You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 8 (2001) 251}260

Consumers use of price promotions: a model and its potential


moderators
Michel Laroche *, Frank Pons , Nadia Zgolli , Chankon Kim
Department of Marketing, Faculty of Commerce and Administration, Concordia University, 1455 de Maisonneuve West, Montreal,
Que& bec, Canada H3G 1M8
Saint Mary's University, Halifax, NS, Canada

Abstract

In the context of consumers' use of price-oriented promotions, a multidimensional model, which follows a cognitive}a!ective-
behavior pattern was proposed. This model integrates all the aspects of how and why consumers use sales promotions and speci"es
the linkage between consumers' beliefs, attitude and behavior. The cognitive component of the pattern included two dimensions: (a)
the overall evaluation of the bene"ts triggered by the use of the promotion and (b) the information search about the potential price
promotions. It is posited to in#uence the degree of liking of speci"c price promotions as well as the liking of deals in general. This
a!ective component is then posited to in#uence behavioral intentions towards price promotions and stockpiling. A structural
equation modeling analysis (SEM) o!ers relatively strong support for the expected paths. Moreover, the potential in#uences on this
pattern of consumers' traits are included. Busyness, variety seeking, perceived "nancial wellness, market maven and brand loyalty are
posited to have speci"c in#uences on the three components of the pattern. Results are presented and discussed.  2001 Elsevier
Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Price-oriented; Promotions; Sales promotion; Lifestyle; Structural equation modeling

1. Introduction an attempt to identify and understand the deal-prone


consumer. Nevertheless, "ndings pertaining to the sales
Sales promotions have constituted an increasing pro- promotions context have somewhat failed to clarify
portion of the promotional budgets in recent years. Don- correlates of deal proneness. The main criticisms lie in
nelley's survey (Donnelley Marketing, 1994) found that the inability of previous studies to consider deals from
packaged goods manufacturers' spending on consumer the consumer's perspective (Shimp and Kavas, 1984),
sales promotions now exceeds the amount spent on but also in the fact they use single-product categories
advertising and that "rms are using on average more for the assessment of deal proneness (Henderson, 1987)
than eight di!erent types of consumer sales promotions. and "nally in their failure to treat deal proneness as
Several authors (Blattberg and Neslin, 1990; Schneider a latent construct (Lichtenstein et al., 1990) that may
and Currim, 1991; Lichtenstein et al., 1995) identi"ed be domain speci"c. Moreover, the lack of distinction
di!erent categories of consumer-oriented sales promo- made among the attitudinal and behavioral constructs
tions and price promotions were often considered as the when measuring the deal proneness concept is also a
main focus of these studies. Indeed, price reduction is the crucial issue in sales promotion studies (Lichtenstein
most cited bene"t of consumer sales promotions and et al., 1990).
a substantial variance can be found across sales promo- In fact, Lichtenstein et al. (1995, 1998) argue that the
tions with respect to this bene"t (Blattberg and Neslin, traditional behavioral-level measurement of coupon
1990; Lichtenstein et al., 1995). Because of the continuous proneness was unable to fully describe the consumer's
growing importance of sales promotions, considerable perspective on coupon's use and that a psychological-
research has been directed to study their mechanisms in level of measurement would be more desirable in order
to provide a better understanding of the coupon re-
demption process. More speci"cally, investigations
* Corresponding author. Tel.:#1-514-848-2942; fax:#1-514-848-4576. into cognitive and a!ective e!ects of coupons have been
E-mail address: laroche@mercato.concordia.ca (M. Laroche). surprisingly understudied in the past (Shimp and Kavas,

0969-6989/01/$ - see front matter  2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 9 6 9 - 6 9 8 9 ( 0 0 ) 0 0 0 3 1 - X
252 M. Laroche et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 8 (2001) 251}260

1984; Hahn et al., 1995) but would certainly give a clearer the trade by inducing consumers to take more immediate
hint on how the promotion use is initiated. action.
Furthermore, results of previous studies indicate that Sales promotions can be classi"ed as price or non-
ambiguity surrounds the impact of traits and demo- price promotions (Lichtenstein et al., 1995). The two
graphic variables on the portrait of the deal prone mainly used price promotion techniques are the coupons
consumer (Bawa and Shoemaker, 1989; Blattberg et al., and the cents-o!. The coupon is a promotional tool that
1978). The extent of relationships that are reported in requires consumers to redeem the promotion (Shimp,
most of these studies tend to be inconsistent and unclear. 1993). Coupons can be disseminated to consumers
As stated by Blattberg and Neslin (1990), `the deal prone through newspapers and magazines, direct mail and
literature has yielded some important de"nitive results, packages with di!erent outcomes in terms of capturing
but they are far from completely understooda. Therefore, the consumer's attention and overcoming the competi-
the challenge for researchers is twofold. First, they have tive clutter.
to better identify those consumers who possess the deal- The cents-o! promotions are reductions in a brand's
prone trait; but moreover, they have to understand how regular price. They are o!ered right on the package and
demographics and lifestyles can impact the coupon atti- typically range from 10 to 25% o! the regular price for
tude/use process. grocery packaged goods.
The purpose of this study is to de"ne the consumers' Several researchers suggest that sensitivity to di!erent
deal usage process and to examine the potential in#uen- types of sales promotions might explain di!erent promo-
ces of traits variables on this process. A multidimensional tional responses (Schneider and Currim, 1991; Hender-
model, based on the hierarchy of e!ects model (Rosen- son, 1987). Following this rationale, in our study, two
berg, 1956) is drawn from previous research results and promotional mechanisms supposed to trigger di!erent
hypothesized. Particularly, we contrast the process for consumer responses were included in the model as
two di!erent price promotions } coupons and cents-o! separate constructs: coupons and cents-o!. In addition,
promotions * that are supposed to trigger di!erent following the view that there are su$cient similarities in
promotional responses and we compare them with the consumers' behavior from one deal mechanism to an-
process for deals in general (domain speci"c versus do- other to consider deal-responsive behavior as a generaliz-
main general). The results are then discussed, and several ed construct (Shimp and Kavas, 1984; Lichtenstein et al.,
implications are detailed. 1997), we also include in the model a generalized con-
struct measuring the attitude toward deals in general.

2. Literature review and hypotheses 2.2. Price perception and price-oriented sales promotions:
toward a cognitive process
2.1. Overview of sales promotion
Several recent studies tend to support the presence of
Sales promotions encompasses all promotional activ- mental processing made by the consumer as the initial
ities other than advertising, personal selling, or public stage of promotion use, particularly regarding the in#u-
relations. Blattberg and Neslin (1990) summarize the ence of the promotion on the price perception of the
various de"nitions o!ered by several authors (Kotler, purchase (Lichtenstein et al., 1995, 1998; Folkes and
1988; Webster, 1965; Schultz and Robinson, 1982), and Wheat, 1995; Cheong, 1993). Price perception theories
consider sales promotions as `an action-focused market- study the relationship between objective price and con-
ing event whose purpose is to have a direct impact on the sumers' judgments of the price (Dickson, 1983; Sawyer
behavior of the "rm's customersa. et al., 1984).
Several important aspects of sales promotions should Price promotions signi"cantly a!ect consumers' price
be highlighted to complete the de"nition mentioned perception (Folkes and Wheat, 1995). O!ering a product
above. First, sales promotions involve some type of with a rebate results in higher perceptions as measured
inducement that provides an extra incentive to buy by the most one would pay, expected price, fair price and
(Schultz and Robinson, 1982) and represents the key reasonable price. This di!erence is explained from
element in a promotional program. According to Strang a Mental Accounting perspective (Thaler, 1985). Kalwani
(1983), this incentive is added to the basic bene"ts and Yim (1992) also explored the "eld of consumers' price
provided by the brand. It temporarily alters the per- expectations. Their study proposes that consumers form
ceived brand price or value. It is also considered as expectations of prices and use them in formulating their
an acceleration tool designed to speed up the selling response to retail pricing. Their "ndings reveal that con-
process and maximize sales volume (Neslin et al., sumer reaction depends not only on retail price, but also
1984). By o!ering this extra incentive, sales promotion on the comparison they make with the reservation price.
techniques can motivate consumers to buy a larger In other words, consumers use the price they expect to
quantity of a brand or shorten the interpurchase time of pay for a brand on a given purchase occasion as reference
M. Laroche et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 8 (2001) 251}260 253

in forming price judgments. Therefore, even if di!erent classes, they found statistically signi"cant evidence of
processes have been hypothesized, there is a consensus purchase acceleration in terms of both larger quantities
on the existence of a cognitive process. This process can and shorter interpurchase times. Raju and Hastak (1983),
be described as a cognitive evaluation, made by the who examined the e!ect of deal's magnitude on the
consumers, of the bene"ts of a price promotion. More- purchase behavior due to promotions, con"rmed these
over, this idea is developed by Lichtenstein et al. (1998), results. At the individual level, however, price pro-
who "nd similarities between consumers with positive motions seemed to act as a disturbing element, which
attitude toward private label products and consumers inhibits negative thoughts that might arise about the
using price promotions, particularly regarding the `men- brand. Also, Neslin et al. (1984) expanded this empirical
tal calculationa involved. They describe these consumers line of inquiry by developing an analytical framework for
as `thoughtful shoppers who take pride in their deci- studying purchase acceleration.
sion-making abilitya. These evaluations made by the Their main "ndings concern how coupons, temporary
consumers positively in#uence the purchase intention of price cuts, and featured price cuts were all associated with
private label products through the liking of these private higher purchase quantities. In summary, there is a good
labels. Similarly, the same cognitive}a!ective-behavioral deal of empirical support for an increase in purchase
pattern can be hypothesized in the case of price pro- quantity due to sales promotions in the consumer
motions. Consequently, the following hypotheses are packaged goods area, supporting the concept of potential
proposed: stockpiling (Helsen and Schmittlein, 1992). Also, as sug-
gested by Lichtenstein et al. (1995, 1998), deal-prone
H1: Consumers' evaluations of promotions bene"ts pos- consumers tend to develop links between their liking of
itively in#uence their liking of (a) coupons (b) cents- speci"c price promotions and their inclination to buy
o! and (c) deals in general. products using these promotions. Indeed, the notion of
Finally, certain promotional mechanisms such as cou- transaction utility dictates the behavior of the consumer.
pons inherently require `searchinga costs (Schneider and It allows a particular psychological inducement (i.e., feel-
Currim, 1991). Indeed, coupon-users are used to engage ing good about using the promotion) that plays
in coupon search and sort (Bawa and Shoemaker, 1987). the major role in directing the behavior. Consequently,
Particularly, the liking of coupons seems correlated with the following hypotheses are proposed:
speci"c behavior such as intensive use of weekly store
H3: Consumers' liking of (a) coupons, (b) cents-o! and
#iers containing coupons as well as information on sales
(c) deals in general positively in#uence their pur-
(Lichtenstein et al., 1995). Hence, using a coupon and
chase intention of the promoted product.
identifying cents-o! are thoughtful decisions and
H4: Consumers' liking of (a) coupons, (b) cents-o! and
involve information search costs (Kahn and Schmittlein,
(c) deals in general positively in#uence their stock-
1992). Moreover, Kalwani and Yim (1992) showed that
piling intention.
consumers tend to use the last few purchase prices as
a reference, as well as readily available information from
the environment concerning promotions. This infor- 2.4. Potential traits inyuences on the
mation included circulars as well as other advertising cognitive}awective}conative pattern
information. Consequently, the following hypotheses can
be proposed: From managerial and academic perspectives, a tre-
mendous e!ort has been made to de"ne the `deal-pronea
H2: Consumers' information search about promotions consumer. The targeting of consumer deals, as well as the
positively in#uences their liking of (a) coupons understanding of consumer behavior would be improved
(b) cents-o! and (c) deals in general. by the knowledge of the consumers' characteristics that
relate to deal purchasing in a product market.
2.3. Liking of price-oriented promotions and deals: Mittal (1994) presented a set of explanatory variables
ewects on purchase intentions to capture the psychology of coupon-use behaviors. His
model regrouped demographic variables, lifestyles and
Shoemaker (1979) was one of the "rst to investigate the self-perception traits (busyness, "nancial wellness, and
e!ects of promotions on purchase behavior. He empiric- pride in homemaking). Particularly, he underlined the
ally showed that purchase acceleration was due to sales fact that busy and well-o! people were buying fewer
promotion. His "ndings suggested that promotions were promoted products than their counterpart. He also re-
more apt to be associated with increased quantity than viewed some of the consumer shopping traits, such as
with shorter interpurchase time. Then, Blattberg et al. brand loyalty, store loyalty and comparison-shopping.
(1981) tried to explain the dealing of storable products One of his main "ndings was that coupon redemption-
based on the idea of transferring inventory-carrying costs behavior and demographics were not linked in any
from the retailer to the consumer. For four product way. This "nding contradicted previous "ndings
254 M. Laroche et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 8 (2001) 251}260

(Montgomery, 1971; Blattberg et al., 1978; Teel et al., pons, (e) liking of cents-o! and (f) liking of deals in
1980; Bawa and Shoemaker, 1987, Narasimhan, 1984). general.
However, consumers' shopping traits did have a signi"-
H9: Financial wellness negatively in#uences consumers'
cant e!ect in explaining coupon redemption.
(a) purchase intentions of coupons promoted prod-
The brand loyalty trait is probably the most studied
ucts, (b) purchase intentions of cents-o! promoted
individual-di!erence variable in the promotional litera-
products and (c) stockpiling intentions.
ture. Results are consistent in suggesting that brand
loyalty negatively a!ects deal or coupon attitude and use The hypotheses concerning the potential in#uence of
(Webster, 1965; Montgomery, 1971). traits variables on the model as well as the general
Price et al. (1988) included another personality trait in process of promotion use itself are summarized in Fig. 1.
the study of coupon proneness, which is `market maven-
isma. The authors de"ned the market mavens as the
people who possess information about the market prod- 3. Method
ucts, places to shop, and who provide other consumers
with market information. These consumers are charac- 3.1. Sample and data collection
terized by their expertise, in that they plan their shopping
trips, their expenses and are heavy users of coupons. The The population targeted for this survey consisted in
results of this work indicated that this type of extra- residents living in the Greater metropolitan area of
consumers was likely to engage in `smart shoppinga a North American city. In order to ensure a representa-
behaviors. Urbany et al. (1996) found a positive e!ect of tive sample, the data collection was con"ned to a selected
market mavenism on search behaviors and cognitive number of census tracts located in the city and its sur-
activities. rounding area. A sample of at least 200 usable question-
An additional trait variable, which could signi"cantly naires was deemed appropriate for this research to give
in#uence promotion use process, is the variety-seeking a respectable measure of validity and reliability.
trait. In particular, when consumers seek variety, they Based on previous surveys, a usable return rate of 30%
should like promotions that might help them discover was expected. Within each of the census tracts, a quota of
new products (Narasimhan, 1984). questionnaires to be distributed was established propor-
Lichteinstein et al. (1990) were among the "rst to tioned to the relative population of the particular census
describe moderators of the relationship between coupon tract. The questionnaires were administered door to door
proneness and coupon redemption behavior. They and a prepaid envelope was provided to respondents to
developed a two-staged model in which a psychological allow them to mail the completed questionnaire at their
level in#uenced the coupon-responsive behavior rather convenience. A total of 898 questionnaires were distrib-
than seeing it as `isomorphic with the behaviora. Consid- uted, 228 usable questionnaires were returned for an
ering this approach, the impact of the potential variables average response rate of 25.4%. Among the respondents,
will be considered at each speci"c component level of the the initial repartition of quotas was fairly respected. The
model (cognitive, a!ective and conative). Moreover, to majority of the respondents for this survey were female
avoid the controversy about demographic variables, no (about 64%). This is adequate for the study's objective
speci"c hypothesis will be made concerning these vari- since it has been reported in prior research that women
ables. Therefore, the following hypotheses are proposed: are the primary shoppers in the family unit.

H5: Market mavens positively in#uence (a) consumers' 3.2. Measurement


information search about promotions and (b) their
evaluations of promotion bene"ts. The questionnaire was designed in order to obtain
detailed data concerning the cognitive, a!ective and con-
H6: Variety seeking positively in#uences consumers' lik-
ative components of the process describing consumers'
ing of (a) coupons, (b) cents-o! and (c) deals in
use of price promotions. The data were collected for the
general.
two previously introduced types of price promotions
H7: Busyness negatively in#uences consumers' (a) pur- (coupons and cents-o!) as well as for deals in general. For
chase intentions of coupons promoted products, clari"cation, a de"nition of each deal option was given to
(b) purchase intentions of cents-o! promoted prod- respondents as a reference in the cover letter accompany-
ucts and (c) stockpiling intentions. ing the questionnaire.
The measures were taken from the study conducted by
H8: Brand loyalty negatively in#uences consumers' Lichtenstein et al. (1997) in which they developed separ-
(a) purchase intentions of coupons promoted prod- ate multi-item scales for each speci"c deal proneness. The
ucts, (b) purchase intentions of cents-o! promoted procedures used to develop these scales closely adhered
products, (c) stockpiling intentions, (d) liking of cou- to procedures recommended in the scale development
M. Laroche et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 8 (2001) 251}260 255

Fig. 1. Consumers use of price promotions: an attitudinal model

literature and support the argument of validity of these 4. Results


scales. Moreover, other measures previously introduced
were included such as economic bene"ts (Mittal, 1994), 4.1. Overview
stockpiling (Helsen and Schmittlein, 1992) and one-item
scales to quantify the usage of deals in the last month. Based on the literature review, the hypotheses have
The whole set of variables was then separated into three been conceptualized in the structural model presented in
subsets, namely cognitive, a!ective and conative vari- Fig. 1. The di!erent paths of the model refer to the
ables. In order to reduce the data into a smaller and more various hypotheses and include the cognitive}a!ec-
meaningful set of components and to improve the relia- tive}conative pattern and the potential in#uence of some
bility of the factors in each subset, several puri"cation traits variables on this pattern. Therefore, the overall
steps (exploratory factor analyses and deletion of items) estimation of the speci"ed model as well as the paths
were run before obtaining meaningful results. A reliabil- allows the test of the hypotheses.
ity check was then performed on the "nal sample of items The model estimation was performed using EQS soft-
for each factor. The results, as well as some sample items ware. Di!erent indicators can be used to assess the over-
are presented in Table 1. all "t qualities of the model. If the  value is historically
The questionnaire also included a section dedicated to the initial estimator of one model's "t (Bollen, 1989;
the collection of respondent traits and demographics. Browne, 1989), its sensibility to sample sizes and dis-
The purchase-related traits included busyness, perceived tributions have oriented researchers towards modi"ed
"nancial wellness, market maven, variety-seeking and versions of the  (Anderson and Gerbing, 1988; Tanaka,
brand loyalty. All these measures were chosen from pre- 1993). The standardized  (/degrees of freedom) rep-
vious studies on lifestyles (e.g., Arnold et al., 1983), and resents the "rst alternative. The acceptance criterion
from other studies related to deal proneness and attitudes ranges between 4 and 5 for the least stringent methods
of households towards sales promotion (Mittal, 1994; (Wheaton et al., 1977) and between 2 and 3 for the strict
Price et al., 1988). Items were measured on a 9-point approaches (Carmines and Mac Iver, 1981). Another "t
Likert scale (1"`disagree stronglya to 9"`agree indicator, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), shows the
stronglya) for all the statements. A reliability check was signi"cance of the hypothesized model. The cuto! value
also performed for all factors and is presented in Table 1. suggested for adequate "t is 0.90 (Bentler and Bonett,
Finally, following the Farnell and Larcker procedure 1980). Finally, the standardized value and the signi"-
(1981), the discriminant and convergent validity of the cance (t-value) of the estimated measurement parameters
measure instrument have been assessed. (paths) are given.
256 M. Laroche et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 8 (2001) 251}260

Table 1
Reliability check and items samples

Description Item Cronbach alpha

Cognitive
Information search in supermarket #iers (3 items) I use grocery store circular/#ier advertisements to decide which store 0.85
I will patronize.
Prior to shopping, I check all the #iers for promoted brands and prices.
Promotions bene"ts evaluation (3 items) Promotions can save me a lot of money. 0.80
Cents-o! promotions can save a shopper a lot of money.
Awective
Liking `Cents-o!a (2 items) I enjoy buying products with cents-o! promotions, regardless of the 0.85
amount I save by doing so.
Buying products with cents-o! promotions makes me feel good.
Liking coupons (3 items) I enjoy clipping coupons out of newspapers or magazines. 0.87
Redeeming coupons makes me feel good.
Liking deals in general (3 items) I enjoy buying brands that are promoted. 0.77
Receiving a promotional o!er with a product purchase makes me feel
like I am a good shopper.
Conative
Behavioral intention toward `Cents-o! a (3 items) During the last month, how many times did you buy products o!ered 0.80
with a `Cents-o!a promotion?
I am more likely to buy a brand if it has a Cent-O! promotion on
the label.
Behavioral intention toward coupons (3 items) I am more likely to buy brands for which I have a coupon. 0.79
Approximately how many coupons do you redeem in a month?
Behavioral intention toward stockpiling (3 items) If a product that I usually buy is being promoted, I will purchase it 0.84
even if I do not need it for immediate use.
If a product that I usually buy is being promoted, I will increase the
quantity of my purchases (i.e., stock up).
Lifestyles and traits
Market maven (3 items) People come to me more often than I go to them for information 0.82
on brands.
My friends think of me as a good source of information when it comes
to new products or sales.
Brand loyalty (4 items) Once I "nd a brand I like I tend to stick with it. 0.77
For most supermarket items, I have favorite brands and limit my
purchasing to them.
Store loyalty (2 items) I do most of my shopping in the stores I have always shopped in. 0.85
Once I get used to where things are in a supermarket, I hate to change
stores.
Perceived "nancial wellness (3 items) I am generally on a tight budget. 0.75
I consider myself "nancially well o!.
Busyness (3 items) `So much to do, so little timea, this saying applies very well to me. 0.76
I am too busy to relax.
Variety seeking (3 items) I often buy other brands to see if they are better than the brand(s) 0.76
I usually buy.
I often try new brands before my friends and neighbors do.

4.2. Test of the hypotheses not supported. Therefore, a scrutiny of the results is
necessary in order to fully understand the hypothesized
The "t indicators of the overall model, the estimated pattern. It will be presented in the following discussion.
structural parameters, their statistical signi"cance and
the associated tests of hypotheses are reported in Table 2.
The overall "t indicators of the model are good and 5. Discussion
therefore we can consider that the proposed model "ts
the data adequately. Consequently, the results support First, even if the expected cognitive}a!ective}conative
the pertinence of the expected cognitive}a!ective}con- model was con"rmed by the general results, based on
ative pattern as well as the potential role played by traits di!erent interpretations of the existing literature, poten-
variables in the use made by consumers of price pro- tial existence of reversed paths for the "rst 3 hypo-
motions. Nevertheless, some of the hypotheses made theses of the study could have been questioned. In order
regarding the potential impact of traits variables are to avoid any doubt about reciprocal paths, a causal
M. Laroche et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 8 (2001) 251}260 257

Table 2
Model "t results and evaluation of the tested structural paths

Fit indicators

CFI 0.928
/df 1.54
Path tested Standardized estimate Test of hypotheses
(t-value)
CognitivePawective pattern
Info. search on liking coupons 0.544 (8.2) H2a supported
Info. search on liking Cents-o! 0.282 (4.2) H2b supported
Info. search on liking deals 0.150 (2.1) H2c supported
Bene"ts evaluation on liking coupons 0.432 (5.1) H1a supported
Bene"ts evaluation on liking cents-o! 0.556 (5.3) H1b supported
Bene"ts evaluation on liking deals 0.562 (4.7) H1c supported
AwectivePconative pattern
Liking coupons on behav. intent for coupons 0.653 (8.6) H3a supported
Liking coupons on behav. intent to stockpile 0.276 (3.0) H4a supported
Liking cents-o! on behav. intent for cents-o! 0.633 (7.1) H3b supported
Liking cents-o! on behav. intent to stockpile N.S H4b not supported
Liking deals on behav. intent for coupons 0.261 (4.2) H3c supported
Liking deals on behav. intent for cents-o! 0.458 (6.2) H3c supported
Liking deals on behav. intent to stockpile 0.172 (2.0) H4c supported
Inyuence of traits variables
Market maven on information search 0.250 (4.1) H5a supported
Market maven on bene"ts evaluation N.S H5b not supported
Variety seeking on liking coupons N.S H6a not supported
Variety seeking on liking cents-o! 0.168 (2.1) H6b supported
Variety seeking on liking deals 0.242 (3.4) H6c supported
Busyness on behav. intent for coupons !0.220 (!3.7) H7a supported
Busyness on behav. intent for cents-o! N.S H7b not supported
Busyness on behav. intent to stockpile 0.230 (3.8) H7c not supported
Brand loyalty on behav. intent for coupons !0.173 (!2.4) H8a is supported
Brand loyalty on behav. intent for cents-o! !0.171 (!2.3) H8b is supported
Brand loyalty on behav. intent to stockpile N.S H8c not supported
Brand loyalty on liking coupons 0.135 (2.2) H8d not supported
Brand loyalty on liking cents-o! N.S H8e not supported
Brand loyalty on liking deals N.S H8f not supported
Financial wellness on behav. intent for coupons N.S H9a not supported
Financial wellness on behav. intent for cents-o! !0.191 (!3.2) H9b is supported
Financial wellness on behav. intent to stockpile !0.215 (!3.4) H9c is supported

predominance analysis was performed (Byrne, 1994). In standardized path coe$cients shows the weaker in#u-
all cases, models with the hypothesized paths had a better ence of information search (compared to bene"ts evalu-
"t (2 signi"cant) than the ones with reciprocal path. ation) on liking of Cents-o! and even a marginal one for
Moreover, the hypothesized paths were signi"cant deals in general. In fact, as presented by Kahn and
whereas reciprocal ones were not, which support the Schmittlein (1992), some promotions like `cents-o!a con-
hypothesized general model. sidered as in-store promotions may involve less informa-
Second, concerning the cognitive}a!ective}conative tion search.
expected pattern, the positive path between both cogni- Third, liking of deals in general and price promotions
tive constructs and the a!ective constructs for all price increase the intention to buy more when a coupon or
promotions as well as deals in general reveals that con- a cents-o! accompanies a product. Moreover, as con-
sumers look for both information in #iers and magazines sumers express positive feelings towards coupons and
seeking valuable coupons and make promotions bene"ts deals in general, the quantity of purchases made using
evaluations. It also indicates that the more people per- promotions increases (H3, H4 a and b). An interesting
ceive that they are doing this cognitive work, the more result concerns the absence of path between the liking of
they appreciate it and feel they are smart and making cents-o! and the intention to stockpiling (H4c). Follow-
good deals. This "nding is supported by the results of ing Helsen and Schmittlein "ndings (1992), all the paths
Schindler (1984) and Shimp and Kavas (1984). This cog- should have been signi"cant. The explanation that can be
nitive e!ort enhances liking of price promotions but also given concerns the perception of the cents-o! promotions
of deals in general (H1, H2). Moreover, a scrutiny at the by the consumers and speci"cally the absence of clear
258 M. Laroche et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 8 (2001) 251}260

date of expiration of the o!ers compared to the coupon The "nding relative to the positive e!ect of brand
o!ers where the date constitutes the main information. In loyalty on the liking of coupons is quite surprising since it
the case of cents-o! promotions, the main information contradicts prior results that dealt with that issue. In fact,
communicated seems to be focused on the rebate o!ered Mittal (1994) found that increased brand loyalty gener-
by the manufacturer (Blair and Landon, 1981). ated less favorable attitudes towards coupons and thus
Finally, the scrutiny of the results concerning the im- yielded less coupon redemption. Other studies also
pact of traits variables on the process components o!ers revealed the negative association between brand loyalty
some interesting insights. and coupon proneness (Webster, 1965; Montgomery,
The market maven variable only a!ects partly the 1971; Teel et al., 1980; Bawa and Shoemaker, 1987).
cognitive component of the expected pattern. In fact, However, this result has to be interpreted with caution
more precisely, it in#uences positively the information because in prior research, authors only tested the e!ect of
search dimension but not the evaluation one. In fact, the promotions on brand loyalty, and more speci"cally on
market maven variable implies a responsibility of know- coupon responsiveness. In our case, we are attempting to
ledge and awareness by the individual about promotions investigate the opposite e!ect, which is the e!ect of brand
and products and it allows him to build his liking for loyalty on attitude towards coupons. In other words, we
price promotions. The non-signi"cance of the in#uence are trying to verify whether brand loyal consumers per-
of this trait on the level of promotions bene"ts evaluation ceive coupons positively. So, for loyal consumers, it is
made by the consumer is interesting. It suggests in fact really possible to like coupons but not to act in redeem-
that market-maven consumers will search for more in- ing them, indeed because their loyalty is strong. This
formation about promotions than other consumers but it result is supported by the negative expected impact of
does not mean that they will evaluate more these promo- brand loyalty on behavioral intentions toward speci"c
tions. This result suggests that the two considered cogni- price promotions.
tive processes are not absolutely linked. The "nancial wellness variable did have a signi"cant
Globally, the variety-seeking variable in#uences posit- in#uence on behavior. In other words, consumers who
ively the a!ective component. It supports the concept of perceived themselves to be less wealthy were more in-
fun seeking (Lichtenstein et al., 1998) and reinforces the clined to use cents-o! while shopping than those who feel
pleasant aspect of promotion in itself (positive a!ect) and they are rich enough not to bother using cents-o!. This is
the opportunity to make the product switch that it o!ers. congruent with Mittal's results (1994) who found that
Nevertheless, the non-signi"cant path for coupon a!ect when the income is higher, it generated a more positive
tends to support the fact that the trait of searching for "nancial wellness self-perception that yielded less
variety in products has no impact on the liking of cou- promotional redemption. The non-signi"cant path for
pons contrarily to cents-o!. The nature itself of the cou- behavioral intentions toward coupons can be explained
pon and the importance of the information search impact again by the nature itself of coupons that do not appear
(0.544) in the elaboration of the a!ect may explain this as obvious money savers as cents-o! promotions and
result. also by the fact that a%uent consumers sometimes want
The busyness variable negatively a!ects the behavioral to be seen as smart and sophisticated shoppers and are
intention toward coupons in our model. People who re- not reluctant in purchasing products with coupons in
port being busy often have a reduced opportunity to incur order to save money. Therefore, the in#uence of "nancial
the "xed and the handling costs that it required redeeming wellness does not have a signi"cant in#uence on their
coupons. This "nding is congruent with Mittal's result redemption behavior.
(1994), which predicted that the busyness of households
increased brand loyalty. The absence of a signi"cant path
for behavioral intentions toward cents-o! supports the 6. Conclusion and implications
lack of in#uence of busyness on this type of promotions.
The explanation considered can be that these kind of A cognitive}a!ective}conative multidimensional
promotions do not need to be planned, require less per- model was conceptualized to integrate all aspects of how
sonal investment to redeem and therefore are not a!ected and why consumers use sales promotion. As the results
by the busyness variable. Moreover, busyness positively indicated, the expected pattern signi"cantly predicted the
impacts the intention to stockpile. In fact, the busier you resulting behavior towards deals and traits variables had
are, the more you maximize your purchase by stockpiling signi"cant e!ects on the process (busyness, variety seek-
promoted products when you have the opportunity to ing, "nancial wellness, brand loyalty, and market maven).
face promoted products. This result suggests that the From a managerial standpoint, the results of the pres-
busy consumer, who loves to save time, also loves saving ent study show that some lifestyle factors (i.e., perceived
money by the mean of deals. Even if he does not have "nancial wellness, variety seeking, brand loyalty and
time for redemption of coupons or search, he will buy busyness) were the factors that most strongly in#uenced
larger quantities when facing a promotion. consumers' deal interest. This "nding suggests the
M. Laroche et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 8 (2001) 251}260 259

importance for managers of consumer knowledge regard- Bawa, K., Shoemaker, R., 1989. Analyzing incremental sales from
ing these traits and appropriate segmentation. Moreover, a direct mail coupon promotion. Journal of Marketing 53,
the identi"cation of average and low-income households 66}78.
Bentler, P., Bonett, D., 1980. Signi"cance tests and goodness of
ought to be a prime research issue for an e$cient execu- "t in the analysis of covariance. Psychological Bulletin 88,
tion of sales promotion strategies. Managers have also to 588}606.
take into account that consumers enjoy using coupons Blair, E.A., Landon, E.L., 1981. The e!ects of reference prices in retail
and cents-o! because it makes them feel like smart shop- advertisements. Journal of Marketing 45 (2), 61}69.
pers. This segment of consumers is willing to look for Blattberg, R., Buesing, T., Peacock, P., Sen, S., 1978. Identifying the deal
prone segment. Journal of Marketing Research 15, 369}377.
information seeking more appealing o!ers. These active Blattberg, R.C., Eppen, G.D., Lieberman, J., 1981. A theoretical and
shoppers use many coupons and cents-o!, are willing to empirical evaluation of price deals for consumer nondurables. Jour-
shop in several di!erent stores, and to consider several nal of Marketing 45, 116}129.
di!erent brands for each product category. One way to Blattberg, R.C., Neslin, S.A., 1990. Sales Promotion: Concepts,
reach these shoppers could be to o!er them multiple Methods and Strategies. Prentice-Hall, Engelwood Cli!s, NJ.
Bollen, K., 1989. Structural Equations with Latent Variables. Wiley,
brand coupons (coupons redeemable only if two brands New York.
or more are purchased) or to build their `coupon loyaltya Browne, M., 1989. Single sample cross validation indices for covariance
by identifying them using panel data, and sending them structures. Multivariate Behavioral Research 24, 445}455.
regularly a package of coupons that will reinforce their Byrne, B., 1994. Structural Equation Modeling with EQS and
out-of-store decision. EQS/Windows. Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA.
Carmines, E., Mc Iver, J., 1981. Analysing models with unobserved
From the research standpoint, several questions are variables: analysis of covariance structures. Social Measurement:
raised and need to be further explored. The "rst fruitful Current Issues. Sage, Newbury Park, CA, pp. 65}115.
avenue for research pertains to the measures used in the Cheong, K. J., 1993. Observations: are cents-o! coupons e!ective?
current study. We view further research that combines Journal of Advertising Research 33 (2), 73}77.
both survey-based measures with scanner panel data that Dickson, P.R., 1983. Pricing theory, research and practice, an examina-
tion of the weber}fechner law for gasoline prices. Unpublished
would be more accurate in measuring the real response Working Paper, The Ohio State University.
to deals. Donnelley Marketing, 1994. 16th Annual Survey of Promotional Prac-
Moreover, this research does not directly examine the tices. Donnelley Marketing, Stamford, CT.
e!ects of various types of promotions on observable Folkes, V., Wheat, R.D., 1995. Consumer's price perceptions of pro-
consumer responses identi"ed in the sales promotion moted products. Journal of Retailing 71 (3), 317}328.
Hahn, M., Chang, D.R., Kim, I.T., Kim, Y., 1995. Consumer response
literature, such as brand choice, purchase intent, repeat to coupon advertising. International Journal of Advertising 14,
purchase, impulse buying and inter-purchase time. It 41}53.
would be crucial to comprehend the responses generated Helsen, K., Schmittlein, D.C., 1992. Some characterizations of stockpil-
by each of the speci"c deals to opt for more e$cient ing behavior under uncertainty. Marketing Letters 3 (1), 5}16.
promotional strategies and reach more accurate targets. Henderson, C.M., 1987. The interaction of coupons with price and store
promotions. Advances in Consumer Research 15, 364}371.
Finally, the present study does not consider the Kahn, B.E., Schmittlein, D.C., 1992. The relationship between pur-
di!erences in response across product categories. Further chases made on promotion and shopping trip behavior. Journal of
research is needed to explore the e!ect of product in- Retailing 68 (3), 294}315.
volvement on responsiveness to deals. Kalwani, M.U., Yim, C.K., 1992. Consumer price and promotion
expectations: an experimental study. Journal of Marketing Research
29 (1), 90}101.
Kotler, P., 1988. Marketing Management. Prentice-Hall, Englewood
Acknowledgements Cli!s, NJ.
Lichtenstein, D.R., Netemeyer, R.G., Burton, S., 1990. Distinguishing
coupon proneness from value consciousness: an acquisition}transac-
The authors gratefully acknowledge the "nancial sup-
tion theory perspective. Journal of Marketing 54, 54}67.
port of the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Lichtenstein, D.R., Netemeyer, R.G., Burton, S., 1995. Assessing the
Council of Canada. domain speci"city of deal proneness: a "eld study. Journal of Con-
sumer Research 22, 314}326.
Lichtenstein, D.R., Netemeyer, R.G., Burton, S., 1997. An examination
of deal proneness across sales promotion types: a consumer segmen-
References tation perspective. Journal of Retailing 73 (2), 283}297.
Lichtenstein, D.R, Burton, S., Netemeyer, R.G., 1998. A scale for
Anderson, J.C., Gerbing, D.W., 1988. Structural equation modeling in measuring attitude toward private label products and an examina-
practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychologi- tion of its psychological and behavioral correlates. Academy of
cal Bulletin 103, 411}423. Marketing Science 26, 293}306.
Arnold, S.J., Oum, T.E., Tigert, D.J., 1983. Determinant attributes in Mittal, B., 1994. An integrated framework for relating diverse consumer
retail patronage. Seasonal, temporal, regional and international com- characteristics to supermarket coupon redemption. Journal of Mar-
parisons. Journal of Marketing Research 20 (2), 149}157. keting Research 31, 533}544.
Bawa, K., Shoemaker, R.W., 1987. The e!ects of a direct mail coupon Montgomery, D.B., 1971. Consumer characteristics associated with
on brand choice behavior. Journal of Marketing Research 24, dealing: an empirical study. Journal of Marketing Research 8,
370}376. 118}120.
260 M. Laroche et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 8 (2001) 251}260

Narasimhan, C., 1984. A price discrimination theory of coupons. Mar- Shimp, T.A., Kavas, A., 1984. The theory of reasoned action applied to
keting Science 3 (2), 128}147. coupon usage. Journal of Consumer Research 11, 795}809.
Neslin, S.A., Quelch, J., Henderson, C., 1984. Consumer promotions Shimp, T.A., 1993. Promotion Management and Marketing Commun-
and the acceleration of product purchases. In: Jocz, K.E. (Ed.), ications, 3rd Edition. Dryden Press, Fort Worth.
Research on Sales Promotion: Collected Papers. Marketing Science Shoemaker, R., 1979. An analysis of consumer reactions to product
Institute, Cambridge, MA, 1984. promotions. In: Educators' Conference Proceedings. American Mar-
Price, L.L., Feick, L.F., Guskey-Federouch, A., 1988. Couponing be- keting Association, Chicago, pp. 244}248.
haviors of the market maven: pro"le of a super couponer. Advances Strang, R.A., 1983. Sales promotion research: contributions and issues.
in Consumer Research 15, 354}359. Unpublished paper presented at the AMA/MSI/PMAA Sales Pro-
Raju, P.S., Hastak, M., 1983. Pre-trial cognitive e!ects of cents-o! motion Workshop, Babson College, May 1983.
coupons. Journal of Advertising 12 (2), 24}32. Tanaka, J., 1993. Multifaceted conceptions of "t in structural equation
Rosenberg, M.J., 1956. Cognitive structure and attitudinal a!ect. Jour- models, Testing Structural Equation Models. Sage Publications,
nal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 53, 367}372. Newbury Park.
Sawyer, A.G., Dickson, P.H., 1984. Psychological perspectives on con- Teel, J.E., Williams, R.H., Bearden, W.O., 1980. Correlates of consumer
sumer response to sales promotion. In: Jocz, K.E. (Ed.), Research on susceptibility to coupons in new grocery product introductions. Jour-
Sales Promotion: Collected Papers. Marketing Science Institute, nal of Advertising 9 (3), 31}46.
Cambridge MA, 1984. Thaler, R., 1985. Mental accounting and consumer choice. Marketing
Schindler, R., 1984. How cents-o! coupons motivate the consumer. In: Science 4, 199}214.
Jocz, K.E. (Ed.), Research on Sales Promotion: Collected Papers. Urbany, J.E., Kalapurakal, R., Dickson, P., 1996. Price search in the
Marketing Science Institute, Cambridge, MA, pp. 47}62. retail grocery market. Journal of Marketing 60 (2), 91}105.
Schneider, L.G., Currim, I.S., 1991. Consumer purchase behaviors asso- Wheaton, B., Muthen, B., Alwin, D., Summers, G., 1977. Assessing
ciated with active and passive deal proneness. International Journal reliability and stability in panel models. Sociological Methodology.
of Research in Marketing 8, 205}222. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp. 84}136.
Schultz, D.E., Robinson, W.A., 1982. Sales Promotion Management. Webster, F., 1965. The deal prone consumer. Journal of Marketing
Crain Books, Chicago. Research 2, 186}189.

You might also like