You are on page 1of 9

bs_bs_banner

Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities


Volume 13 Number 2 pp 181189 June 2016

doi: 10.1111/jppi.12153

Family Caregiving of Aging Adults


With Down Syndrome
Robert M. Hodapp*,, Meghan M. Burke, Crystal I. Finley*,, and Richard C. Urbano*,
*Vanderbilt Kennedy Center, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA; Department of Special Education, Peabody College,
Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA; Department of Special Education, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign,
Champaign, IL, USA; and Department of Pediatrics, Vanderbilt University Medical School, Nashville, TN, USA

Abstract
Although persons with Down syndrome now live to approximately 60 years, the implications of increased longevity on family caregiving have received inadequate attention. Even compared with adults with other types of intellectual disabilities, by their late 40s
and 50s adults with Down syndrome often show multiple health problems, cognitive-adaptive declines, and changes in daily work
and activities. If alive, their parents are more often elderly, as mothers give birth to newborns with Down syndrome at a median age
of 32 years (with high percentages age 40 and older). In addition, higher percentages of adults with (vs. without) Down syndrome
live in their family homes and greater percentages may ultimately reside with siblings. Compared with caring for other adults with
intellectual disabilities, aging adults with Down syndrome may present earlierand possibly more severe and more health-related
caregiving challenges to their parents and siblings. As a field and as a society, the authors note that we need to promote revisions of
family support policies and interventions; recognize the inter-relations of aging-related changes and their effects; and anticipate how
aging-related changes in the brothersister with Down syndrome and the parent(s) might affect sibling caregivers.
Keywords: adult siblings, aging-related health and functional decline, Down syndrome, family caregiving, intellectual disability,
older mothers

Introduction
Of the many modern-day successes in the field of intellectual
disabilities, perhaps most striking is the increasing life expectancy
of individuals with Down syndrome. From a median lifespan of
9 years during the 1920s (Penrose, 1949), individuals with Down
syndrome now live to approximately 60 years (Zigman, 2013). In
addition, such increases appear to be continuing. Consider, for
example, the change from the mid-1980s, when median life spans
were estimated to be 2530 years, until todays near-60 year life
span (Yang, Rasmussen, & Friedman, 2002; see also Janicki, Dalton, Henderson, & Davidson, 1999). Although various changes
have been ascribed to explain increasing longevity, from better
health care to more active involvement in the community, in
most industrialized societies persons with Down syndrome enjoy
increasingly long lives.
Although a cause for celebration, increasing longevity also
brings forth important implications for the families of these
adults. On the most obvious level, when an adult with disabilities
lives longer, families must consider potential changes in the priReceived March 12, 2015; accepted October 21, 2015
Correspondence: Richard C. Urbano, Pediatrics and Vanderbilt Kennedy
Center, Vanderbilt University, PMB 40, 230 Appleton Place, Nashville, TN
37212, USA. Tel: 11 615 875-9659; Fax: 11 615 322-8236;
E-mail: richard.urbano@gmail.com

mary carer. Forty-years ago, when individuals with Down syndrome lived only into their 30s, their parents would usually
outlive them. With life expectancies of persons with Down syndrome now at 60 years or more, fewer parents can be expected to
outlive their offspring. As a resultand considering the inadequacy of adult-disability services in the United States (National
Council on Disability, 2005) and in most industrialized nations
(World Health Organization, 2011)others in the family need
to assume increased caregiving responsibilities.
In this article, we go beyond this obvious change to explore
growing issues of family caregiving for aging adults with Down
syndrome. To state our argument, aging-related caregiving begins
as early as the mid-late 40s for many adults with Down syndrome, who by their late 40s often show health, cognitiveadaptive, and vocational declines. Such caregiving is performed
by parents who are older and may be more infirm, even though
adults with Down syndrome may more often live in their parental homes or with siblings. As a result, adult siblings may need to
assume high levels of care at earlier ages.
This triad of premature aging among adults with Down syndrome, parents who are older, and family caregiving that is more
common may make caregiving different when the adult has
Down syndrome compared with when adults have other types of
intellectual disability (ID). At this point, however, we simply
hypothesize that such differences may exist. In reviewing the
available evidence, we also note that the field has only begun to

C 2016 International Association for the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
V

Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities

Volume 13 Number 2 June 2016

R. M. Hodapp et al. Family Caregiving of Aging Adults with Down Syndrome

TABLE 1
Percentage of samples with specific or general health problems, by decade
Age groups
Problem

Study

Health condition studies


Visual impairment
Hearing impairment
Epilepsy
Dementia
Studies with measure of overall health
Medical change, past 5 years
Major health problem, past year
Poor-Fair overall health

20s

(8.8%)a
(13.3%)
(5.8%)
(5.0%)
(24.3%)

1
2
1
2
1
1

23.3%
7.7%
14.0%
7.7%
0.0%

3
4
5

17.4% (16.2%)
6.0%

30s

36.2%
11.5%
12.8%
9.8%
2.1%
0.0%

(11.6%)
(14.5%)
(6.3%)
(5.1%)
(21.9%)
(1.1%)

12.7% (13.8%)

40s

31.5%
19.0%
24.1%
9.9%
16.7%
22.2%

(17.6%)
(16.2%)
(6.5%)
(7.0%)
(18.5%)
(0.0%)

27.3% (28.0%)

50s

59.1%
18.6%
37.0%
13.7%
21.7%
45.7%

(20.2%)
(18.0%)
(15.2%)
(8.8%)
(12.2%)
(1.7%)

69.0% (37.0%)
38.5% (26.7%)
31.0%

Down syndrome % (Non-DS%, if available).


1 5 van Schrojenstein Lantman-de Valk et al. (1997; see their Table 2); 2 5 Stancliffe et al. (2012) (20s age-group 5 1829 years); 3 5 Patti et al. (2005);
4 5 Hodapp and Urbano (2007) (age-groups changed to decades); 5 5 Esbensen et al. (2013) (age-group 5 mean age of sample at testing).

examine these issues; as a by-product, our review reveals a critical


need for more research. Even basic issuesfor example, the general health of aging adults with Down syndrome or their residences during their later yearsare the subject of but a handful of
studies. Yet despite these limitations, we feel it necessary to call
attention to the aging-related changes in adults with Down syndrome, their parents, and their siblings. To the degree possible,
then, in the pages that follow we document these syndromespecific findings, before ending with practical and policy recommendations for caregiving in families of aging adults with Down
syndrome.
Aging in Down Syndrome: Three
Hypothesized Differences
Like anyone else, individuals with Down syndrome age and
experience aging-related health and functional declines. In the
case of Down syndrome, however, three features seem to influence the timing and nature of care needs for these aging adults.
Old Age in Down Syndrome Begins Earlier and is
Accompanied by Aging-related Health, Adaptive,
and Vocational Changes
To most of us, old age might be considered the retirement
years, beginning at age 65 or 70 and continuing until death. At
around these ages, one expects to develop a variety of agingrelated health problems and, in some cases, to need more intensive, individualized care. In Down syndrome old age starts earlier,
generally at age 45 or 50. In Down syndrome health research,
most studies categorize older adults as those age either 40 or 50
182

and older (Torr, Strydom, Patti, & Jokinen, 2010), and recent
examinations based on DNA methylation levels have found that
trisomy 21 leads to premature aging across different cell types, on
average by 6.6 years (up to 11.5 years for brain cells; Horvath
et al., 2015).
In Table 1, we document what might be called the infirmities
of old age. The table is divided into two studies that directly
report one or more specific medical problems, along with a bottom section of studies that, though not focused on medical conditions, report on the overall health status of their participants.
The first study, by Van Schrojenstein Lantman-deValk et al.
(1997), involved 1,602 persons with ID who lived in either institutions or group homes in two southern Dutch provinces (the
authors note that, in the Netherlands, only 2-5% of people with
ID older than 50 years live with their families, p. 43). Of these
individuals, 243 had Down syndrome (which was not further
broken down by age-groups). As shown in the table, the percentages of specific aging conditionsincluding visual impairments,
hearing impairments, epilepsy, and dementiadiffered for adults
with and without Down syndrome who were in their 40s and
50s. By the 40s and 50s, large percentages of adults with Down
syndrome showed one or more aging-related conditions, and
such percentages often greatly exceeded percentages shown by
adults with (non-DS) intellectual disabilities.
The Stancliffe et al. (2012) study also found that substantial
percentages of adults with Down syndrome displayed these problems during their 40s and 50s, though the two groups often did
not differ. But we also note that Stancliffe et al. (2012), while
studying large numbers of participants, was limited to only those
adults with ID in 25 American states who received adult disability
services. Across the United States, however, estimates are that
only 25% of all adults with intellectual disabilities receive staterun disability services, whereas 75% do not (Hewitt, 2014).

Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities

Volume 13 Number 2 June 2016

R. M. Hodapp et al. Family Caregiving of Aging Adults with Down Syndrome

How, then, do the 25% receiving services differ from the 75%
not receiving services? We do not know. Compared with the Van
Schrojenstein Lantman-deValk et al. (1997) and to other studies
at the bottom of Table 1, however, the Stancliffe et al. study seems
anomalous.
In the lower part of Table 1, we summarize old-age declines
through several studies that did not exclusively focus on health
per se. Esbensen, Mailick, and Silverman (2013) longitudinally
examined 75 home-living adults when they averaged 28 years
and then again at 52 years. Carers were asked about the general
health of their adult with Down syndrome using a simple, singlequestion measure which has proven remarkably predictive of
actual health status (Idler & Benyamini, 1997). In Hodapp and
Urbano (2007same data set as in Hodapp, Finley, & Urbano,
2016), siblings of over 1,100 adults with disabilities were surveyed; 216 of these adults were 2059-year-olds with Down syndrome, 416 were 20-59-year-old adults with non-DS intellectual
disabilities (both groups lived either at home or in a variety of
out-of-home settings). In the final study, Patti, Amble, and Flory
(2005) examined diagnostic and research clinic participants,
including 108 adults with Down syndrome and 43 with nonDown syndrome ID (adults in both samples were aged 5059
years). Through a review of clinical and medical records of each
person, coders examined four different types of life changes,
including the presence or absence of medical changes that had
occurred over the past 5 years.
In all three studies, increased percentages of health problems
or declines occurred over age groups; especially during the 50s,
higher percentages of adults with Down syndrome (vs. with intellectual disabilities) showed declining health. Among adults with
Down syndrome aged 5059 (as opposed to earlier age groups),
increases were apparent in the percentages showing a major
health problem in the past year (Hodapp & Urbano, 2007) or
who showed poor-fair overall health (Esbensen et al., 2013). In
the two studies that compared same-age adults with and without
Down syndrome, the 5059-year-olds with Down syndromeas
compared with 5059-year-olds with (non-DS) IDdisplayed
higher percentages of major health problems within the past year
(Hodapp & Urbano, 2007) or over the past 5 years (Patti et al.,
2005). Granted, individual studies were limited by having crosssectional (not longitudinal) designs; no direct comparison of
groups with and without Down syndrome; health status designations that relied on reports from siblings, carers, or clinical
records; and/or small participant numbers, especially at the older
ages. Still, findings seem fairly consistent across these diverse
studies.
Going beyond health per se, one might also examine agingrelated changes in relation to cognitive and adaptive behavior.
Again, few studies exist, with none featuring designs that are longitudinal and that compare adults with and without Down syndrome. Still, two longitudinal studies document similar agerelated changes in cognitive and adaptive functioning among
adults with Down syndrome. In the first, Oliver, Crayton, Holland, Hall, and Bradbury (1998) examined 57 adults with Down
syndrome, all of whom were 30 years or older and repeatedly
tested over a 4-year span. Three neuropsychological tests (simple
tests of aphasia, agnosia, and apraxia) were administered and the
authors tabulated a composite index of cognitive functioning.
Examining mean scores longitudinally, the subgroup who did

not show declines was younger at first testing (40.33 years)


than the subgroup showing declines (47.99 years). Although
participant numbers became small in later age groups, higher
percentages of adults showed composite declines when initial
testing began at older ages: percentages of composite declines
were 11.8% (2 of 17) of those tested during the 30s, 23% (6 of
26) during the 40s, and 70% (7 of 10) during their 50s. Moreover, all participants showing the most severe test-to-retest
declines were in their 40s (11.5% of age group) or 50s (40% of
age group).
Similar results were noted in a longitudinal study of adaptive
behavior over a 4-year period. Considering as clinically significant any decline in which the individual scored 25 or more
points lower across testing on the Adaptive Behavior Scales (ABS;
Fogelman, 1975), Collacott and Cooper (1997) examined 83
adults with Down syndrome, with the original ABS administration to carers when adults were in their 20s (n 5 19), 30s
(n 5 20), 40s (n 5 20), 50s (n 5 20), and 60s (n 5 4). Major
declines were noted in 10% of participants whose initial assessment occurred in their 20s, 5% of those in their 30s, 25% of
those in their 40s, 65% of those in their 50s, and 100% (all four
participants) of those in their 60s. In addition to these studies
that highlight the percentages of samples with declines or problems, mean-score declines across the 40s and 50s have also been
noted longitudinally (e.g., Carr & Collins, 2014, until 47 years),
cross-sectionally across decades (e.g., Tsao, Kindelberger, Freminville, Touraine, & Busssy, 2015), and in cross-sequential designs
that followed different-age individuals over multiyear spans
(Maaskant et al., 1996; Zigman, Schupf, Urv, Zigman, & Silverman, 2002).
Notably, most studies do not differentiate those adults with
Down syndrome who do and who do not show Alzheimers
dementia. We acknowledge the many debates in this area, particularly whether cognitive, linguistic, or adaptive skills decline for
those individuals without dementia over the 40s or 50s (cf. Hawkins, Eklund, James, & Foose, 2003; Rondal, 2009). From the perspectives of mothers, fathers, siblings, and individuals with
Down syndrome, however, combined-sample percentages (i.e., of
participants with and without dementia) better inform families
just how frequently such health conditions and cognitiveadaptive declines may be occurring among these aging adults.
An additional aging-related change concerns work activities.
Figure 1 shows the percentages of three levels of work-daily activities for adults with Down syndrome vs. those of adults with
(non-DS) ID. These data were reanalyzed from our earlier study
of work among adults with intellectual disabilities, with a specific
focus on correlates of those adults who had no work or daily
activity whatsoever (Taylor & Hodapp, 2012). For Figure 1,
groups are divided into the 216 participants with Down syndrome (left-side bars) and the 416 with intellectual disabilities,
but not Down syndrome (right-side bars). These cross-sectional
groups, comprised those who were in their 20s, 30s, 40s, and 50s,
varied greatly in size, ranging from 69 participants in the 20s
group to 26 in the 50s group for those with Down syndrome,
from 119 in the 20s group to 90 in the 50s group for those with
(non-DS) ID.
Using this 2 (diagnostic group) by 4 (age-group) rubric, we
then examined each participants level of job or activity. Highlevel jobs were defined as when adults worked in the community,
183

Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities

Volume 13 Number 2 June 2016

R. M. Hodapp et al. Family Caregiving of Aging Adults with Down Syndrome

Offspring With Down Syndrome More Often


Have Older Parents

FIGURE 1
Proportion of adults with and without Down syndrome who
engage in higher-level, lower-level, or no work or activities, by
age group.

with varying levels of supports, as well as when adults were in


school or training for future jobs. Lower-level work-activity
included supervised workshops, prevocational training, volunteer
activities, and activity/day settings. Finally, we counted participants as having no job or activity when siblings checked that
their brothersister with disabilities Does not work or has no
activity setting.
Compared with those in their 20s and 30s, adults with Down
syndrome showed fairly marked changes in the 40s and 50s. Percentages of those working in high-level jobs declined rapidly,
from somewhere between 1/3 and 1/2 of participants in the 20s
and 30s, to smaller percentages in the 40s, before totally disappearing in the 50s; indeed, of 26 participants with Down syndrome aged 50 and above, none (0%) worked in high-level
community jobs (even with help or accommodations). Conversely, those with no job or activity doubled from the 20s30s to
the 40s (about 10%), before doubling again in the 50s (slightly
less than 20%). The middle barconstituting sheltered workshops, day programs, or activity settingsrose markedly in the
40s and 50s. Granted, these data were reported by siblings and
were cross-sectional. As a result, we cannot conclude that these
adults underwent changes over time, as conceivably adults with
Down syndrome who were in their 50s had never held jobs in the
community. Still, this change over age groups fits well with the
health, cognitive, and adaptive findings noted above. Although
similar changes across age-periods occurred in the non-DS group
(especially by the 50s), such changes were much less extreme
(e.g., among the 50-year-olds with non-DS intellectual disabilities, 15.6% worked in community jobs).
184

Although the focus of many public health studies, advanced


maternal age has received little attention in other areas of Down
syndrome research. During childhood, older motherswho are
more often better educated, married and less likely to divorce
generally provide their children more resources (McLanahan,
2004; in Down syndrome, see Hodapp, Burke, & Urbano, 2012),
but the issue of older-than-usual parents has less often been
explored during adulthood.
To document that mothers of offspring with Down syndrome
are indeed older, we compared the age distributions of mothers
who gave birth to infants with Down syndrome in the United
States (Martin, Hamilton, Osterman, Curtin, & Mathews, 2015),
the United Kingdom (Congenital Anomaly Statistics, 2010), and
Australia (Staples, Sutherland, Haan, & Clisby, 1991these data,
in particular, are somewhat dated). Across these three countries
(which differ somewhat in distributions), the 50%, 75%, and
90% maternal birth ages in the overall population were 27, 32,
and 37 years, respectively, whereas maternal ages at birth of the
newborn with Down syndrome were 32, 37, and 42 years, respectively. Compared with mothers in the general population, mothers of newborns with Down syndrome are, on average, roughly 5
years older.
For our purposes, having older parents involves age
differentials that continue throughout life. Thus, the mother who
is 35 years old at the birth of a newborn (a 0-year-old) is 70
years old when the offspring is 35; 80 years old when her offspring is 45. Fathers, who are generally 12 years older than their
wives, also have average life spans that are 45 years shorter. As a
result, when offspring with Down syndrome are in their late 40s
and 50s, and often coping with aging-related health problems
and adaptive-cognitive declines, their parents (if alive) are most
likely experiencing the aging-related issues of anyone who is in
their 70s and 80s.
Adults With Down Syndrome Experience Different
Forms of Family Caregiving
Although estimates vary, from 75% to 84% of all persons
with intellectual and developmental disabilities live with their
families (Fujiura, 2014). But as the field has begun to examine
residential issues for adults with Down syndrome, more specific
differences have also arisen. The first involves the age-related
nature of home living in adulthood (Hodapp et al., 2016; Stancliffe et al., 2012; Tsao et al., 2015). Across individuals with and
without Down syndrome, decreasing percentages of adults live in
their family homes across each decade of adulthood. Thus, higher
percentages live in the family home during the 20s, which
decreases during the 30s, decreasing again during the 40s and yet
again during the 50s. Especially, during the first two decades of
adulthoodwhen adults with ID are in their 20s and 30s
higher percentages of adults with (vs. without) Down syndrome
live with their parents. By the time these adults are in their 40s
and 50s, however, fewer live in their family home.
Figure 2 shows changes in percentages of adults with Down
syndrome who live in the parents home across decades for the

Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities

Volume 13 Number 2 June 2016

R. M. Hodapp et al. Family Caregiving of Aging Adults with Down Syndrome

FIGURE 2
Proportion of adults with Down syndrome who live in their
family homes from three recent studies, by age group.

three studies of this issue. Stancliffe et al. (2012) examined data


from the National Core Indicators (NCI) project, which examined
those individuals who received state disability services in 25 U.S.
states. Hodapp et al. (2016) analyzed the Adult Sibling Survey of
almost 1,200 adult American siblings from a web-based survey,
while Tsao et al. (2015) studied 120 French adults with Down syndrome who did not show Alzheimers dementia (providing participants living status by decade; Tsao et al.s Table 1, p. 6).
Despite differences in samples, study emphases, and methodologies, a similarthough not identicalpattern emerges across
studies. In all studies, high percentages of adults live in the family
home during the 20s and 30s. In the two studies that also examined adults with (non-DS) ID (Hodapp et al., 2016; Stancliffe
et al., 2012), overall home-living percentages among adults with
Down syndrome exceeded percentages of others with ID. In all
three studies, such home-living percentages changed over age
groups, with the percentages of adults with Down syndrome (and
without Down syndrome) declining across the decades. Within
the Down syndrome groups, the most striking changes occurred
from the 20s30s to the 40s (and 50s) in the Hodapp et al. (2016)
and Tsao et al. (2015) studies, whereas the Stancliffe et al. (2012)
study showed differences from the 20s to the 30s, the 30s to the
40s, and the 40s to the 50s. Thus, although all studies showed similar declining percentages of home-living adults across age-groups
(and, for those studies reporting both groups, among those with
and without Down syndrome), the exact timing of changes differed slightly. Although not reported by decade, Woodman, Mailick, Anderson, and Esbensen (2014) also noted that, among those
who did move away from home, average ages of moving out of the
family home were younger among the Down syndrome vs. the

non-Down syndrome groups (although Woodman et al. also


found similar percentagesduring the mid-40sof each group in
each type of residential setting). Thus, though studies disagree to
some extent, adults with Down syndrome seem to more often
spend their 20s and 30s with their parents (possibly in higher percentages than do their non-Down syndrome age mates). Then, in
later decades, they move out of the family home.
A second recent suggestion concerns co-residing with siblings
after adults with Down syndrome leave their family homes. In
the Woodman et al. (2014) study noted above, high percentages
of adults with Down syndrome were reported to subsequently
reside with their siblings. Although Woodman et al. (2014) did
not specifically compare over 20 years time families of adults
with and without Down syndrome (only the group with Down
syndrome was followed over 20 years), After a 20-year period,
half of the adults with Down syndrome in family settings lived
with their adult siblings (Woodman et al., 2014, p. 508). Given
that the average age of these adults at final testing was 52 years,
only 35% lived with family members (either parents or siblings).
Of those who did live with relatives, however, half lived with
adult siblings. Thus, high percentages of adults with Down syndromeprobably between 10% and 20%ultimately live with
one of their adult siblings.
Although additional longitudinal and comparative studies are
neededand the few existing studies do differ to some extenta
reasonably clear picture still emerges. During their 20s and 30s,
most adults with Down syndrome seem fairly healthy, with stable
cognitive and adaptive functioning, and many work in the community. By their 50s, increasing percentages show specific health
conditions and fair-poor overall health, noticeable declines in
cognitive and adaptive skills, and few hold jobs in the community
(by the 50s, almost all hold either lower-level jobs-activities or
have no daily jobs or activities).
Important changes may also occur for parents, for where the
adult with Down syndrome lives, and involvement of siblings.
Thus, when parents care or oversee care for their 4050-year-old
offspring, they themselves are more often in their 70s or 80s and
presumably more often frail. The adults with Down syndrome,
who (compared with those without Down syndrome) more often
lived at home in their 20s and 30s, increasingly live away from
the family home. The familys other adult offspring, the siblings
of adults with Down syndrome, may also need to be involved
earlier, caring or overseeing care for their brothersister with
Down syndrome and their parents.
Implications for Practice, Policy, and Research
Although many implications could be drawn from the findings above, we, here, provide three policy recommendations,
along with some thoughts concerning future research.
Practical and Policy Recommendations
Supports for aging at non-old ages. Although a major
component of aging relates to planning for the future, most families of individuals with ID do not pursue future planning
(Freedman, Krauss, & Seltzer, 1997; Gilbert, Lankshear, &
Petersen, 2007; Heller & Factor, 1993; Weeks, Nilsson, Bryanton,
185

Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities

Volume 13 Number 2 June 2016

R. M. Hodapp et al. Family Caregiving of Aging Adults with Down Syndrome

& Kozma, 2009; see Lunsky, Tint, Robinson, Gordeyko, &


Ouellette-Kuntz, 2014 for a review). Families face many barriers
to future planning, including a lack of information, psychosocial
issues involved in addressing the topic, and trouble navigating
the service system. Yet without future planning, individuals with
ID are at risk for institutional care (McCann, Hebert, Bienias,
Morris, & Evans, 2004), losing benefits, and being in crisis situations (Heller & Caldwell, 2006). It is thus necessary for families
to begin future planning before a crisis occurs.
For individuals with Down syndrome and their families,
such aging-related issues need to be addressed at earlier ages.
As these adults often incur health and functional declines
beginning in their 40s (when parents are in their 70s or 80s),
future planning may need to occur while the adults are in their
30s and their parents are in their 60s. In addition, such future
planning should include individuals with Down syndrome
themselves. By educating these adults about future planning,
they can make more informed later-life decisions. Recognizing
the need to intervene earlier with adults with Down syndrome,
one training study of person-centered life planning included 60
adults with intellectual disabilities who were either 35 years or
older with Down syndrome or age 50 or older without Down
syndrome (Heller, Miller, Hsieh, & Sterns, 2000). Adults who
received the training (vs. those who did not) gained more
knowledge about future planning, made more choices over
time, and were more likely to reach their goals. Such interventions should be available for adults with Down syndrome when
they are in their 30s.
In addition, several existing services might focus more on
families of aging adults with Down syndrome. In the United
States, the Older Americans Act includes the National Family
Caregiver Support Program, which is intended to provide information, referral, counseling, and respite to carers. Unfortunately,
the Program is directed toward carers of older individuals (e.g.,
aging parents) or grandparents providing care to their children;
programming is not directed toward aging carers of individuals
with disabilities. The Older Americans Act also legislated the creation of Aging and Disability Resource Centers (ADRCs).
Although ADRCs help families navigate the service system, these
centers do not provide future planning services (though they do
provide web links to disability-related organizations; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2013). Considering the
findings noted in this paper, the National Family Caregiver Support Program and ADRCs should be expanded to provide information, training, and support about future planning to aging
carers of individuals with Down syndrome.
Cascade of problems in the family. Especially in families of
adults with Down syndrome, policymakers need to emphasize
the multiple health problems facing both the adult and his or her
parents. Among adults with ID more generally, Krahn, Hammond, and Turner (2006) have noted a cascade of health problems relating to disparities in (1) health conditions, (2) attention
to care needs, (3) preventative care and health promotion practices, and (4) access to health care. These same health issues also
affect adults with Down syndrome, albeit at earlier ages. But such
issues also inter-relate. As opposed to other 50-year-olds with
ID, 50-year-olds with Down syndrome may experience worse
186

health problems (Table 1) as well as a greater number of negative


life events (Patti et al., 2005). As Jokinen, Janicki, Hogan, and
Force (2012) note, even planned-for transitions do not always go
smoothly, as when moving to an out-of-home placement is
unsuccessful or when parents who had been proactive become ill
and lose control over the transition process. For all individuals
with intellectual disabilities, such life events may relate to
increased risks of depression (Tsanikos, Bouras, Costello, & Holt,
2007). Specifically, for adults with Down syndrome, connections
have also been noted between depression and lower levels of
active engagement in employment and social activities (Mallardo,
Cuskelly, White, & Jobling, 2014; also Dykens et al., 2015), as
well as between bereavement and subsequent behavioral changes
and cognitive declines (Fonseca, Oliveira, Guilhoto, Cavalheiro,
& Bottino, 2014). With parents facing increased risks of dying or
of experiencing functional-health declines, adults with Down
syndrome in their 40s and 50s may face a cascade of changes to
themselves and to their parents.
Triad of caregiving. Given the aging of society in most industrialized countries, caregiving is becoming an increasingly important topic. Examining the demography of the U.S. population in
the context of caregiving by the Baby Boom generation (i.e.,
those born between 194664), Rogerson and Kim (2005) illustrated the ways in which the timing of marriage, childrearing
(essentially, a 30-year period), and caring for ones parents
(which they describe as mostly starting at age 75) can bring about
different versions of the sandwich generation, the phenomenon
of simultaneously caring for ones children and for ones parents.
The sandwich generation also applies to families of adults
with Down syndrome, but with two important differences. First,
though most siblings of adults with a disability will belong to the
sandwich generation, many (especially females; Orsmond & Seltzer, 2000) also expect to provide future care for their brothersister with disabilities (Burke, Taylor, Urbano, & Hodapp, 2012). In
the case of Down syndrome, some siblings may fulfill all three
caregiving roles simultaneously, experiencing the triple-decker
sandwich of caring for their aging parent(s), their own offspring,
and their brothersister with Down syndrome.
Second, though siblings of adults with Down syndrome may
be assuming these multiple carer roles, family support policies in
most countries either overlook or have only recently begun to
consider siblings. Consider the Family Medical Leave Act
(FMLA) in the United States. Signed into law in 1993, the FMLA
allows family members to take up to 12 work weeks of unpaid
leave to attend to the health conditions of themselves or their
parents, spouses, or children. Until recently, the language did not
specify whether adult siblings could access FMLA to care for their
brotherssisters with Down syndrome. In July 2015, the Department of Labor further expanded the definitions and contexts for
FMLA protections. Specifically, the changes now explicitly state
that a child can refer to an individual older than 18 who is
incapable of self-care because of a mental or physical disability
(U.S. Department of Labor, 2015). Additionally, the language
clarifies that anyone who acts in the place of a parent could
access FMLA protections. Such language could mean that siblings
can now access FMLA.

Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities

Volume 13 Number 2 June 2016

R. M. Hodapp et al. Family Caregiving of Aging Adults with Down Syndrome

A further complication involves the higher rates of adults


with Down syndrome who live with their adult sibling (Woodman, et al., 2014). In light of increased rates of co-residence with
siblings, policymakers should examine in-home supports. In one
recent estimate, the United States spent $61.2 billion on supports
for individuals with ID, even as only 6.7% was allocated to family
support (Braddock et al., 2015). Given that, even compared with
others with ID, adults with Down syndrome more often live with
family members (either parents or siblings), it seems crucial that
more funding and services be directed toward support that is
centered on family members.
Research Implications
In describing this complex set of decade-by-decade changes,
we are aware of the weaknesses of the available research. On
some topics, few studies even exist. Given the impending caregiving crisis, not nearly enough studies exist on aging parents, and
few examine siblings when their brothersister with Down syndrome is in their 40s and 50s. Even among studies of adults with
Down syndrome, their parents, or their siblings, these studies
vary in numbers of participants at various ages, with often only a
few at the later ages. Studies vary in whether they are crosssectional or longitudinal, in how recently they were conducted,
and in whether functioning levels of adults with Down syndrome
were directly tested or reported by a carer or sibling. Studies also
span a variety of industrialized countries, including the United
States, United Kingdom, the Netherlands, Australia, and France.
Beyond the scarcity of research, we also acknowledge that
studies focused exclusively on families of adults with Down syndrome run counter to several influential recommendations. In
looking toward the future of health research, for example, the
World Report on Disability (World Health Organization, 2011)
advocates that, wherever possible, mainstream research should
be performed, or research that examines the general population
and includes some individuals with disabilities. From the findings noted above, however, it may be essential to study families
of aging adults with Down syndrome. By comparing those aging
individuals vs. those without Down syndrome and their families,
we can better differentiate the changes and issues that are common to all families of aging individuals with intellectual disabilities, and those that seem unique to Down syndrome.
Finally, these findings strongly suggest that researchers
should more often employ a wider family perspective. In characterizing the decades of the 40s and 50s for adults with Down
syndrome and their families, changes are not confined to a single domain or to a single person (Janicki, Zendell, & DeHaven,
2010). For the adults themselves, findings relate to health, cognitive, adaptive, and vocational functioning; for parents, to their
own aging-related changes, which are exacerbated by offspring
who more often live at home and show old-age problems during
the middle-age years; for siblings, to family ties that foster caregiving by adult (mostly female) siblings for their brotherssisters with disabilities when parents can no longer do so. In short,
family caregiving during adulthood involves changes in multiple
domains across multiple people, each of which affects the person who is changing as well as everyone else.

Conclusion
Across the field of disability, the increased life expectancy of
persons with Down syndrome is among the most profound and
exciting of all changes. Like all changes, however, such increased
longevity brings forth its own, often unexpected implications. In
Down syndrome, these changes involve three sets of people:
adults with Down syndrome, their mothers and fathers, and one
or more adult siblings. Such changes involve health and functional declines of the offspring with Down syndrome and of the
parents, both occurring when adults with Down syndrome are in
their late 40s and 50s.
As with any societal change, we now need to embark on policies and practices to help these families cope in the best way possible. As an early attempt, this article documents changes that
routinely occur for aging adults with Down syndrome and their
families; more detailed studies will be needed in the future. But
beyond documenting these aging-related changes, we also need
to develop and implement policies to aid these families. Although
the specifics of such policies and practices will differ from one
country to another, all will necessarily involve addressing family
caregiving at earlier ages, more fully utilizing future planning,
and providing help and supports for aging adults with Down
syndrome, their parents, and their siblings.
References
Braddock, B., Hemp, R., Rizzolo, M. C., Tanis, E. S., Haffer, L., & Wu, J.
(2015). The state of the states in developmental disabilities (10th ed.).
Washington, DC: American Association on Intellectual and
Developmental Disabilities.
Burke, M. M., Taylor, J. L., Urbano, R. C., & Hodapp, R. M. (2012).
Predictors of future caregiving by adult siblings of individuals with
intellectual and developmental disabilities. American Journal on
Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 117, 3347.
Carr, J., & Collins, S. (2014). Ageing and dementia in a longitudinal
study of a cohort with Down syndrome. Journal of Applied Research
in Intellectual Disabilities, 27, 555563.
Collacott, R. A., & Cooper, S. A. (1997). A five-year follow up study of
adaptive behaviour in adults with Down syndrome. Journal of
Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 22, 187197.
Congenital Anomaly Statistics, England and Wales (Series MB3). (2010).
Official National Statistics, 2010. Retrieved from http://www.ons.gov.
uk/ons/rel/vsob1/congenital-anomaly-statisticsengland-and-wales
series-mb3-/no232008/index.html.
Dykens, E. M., Shah, B., Davis, B., Baker, C., Fife, T., & Fitzpatrick, J.
(2015). Psychiatric disorders in adolescents and young adults with
Down syndrome and other intellectual disabilities. Journal of
Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 7, 18.
Esbensen, A. J., Mailick, M. R., & Silverman, W. (2013). Long-term
impact of parental well-being on adult outcomes and dementia
status in individuals with Down syndrome. American Journal on
Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 118, 294309.
Fogelman, C. J. (1975). AAMD adaptive behavior scale manual.
Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Deficiency.
Fonseca, L. M., de Oliveira, M. C., de Figueiredo, L. M., Guilhoto, F.,
Cavalheiro, E. A., & Bottino, C. M. (2014). Bereavement and
behavioral changes as risk factors for cognitive decline in adults with
Down syndrome. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 10, 2209
2219. http://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S68831
187

Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities

Volume 13 Number 2 June 2016

R. M. Hodapp et al. Family Caregiving of Aging Adults with Down Syndrome

Freedman, R., Krauss, M., & Seltzer, M. M. (1997). Aging parents


residential plans for adults with mental retardation. Mental
Retardation, 35, 114123.
Fujiura, G.T. (2014). The political arithmetic of disability and the
American family: A demographic perspective. Family Relations, 63,
719.
Gilbert, A., Lankshear, G., & Petersen, A. (2007). Older family-carers
views on the future accommodation needs of relatives who have an
intellectual disability. International Journal of Social Welfare, 17, 54
64.
Hawkins, B. A., Eklund, S. J., James, D. R., & Foose, A. K. (2003).
Adaptive behavior and cognitive function of adults with Down
syndrome: Modeling change with age. Mental Retardation, 41, 728.
Heller, T., & Caldwell, J. (2006). Supporting aging caregivers and adults
with developmental disabilities in future planning. Mental
Retardation, 44, 189202.
Heller, T., & Factor, A. (1993). Aging family caregivers: Support resources
and changes in burden and placement desire. American Journal of
Mental Retardation, 98, 417426.
Heller, T., Miller, A. B., Hsieh, K., & Sterns, H. (2000). Later-life
planning: Promoting knowledge of options and choice-making.
Mental Retardation, 38, 395406.
Hewitt, A. (2014). Embracing complexity: Community inclusion,
participation, and citizenship. Intellectual and Developmental
Disabilities, 52, 475495.
Hodapp, R. M., Burke, M. M., & Urbano, R. C. (2012). Whats age got to
do with it? Implications of maternal age on families of offspring with
Down syndrome. International Review of Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 42, 109145.
Hodapp, R. M., Finley, C. I., & Urbano, R. C. (2016). Changes during the
40s for adults with Down syndrome, their parents, and siblings.
Unpublished manuscript, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN.
Hodapp, R. M., & Urbano, R. C. (2007). Adult siblings of individuals
with Down syndrome versus with autism: Findings from a largescale American survey. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51,
10181029.
Horvath, S., Garagnani, P., Bacalini, M. G., Pirazzini, C., Salvioli, S.,
Gentilini, D., . . . Franceschi, C. (2015). Accelerated epigenetic aging
in Down syndrome. Aging Cell, 14, 15.
Idler, E. L., & Benyamini, Y. (1997). Self-rated health and mortality: A
review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health &
Social Behavior, 38, 2137.
Janicki, M. P., Dalton, A. J., Henderson, C. M., & Davidson, P. W. (1999).
Mortality and morbidity among older adults with intellectual
disability: Health service considerations. Disability and Rehabilitation, 21, 284294.
Janicki, M. P, Zendell, A., & DeHaven, K. (2010). Coping with dementia
and older families of adults with Down syndrome. Dementia, 9, 391
407.
Jokinen, N. S., Janicki, M. P., Hogan, M., & Force, L. T. (2012). The
middle years and beyond: Transitions and families of adults with
Down syndrome. Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 18, 5969.
Krahn, G. L., Hammond, L., & Turner, A. (2006). A cascade of
disparities: Health and health care access for people with intellectual
disabilities. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 12,
7082.
Lunsky, Y., Tint, A., Robinson, S., Gordeyko, M., & Ouellette-Kuntz, H.
(2014). System-wide information about family carers of adults with
intellectual/developmental disabilities: A scoping review of the
literature. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 11,
818.
Maaskant, M. A., van den Akker, M., Kessels, A. G. H., Haveman, M. J.,
van Schrojenstein Lantman-de Valk, H. M. J., & Urlings, H. F. J.
(1996). Care dependence and activities of daily living in relation to
188

ageing: Results of a longitudinal study. Journal of Intellectual


Disability Research, 40, 535543.
Mallardo, M., Cuskelly, M., White, P., & Jobling, A. (2014). Mental
health problems in adults with Down syndrome and their
association with life circumstances. Journal of Mental Health Research
in Intellectual Disabilities, 7, 229245.
Martin, J., Hamilton, B., Osterman, M., Curtin, M., & Mathews, M.
(2015). Births: Final data for 2013Internet TABLES. In: National
Vital Statistics Reports. Vol. 1 (Supplement). Retrieved from http://
www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nvsr/nvsr64/nvsr64_01_tables.pdf.
McCann, J. J., Hebert, L. E., Bienias, J. L., Morris, M. C., & Evans, D. A.
(2004). Predictors of beginning of and ending caregiving during a 3year period in a biracial community population of older adults.
American Journal of Public Health, 94, 18001806.
McLanahan, S. (2004). Diverging destinies: How children are faring
under the second demographic transition. Demography, 41,
607627.
National Council on Disability. (2005). The state of 21st century longterm services and supports: Financing and systems reform for
Americans with disabilities. Washington, DC: Author.
Oliver, C., Crayton, L., Holland, A., Hall, S., & Bradbury, J. (1998). A
four-year prospective study of age-related cognitive change in adults
with Downs syndrome. Psychological Medicine, 28, 13651377.
Orsmond, G. I., & Seltzer, M. M. (2000). Brothers and sisters of adults
with mental retardation: Gendered nature of the sibling relationship.
American Journal on Mental Retardation, 105, 486508.
Patti, P. J., Amble, K. B., & Flory, M. J. (2005). Life events in older adults
with intellectual disabilities: Differences between adults with and
without Down syndrome. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual
Disabilities, 2, 149155.
Penrose, L. S. (1949). The incidence of Mongolism in the general
population. Journal of Mental Science, 95, 685688.
Rogerson, P. A., & Kim, D. (2005). Population distribution and
redistribution of the baby-boom cohort in the United States:
Recent trends and implications. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 15319
15324.
Rondal, J. A. (2009). Psycholinguistique du handicap mental [Psycholinguistics of mental disability]. Marseilles, France: Solal.
Stancliffe, R. J., Lakin, C. K., Larsen, S. A., Engler, J., Taub, S., Fortune, J.,
& Bershadsky, J. (2012). Demographic characteristics, health
conditions, and residential service use in adults with Down syndrome in 25 U.S. states. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities,
50, 92108.
Staples, A. J., Sutherland, G. R., Haan, E. A., & Clisby, S. (1991).
Epidemiology of Down syndrome in South Australia, 1960-89.
American Journal of Human Genetics, 49, 10141024.
Taylor, J. L., & Hodapp, R. M. (2012). Doing nothing: Adults
with disabilities with no daily activities and their siblings.
American Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities,
117, 6779.
Torr, J., Strydom, A., Patti, P., & Jokinen, N. (2010). Aging in Down
syndrome: Morbidity and mortality. Journal of Policy and Practice in
Intellectual Disabilities. 7, 7081.
Tsao, R., Kindelberger, C., Freminville, B., Touraine, R., & Bussy, G.
(2015). Variability of the aging process in dementia-free adults with
Down syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental
Disabilities, 120, 315.
Tsanikos, E., Bouras, N., Costello, H., & Holt, G. (2007). Multiple
exposure to life events and clinical psychopathology in adults with
intellectual disability. Social Psychiatry: Psychiatric Epidemiology, 42,
2428.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2013). Administration
for community living, help and resources, people with disabilities. Re-

Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities

Volume 13 Number 2 June 2016

R. M. Hodapp et al. Family Caregiving of Aging Adults with Down Syndrome

trieved from www.acl.gov/Get_Help/Help_Indiv_Disabilities/Index.


aspx.
U.S. Department of Labor. (2015). Fact Sheet #28K: Son or Daughter 18
years of age and older under the Family and Medical Leave Act.
Retrieved from http://www.dol.gov/whd/regs/compliance/whdfs28k.
pdf.
Van Schrojenstein Lantman-deValk, H. M. J., van den Akker, M.,
Maaskant, M. A., Haveman, M. J., Urlings, H. F. J., Kessels, A. G. H.,
& Crebolder, H. F. J. M. (1997). Prevalence and incidence of health
problems in people with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual
Disability Research, 41, 4251.
Weeks, L. E., Nilsson, T., Bryanton, O., & Kozma, A. (2009). Current and
future concerns of older parents of sons and daughters with
intellectual disabilities. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual
Disabilities, 6, 180188.

Woodman, A. C., Mailick, M. R., Anderson, K. A., & Esbensen, A.


(2014). Residential transitions among adults with intellectual
disability across 20 years. American Journal on Intellectual and
Developmental Disabilities, 119, 496515.
World Health Organization. (2011). World report on disability. Retrieved
from http://whqlibdoc.who.int/publications/2011/9789240685215_eng.
pdf?ua51.
Yang, Q., Rasmussen, S. A., & Friedman, J. M. (2002). Mortality
associated with Downs syndrome in the USA from 1983 to 1997: A
population-based study. Lancet, 359, 10191025.
Zigman, W. B. (2013). Atypical aging in Down syndrome. Developmental
Disabilities Research Reviews, 18, 5167.
Zigman, W. B., Schupf, N., Urv, T., Zigman, A., & Silverman, W. (2002).
Incidence and temporal patterns of adaptive behavior change in
adults with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental
Retardation, 107, 161174.

189

You might also like