that people draw feel safer than unfamiliar ones. In grocery
However, the same inclination to defer on their moods as a source of information aisles, we often prefer the same familiar
choice can be observed when vegetables over less
the experienced difficulty familiar exotic ones
arises merely from the print because we do not want to
font in which the choice run the risk of picking one
alternatives are described. with a strange taste or
Novemsky and colleagues unknown allergens.
(2007) presented the same Similarly, people perceive
information about two technologies, investments
cordless phones in easy- or and leisure activities as less
difficult-to-read fonts. They risky the more familiar
observed that 17 per cent of they are with them. But
their participants postponed does this observation really
choice when the font was easy reflect the influence of
to read, whereas 41 per cent mere familiarity or does
did so when the font was extended exposure to a
difficult to read. Apparently, potential threat desensitise
participants misread the people to the risks
difficulty arising from the print involved? To address this
font as reflecting the difficulty issue, we took advantage of
of making a choice. the well-established fluency-
Supporting this interpretation, People perceive technologies, investments, leisure activities, even –familiarity link. Given that
the effect was eliminated when vegetables, as less risky the more familiar they are with them fluently processed stimuli

Experimental Social Psychology, 39, questions. Social cognition, 26, 20(2), 135–138. Personality and Social Psychology, 81,
127-161. 791–799. Weaver, K., Garcia, S.M., Schwarz, N. & 989–1000.
Schwarz, N., Song, H. & Xu, J. (2009). Song, H. & Schwarz, N. (2008b). If it’s Miller, D.T. (2007). Inferring the Winkielman, P., Halberstadt, J.,
When thinking is difficult: hard to read, it’s hard to do: popularity of an opinion from its Fazendeiro, T. & Catty, S. (2006).
Metacognitive experiences as Processing fluency affects effort familiarity. Journal of Personality and Prototypes are attractive because
information. In M. Wänke (Ed.) The prediction and motivation. Social Psychology, 92, 821–833. they are easy on mind. Psychological
social psychology of consumer Psychological Science, 19, 986–988. Winkielman, P. & Cacioppo, J.T. (2001). Science, 17(9), 799–806.
behavior. New York: Psychology Song, H. & Schwarz, N. (2009). If it’s Mind at ease puts a smile on the Zajonc, R.B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of
Press. difficult to pronounce, it must be face: Psychophysiological evidence mere exposure. Journal of Personality
Song, H. & Schwarz, N. (2008a). Fluency risky: Fluency, familiarity, and risk that processing facilitation leads and Social Psychology, Monograph
and the detection of misleading perception. Psychological Science, to positive affect. Journal of Supplement, 9, 1–27.

read discuss contribute at www.thepsychologist.org.uk 109


processing fluency

seem more familiar, they should also be


perceived as less threatening and less risky.
Empirically this is the case (Song &
Schwarz, 2009). In one study, participants
perceived ostensible food additives with
hard-to-pronounce names (e.g.
Hnegripitrom) as more harmful than food
additives with easy-to-pronounce names
(e.g. Magnalroxate). In addition, the food
additives with difficult names were
perceived as more novel than the ones
with easy names, and perceived novelty
mediated the influence of ease of
pronunciation on perceived risk.
Given that none of our participants
could know anything about these
ostensible food additives (after all, we
made up the names), this finding provided
first evidence that perceived familiarity, by
itself, influences perceptions of risk.
Moreover, this influence is not limited to
the perception of negative risks, as in the
case of food additives, but can also be
observed in the perception of risks that
people consider desirable. For instance,
people may want to take risky amusement
park rides to enjoy the feeling of
excitement and adventure. Would their
choice be influenced by the ease or Participants perceived rides with difficult-to-pronounce names as more exciting and
difficulty with which the names of the adventurous than rides with easy-to-pronounce names
amusement park rides can be pronounced?
The answer is a clear ‘yes’ (Song & Presumably, investment opportunities information to determine whether
Schwarz, 2009). Participants perceived with easy-to-pronounce ticker symbols something is true or not: If many people
rides with difficult-to-pronounce names seemed less risky, giving them an believe it, there’s probably something to
(e.g. Tsiischili) as more exciting and advantage in initial public offerings. it. Unfortunately, however, we are poor at
adventurous than rides with easy-to- The observed link between fluency, tracking how often we heard something
pronounce names (e.g. Chunta). Other familiarity and risk perception has many and rely instead on whether it sounds
participants, however, were asked how important practical implications. In familiar – if it does, we probably heard it
likely the rides would make them feel sick certain product domains, like insurance before. Hence, variables that increase the
– and once again, the rides with difficult- and food, safety is highly valued. Hence, perceived familiarity of a statement also
to-pronounce names won. Throughout, marketers may want to give these increase its perceived social consensus
the ease with which the names of stimuli products easy-to-pronounce names and and the impression that the statement is
could be pronounced influenced their may want to present the product likely to be true (for a review see Schwarz
perceived familiarity. This perceived information in ways that facilitate easy et al., 2007).
familiarity, in turn, influenced how risky processing. In other domains, however, For example, Weaver et al. (2007)
the stimuli seemed, no matter if the risk risk is valued. For instance, sports like presented participants with multiple
was desirable or undesirable. bungee jumping, parachuting or hang repetitions of the same opinion statement.
Similar observations have been made gliding derive their excitement from the For some participants, each repetition
in a real-world domain with high stakes: risks involved. In such cases, difficult-to- came from a different communicator,
people’s investments in the stock market. pronounce names and hard-to-process whereas for others, all repetitions came
Analysing the performance of initial descriptions may highlight the promise from the same communicator. When
public offerings on the New York Stock of adventure and excitement. Similarly, later asked to estimate how widely the
Exchange, Alter and Oppenheimer (2006) policy makers may want to pay attention conveyed opinion is shared, participants
found that companies with easy-to- to fluency variables to alert consumers to estimated higher social consensus the
pronounce ticker symbols (e.g. KAR) potential hazards and to prevent the more often they had read the identical
performed better than companies with erroneous impression that a hazardous statement – even when each repetition
difficult-to-pronounce ticker symbols product is safe simply because its name came from the same single source.
(RDO). Investing $1000 in a basket of is easy to pronounce. Apparently, participants drew on the
stocks with fluent ticker symbols would familiarity of the opinion to estimate
have yielded an excess profit of $85.35 its popularity – and were once again
over a basket with disfluent ticker Social consensus and truth insensitive to where this feeling of
symbols on the first day of trading. This The observed fluency–familiarity link familiarity came from. As a result, a single
advantage dropped to a still impressive also has important implications for repetitive voice sounded like a chorus. And
$20.25 by the end of the first year of judgements of truth. As social once people infer that an opinion is widely
trading, as more diagnostic information psychologists have long been aware, shared, it is also likely to be accepted as
about the companies became available. people often rely on social consensus true – after all, if many people believe it,

110 vol 23 no 2 february 2010


processing fluency

there’s probably something to it. Hence, case, as a growing number of studies However, a change in print fonts is
the mere repetition of a statement shows. For example, we like a stimulus sufficient to attenuate this Moses illusion.
facilitates its acceptance as true, as more when a preceding visual or semantic When the question was presented in an
naturalistic studies of war-time rumours prime facilitates its processing – we even easy-to-read font, only 7 per cent of the
and many laboratory experiments find a picture of a lock more beautiful readers noticed the error, whereas 40 per
demonstrated (for a review see Schwarz when it was preceded by the word ‘key’ cent did so when it was presented in a
et al., 2009). (see Reber et al., 2004). This positive difficult-to-read font, similar to the one
As already seen, however, repetition response to fluently processed stimuli can shown in Figure 1 (Song & Schwarz,
is not the only variable that makes things also be captured with electromyography, 2008a). Whether this helps or hurts task
seem familiar – any other variable that a procedure that measures subtle muscle performance depends on whether the first
increases processing fluency can do the responses in the face (Winkielman & thing that comes to mind is correct or not.
trick. For example, Reber and Schwarz Cacioppo, 2001), indicating that fluent This phenomenon has potentially
(1999) presented participants with processing feels good. important practical implications. For
statements like ‘Orsono is a city in Chile’ Our preference for fluently processed example, product manufacturers often
and asked them to judge whether the stimuli underlies many of the variables hide deceptive information in the fine
statement is true or false. To manipulate known to influence aesthetic experience, print to make it less noticeable. If
the statements’ perceived familiarity, they from symmetry and figure–ground contrast consumers ever read the fine print,
presented the statements in colours that to the gestalt laws – all of these variables however, the disfluency associated with
were easy or difficult to read against a facilitate fluent processing (Reber et al., processing it may make it more likely
coloured background. As expected, the 2004). The same principle is also central to that they notice the deception. Similarly,
same statement was more often accepted the observation that we prefer prototypical presenting multiple-choice questions in
as true when the colour contrast made faces over more unusual ones – a difficult-to-read font may attenuate the
reading easy rather than difficult. Similarly, prototypical faces are easier to process and allure of familiar but erroneous response
McGlone and Tofighbakhsh (2000) elicit a more positive affective response alternatives.
reported that substantively equivalent (Winkielman et al., 2006). Moreover, this
aphorisms were more likely to be accepted research also sheds light on why scientists
as true when they were presented in a and poets alike believe that beauty and Conclusion
rhyming (e.g. ‘Woes unite foes’) rather truth go hand in hand, despite all the As the reviewed examples illustrate,
than non-rhyming form (e.g. ‘Woes unite beautiful and elegant theories that landed people attend to the dynamics of their
enemies’). Throughout, variables that on the trash heap of science – intuitive own information processing and are
facilitate fluent processing also facilitate judgements of beauty and truth are based highly sensitive to the resulting feelings
the impression that a statement is familiar on the same input, namely the experience of ease or difficulty. Unfortunately, they
and hence likely to be true. of fluent processing (Reber et al., 2009; are much less sensitive to where these
This fluency–familiarity–truth link Schwarz, 2006). feelings come from. As has been observed
presents a particular problem when we for moods and emotions (for a review see
attempt to counter rumours or to discredit Schwarz & Clore, 2007), they assume
misleading information. In most cases, the Fluency and processing style – that their feelings bear on whatever they
correction includes a repetition of the false Do I need to think twice? are thinking about, unless their attention
statement, along with reasons why it is Our positive affective response to fluently is drawn to an incidental source. Hence,
false. Unfortunately, this repetition processed material and the role of fluency any variable that facilitates or impairs
increases the experience of familiarity in judgements of popularity and truth fluent information processing can
when the false statement is encountered converge to predict an additional effect: profoundly affect people’s judgements and
again at a later time – long after the correct Fluently processed material should decisions. Communicators and educators
facts have been forgotten. As a result, receive less scrutiny. On the one hand, are therefore well advised to present
corrections that repeat false information statements that sound like we heard them information in a form that facilitates easy
ironically facilitate its later acceptance as before are less likely to invite scrutiny processing: if it’s easy to read, it seems
true (see Schwarz et al., 2007). It is than statements that seem unfamiliar. On easy to do, pretty, good, and true.
therefore important never to repeat the other hand, positive affect generally
anything that is false. Instead, increases heuristic processing with
communicators should attempt to make limited attention to detail, whereas
the truth as fluent and familiar as possible, negative affect facilitates systematic I Hyunjin Song
taking advantage of variables like processing with higher detail orientation is at Yale University
repetition, rhyme and easy readability. (see Schwarz & Clore, 2007). Hence, Hyunjin.song@yale.edu
material that is presented in a difficult-
to-read print font should receive more
Affect and beauty scrutiny, making it more likely that
One of the best known fluency effects readers detect substantive errors.
is the mere exposure effect originally As an example, consider the question
identified by Zajonc (1968): The more ‘How many animals of each kind did
often we see an object, like a Chinese Moses take on the Ark?’ Most people I Norbert Schwarz
ideograph, the more we like it. From a answer ‘two’ despite knowing that the is at the University of
fluency perspective, repeated exposure is biblical actor was Noah, not Moses. Even Michigan
just one of many variables that facilitate when warned that some of the statements nschwarz@umich.edu
fluent processing. If so, any other variable may be distorted, most people fail to
that makes processing easy should also notice the error because both actors are
increase liking. Empirically this is the similar in the context of biblical stories.

read discuss contribute at www.thepsychologist.org.uk 111

You might also like