You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 543 – 551

The Promotion Affect Scale: Defining the Affective


Dimensions of Promotion
Heather Honeaa,*, Darren W. Dahlb
a
College of Business Administration, San Diego State University, 5500 Campanile Drive, San Diego, CA 92182, USA
b
Faculty of Commerce and Business Administration, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada V6T 1Z2

Abstract

This article examines the psychometric properties of consumer affect that is produced by a sales promotion offer, e.g., price discount,
coupon redemption, free gift, etc. Building from the existing sales promotion and emotion literature, we propose a new 28-item scale
instrument, the Promotion Affect Scale (PAS), which measures affective response to promotion. Five phases of data collection are reported,
which systematically refine the instrument and assess the resulting dimensionality and discriminating ability of the proposed scale. Results
indicate that the 28-item PAS is a parsimonious and effective measurement tool of the affect produced by a sales promotion. This research
contributes towards a better theoretical understanding of the consumer affect resulting from promotional purchase and provides a readily
usable measurement instrument for theoretical researchers and marketing practitioners.
D 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Promotion Affect Scale; Promotion; Consumer

1. Introduction consumer contexts. For example, affective response to


consumer promotion activities—‘‘action-focused marketing
Over the last two decades, affect has taken hold as a key events whose purpose is to have a direct impact on the
explanatory construct in the field of consumer behavior. A behavior of the firm’s customers’’ (Blattberg and Neslin,
growing stream of research shows that affective reactions 1990, p.3)—may differ from that of advertising.
are generated in response to the purchase environment (e.g., The purpose of this research is to broaden the investiga-
Yoo et al., 1998), consumption of products and services tion of affect in consumer behavior by exploring the
(e.g., Oliver, 1994), and marketing mix variables such as affective reactions to consumer promotion activities. We
advertisements (e.g., Aaker et al., 1988). These affective study the affect generated by promotions and psychometri-
responses have been shown to influence attitudes, evalua- cally define its dimensions. Five phases of data collection
tions, and behavior (e.g., Burke and Edell, 1989). As the are reported, which systematically develop the proposed
importance and influence of affect become more recognized Promotion Affect Scale (PAS). Results indicate that the 28-
in consumer research, the next challenge is to develop more item PAS is an effective tool to measure the affect produced
appropriate and context-relevant measurement instruments by consumer promotion activities and can parsimoniously
to capture the specific affective reactions elicited by con- capture diverse affective reactions. The affect descriptors
sumption activities and marketing stimuli. used in the scale are shown to differentiate key aspects of
Efforts to develop affect measures specific to the domain the promotional purchase environment. The article con-
of consumer research have resulted in instruments that cludes with a discussion of how PAS can be used to support
measure affective responses to advertising (e.g., Edell and promotion research and help managers develop effective
Burke, 1987) and consumption activities (Richins, 1997). promotion campaigns.
These instruments are effective in measuring affect in their
specific domain of interest. However, these instruments may
not provide the best measure of affective response in all 2. Consumer promotions

* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-619-594-4308. Consumer promotions (e.g., coupons, sweepstakes) now
E-mail address: hhonea@mail.sdsu.edu (H. Honea). account for almost a quarter of the marketing budgets of

0148-2963/$ – see front matter D 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0148-2963(03)00145-0
544 H. Honea, D.W. Dahl / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 543–551

consumer product companies, and expenditures have been outward-focused dimension that included negative affective
increasing at almost twice the rate of advertising (Breen, responses. Edell and Burke (1987) identify upbeat, warm,
2000). Past research in this area examines the economic and negative feelings as the general categories of affective
aspects of a promotion where an immediate economic response to advertisements. They find that these feeling-
incentive to purchase a product is provided (for a review, based responses are important predictors of advertising
see Blattberg and Neslin, 1990). Research also focuses on effectiveness and consumer beliefs.
the informational aspects of consumer promotions (e.g., Richins (1997) moves away from advertising-based
Raghubir and Corfman, 1999), and although there is sug- measurements to examine consumption-related affect. She
gestive evidence that affective reactions are also a relevant finds that the Consumption Emotion Set (CES) is a more
part of consumer response to promotion, there is very little appropriate instrument to measure consumption-related af-
research in this vein. fect than DES, Plutchik, and PAD because these instruments
Many researchers have hinted at a feelings-based route do not represent the diversity of emotions elicited by
through which promotions may impact consumers. Scott consumption situations. Even though Holbrook and Batra
(1976) was the first to suggest that the effectiveness of a (1987) and Edell and Burke (1987) provide more extensive
deal might be explainable by the affect generated by that emotion lists, they fail to include some critical consumption
promotional offer. Regret (Inman and McAlister, 1994) and emotions. However, Richins (1997) acknowledges that the
hedonic enjoyment of a deal (see Chandon et al., 2000) have Edell and Burke (1987) instrument is able to provide a
also appeared in promotion research. Promotions are shown reasonable measure of consumption-related affect.
to reduce feelings of guilt associated with consumption of In the context of consumer promotions, we define the
certain products (Strahilevitz and Myers, 1998), and the affect towards a promotion as how consumers feel about
outcome of promotional purchases is predicted to result in buying (or not buying) something on deal in terms of (1) the
specific pride-related responses such feeling like a ‘‘smart overall encounter, (2) the product, (3) the seller, (4) the
shopper’’ (Schindler, 1989). Despite indications of affect- components of the deal (e.g., price or purchase require-
based reactions to promotions, there has not been a system- ments), or (5) themselves. We expect that measurement in
atic delineation of the dimensions of affect as they pertain to this context will capture three main categories of affective
promotional response. response. Affective responses directed towards the self,
other parties (e.g., the product, the seller), and the situation
in general. In the following sections, a series of studies are
3. Affect measurement in consumer behavior reported that examine affective reactions elicited by oppor-
tunities and missed opportunities to take advantage of
Affect is defined as a ‘‘valenced feeling state’’ (Cohen different types of promotions.
and Areni, 1991) and consumer behavior research generally
relies on typologies from psychology, such as Izard’s (1977)
Differential Emotion Scale (DES), Plutchik’s (1980) work 4. Study 1
on ‘‘primary’’ or ‘‘basic’’ emotions, and Mehrabian and
Russell’s (1974) Pleasure-Arousal-Dominance (PAD). How- An initial study, using two exploratory research ap-
ever, DES and Plutchik’s (1980) work do not provide an proaches, was conducted to identify a range of affective
easy mechanism to identify everyday emotions such as responses consumers might experience in response to pro-
pride, and PAD’s 18 semantic differential scales make it motional offerings.
difficult to draw inferences about the existence of specific
affect states (Richins, 1997). 4.1. Methods
Instruments developed for the specific purpose of mea-
suring affective responses to marketing stimuli largely 4.1.1. Literature review
address advertising-induced emotions. Aaker et al. (1988) Following the methodology used in the previous scale
identify an extensive range of emotions elicited by adver- development (e.g., Richins, 1997; Zaichkowsky, 1985), we
tisements; however, this 180-word battery of descriptors is first identified affect descriptors through a review of the
too long to be employed as a reasonable measurement affect literature, standard inventory measures of affect, and
instrument. Batra and Ray (1986) identify a range of prior promotion research. Two trained research assistants
affective responses to advertising that can be categorized evaluated the affect descriptors and determined how repre-
as follows: (1) surgency, elation, and vigor/activation sentative the adjectives were of actual affective responses
(SEVA); (2) deactivation; and (3) social affection. SEVA elicited by a promotional context. Items were judged in
is interesting in that it identifies outward-focused affect but terms of either being ‘‘not at all representative’’ or ‘‘some-
it is limited in that it measures only positive affect what/likely representative’’ of an affective reaction to a
responses. Holbrook and Batra (1987) on the other hand promotional offer. Agreement between the two research
identify pleasure, arousal, and dominance dimensions that assistants was 97%. Disagreements were resolved through
are consistent with PAD. Dominance represented an inward/ discussion with one of the authors.
H. Honea, D.W. Dahl / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 543–551 545

4.1.2. Focus groups 5.1. Methods


To complement the literature review, six focus group
sessions were used to identify potential descriptors. Each Using popular marketing textbooks (e.g., Kotler, 2000)
session contained a mix of male and female participants and Blattberg and Neslin’s (1990) review of sales promo-
drawn from graduate/undergraduate classes. Drawing from tions, we identified seven different categories of promotion:
Smith and Ellsworth’s (1985) key appraisal dimensions for sale (e.g., percent/price off), price reduction certificate (e.g.,
emotion, participants were first prompted to recall promo- coupon/rebate), value pack (e.g., bundled/bonus pack),
tion experiences that varied in the effort they expended to contests (e.g., sweepstakes/games), continuity program
take advantage of the offer, the certainty of receiving the (e.g., frequent buyer/loyalty rewards), free offer (e.g., sam-
promotion, the amount of personal responsibility in obtain- ple/trial), and premium offer (e.g., free gift/product with
ing the promotion, and whether or not the outcome was purchase). Drawing exemplars from each of the identified
viewed as positive or negative. Participants were then asked categories, survey forms presented each type of promotion.
to assess a specific promotional situation and relate their A sample of 104 undergraduate students were assigned to
affective response. Next, we prompted participants to imag- one of the survey scenarios and received instruction that
ine promotion situations that varied by product type, in- they were to imagine/recall situations in which they had
volvement, and promotion type and convey the emotional received the type of promotion outlined. To capture nega-
responses these situations would elicit. Throughout the tively valenced affect, a second sample of 105 undergrad-
session, the focus group moderator utilized laddering tech- uate students was assigned to one of the seven scenarios
niques and chain analysis to drill down to the affective and asked to imagine/recall situations in which they had
responses generated. A trained research assistant examined ‘‘missed out’’ on the promotion. Across both samples,
the focus group transcripts and recorded the specific affect subjects rated their affective response to the specific pro-
descriptors used by participants across each of the sessions. motion scenario using the 124 adjective descriptors and a
four-point scale (never/often). Only one subject in the
4.2. Results second sample indicated an inability to recall/imagine and
was thus eliminated from the analysis. Finally, subjects were
4.2.1. Literature review asked to indicate their sex, age, and language spoken—
Of the 195 initial affect descriptors identified in the responses to these questions had no effect on the results
literature review, 94 items were judged to be representative reported below.
of affective reactions experienced in a promotional context.
One hundred and one items were judged to be nonrepresen- 5.2. Results
tative and were thus eliminated from the listing. Examples of
eliminated items include traditional measures of arousal (e.g., Frequency counts of scale item response were used to
sleepy, sluggish, jittery) and other more extreme emotions identify items for elimination. Collapsing across the seven
that are unlikely to be experienced in response to promotion promotion scenarios, if a descriptor item did not achieve a
activity (e.g., afraid, loving, sorrowful). rating of 3 or 4 on the four-point scale, by more than 50% of
the subjects, in at least one of the two samples, it was
4.2.2. Focus groups eliminated. We utilized this cutoff approach to identify
Analysis of the focus group transcripts revealed 83 distinct descriptor adjectives that had wide use and applicability
affect adjectives that were used to describe feelings experi- across a variety of promotion contexts. This approach
enced during a promotion. Importantly, participants identi- resulted in 64 adjective descriptors being eliminated from
fied both feelings of positive affect from taking advantage of the scale battery.
an offer (e.g., happy, proud) and also feelings of negative Following Richins (1997) (see also Shaver et al., 1987),
affect when they missed out on an offer or an offer did not the remaining 60 items were grouped into subcategories and
meet their expectations (e.g., unhappy, annoyed). their synonymy was assessed. When descriptors were close
The two separate listings were combined to form an in meaning and one of the descriptors had received a lower
overall battery of descriptor adjectives pertinent to affect frequency count across both samples, the adjective with the
generated by promotional experiences. After eliminating lower frequency count was eliminated. In total, these two
matching terms across the two listings, the reduced list reduction techniques resulted in 81 affective descriptors
contained 124 prospective items. being eliminated, leaving a battery of 43 scale items for
further analysis.

5. Study 2
6. Study 3
Study 2 attempted to reduce the set of 124 adjective
descriptors by eliminating items that are unfamiliar, shared The third study utilized multidimensional scaling (MDS)
similar meanings, and rarely used. to better understand the relationship between the affect
546 H. Honea, D.W. Dahl / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 543–551

measures in a promotion context and search for cooccur- versus nonmonetary promotions, hedonic versus utilitarian
rence in the identified affective reactions. To ensure we products, and expensive versus inexpensive products, to
examined affective reactions relevant to both positive and determine which clusters of adjectives held together re-
negative promotion experiences, we provided subjects with gardless of how the space was partitioned. This was useful
specific promotional scenarios. in eliminating redundant terms as well as identifying terms
that would make appropriate scale item sets. The received
6.1. Methods opportunity group (individuals that imagined that they
received the promotion) and missed opportunity group
To ensure that the battery of items retained were (individuals that imagined that they did not receive the
appropriate across a range of promotion situations, the promotion) were analyzed separately to ensure interpret-
scenarios varied by promotion type, product type, product ability of the emotion space. Euclidean distance measures
price, and outcome. The same promotion types were used were calculated for each subsample and were used in the
as in Study 1; however, immediate (coupon) and future MDS analysis.
(rebate) price reduction certificates were separated into For the received opportunity group, stress coefficients
different scenarios [in Study 2, when subjects imagined/ and interpretability criteria suggested a two-dimensional
recalled a rebate (vs. coupon), they commented on the fact solution to the promotion emotion space regardless of
that taking advantage of a rebate placed additional require- whether the group was divided into subsamples based on
ments on them. Based on this subtle differentiation, it promotion category, product type, or product price. Stress
seemed appropriate to present these promotions separately coefficients for the one-, two-, and three-dimensional
in Study 3]. This resulted in eight different promotions, solutions ranged between .18– .21, .11 – .13, and .08 – .10,
half of which are considered monetary and the other half respectively, with solutions accounting for 88.1– 91.4%,
nonmonetary (Lichtenstein et al., 1997). In each promotion 94.0 – 95.3%, and 96.0 – 97.0% of the variance (see Table
category (monetary vs. nonmonetary), one promotion was 1). Across subsamples, the first dimension represented the
combined with one of the four product type/price options. positivity – negativity of affect generally found in emotion
These options were generated from a pretest identifying (1) research. The second dimension appeared to differentiate
hedonic expensive, (2) hedonic inexpensive, (3) utilitarian the ‘‘focus’’ of the emotional responses. More outward-
expensive, and (4) utilitarian inexpensive products. To focused emotion descriptors, such as hesitant, deceived,
provide additional variance in the promotional situations, fortunate, and thankful, tended to fall on one pole. The
we presented participants with scenarios where they were other pole was anchored by inward-focused emotion
able and were not able to take advantage of the promotion descriptors, such as proud, discouraged, victorious, and
situation described. In total, this design resulted in 16 disappointed.
different promotional scenarios. The stress coefficients for the missed opportunity group
Three hundred and eighty-nine adult consumers were also suggest that a two-dimensional solution provides the
randomly assigned to one of the scenario conditions, asked best interpretation. The stress coefficients for the one-, two-,
to read through the scenario, and imagine experiencing the and three-dimensional solutions ranged between .07 – .08,
event. They then indicated their affective reactions towards .05 – .06, and .04– .05, respectively, with solutions account-
the situation using the 43 affect adjectives. Subjects indi- ing for 98.4– 99.6% of the variance (see Table 1). The
cated the degree to which they experienced each emotion by minimal changes in explained variance as dimensionality
responding to a four-point scale (not at all/very strongly). To increased are probably attributable to the fact that the
control for possible order effects, the scale items were scenario represented a ‘‘missed’’ outcome, and as a result
reverse ordered for half of the sample subjects. Subjects there was little differentiation in participants’ positive af-
were asked to indicate their sex, age, and area of resi- fective responses. Examining the derived emotion space
dence—responses to these questions had no effect on the confirms that in the negative domain, affective responses
results reported below. are clustering in a similar manner to negative affective
responses in the received group. The more outward-focused
6.2. Results descriptors (such as deceived, mislead, and skeptical) cluster
in one area, while frustrated, disappointed, and regret tended
6.2.1. Dimensions of affective response to promotion to cluster in another area.
Following Richins (1997), we elected to utilize MDS to
assess redundancy across our scale items. Given our 6.2.2. Adjective clusters
objective to better define the range of specific affective As expected, the valence of emotion dimension emerged
reactions to promotion, it was deemed that the data in the emotion spaces. The other dimension that displayed
reduction approach inherent in a factor analysis approach some similarities to dimensions found in other emotion
would not be appropriate for our purposes (Richins, 1997). spaces, such as the dominance dimension in PAD, the
Additionally, the design of the survey instrument allowed intensity dimension of Shaver et al. (1987), and Richin’s
us to examine the promotion emotion space for monetary (1997) receptivity/activity dimension, really appears to
H. Honea, D.W. Dahl / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 543–551 547

Table 1
Stress coefficients and variance explained for dimensional solution of emotion space
One dimensional Two dimensional Three dimensional
Stress Percent of Stress Percent of Stress Percent of
coefficient variance coefficient variance coefficient variance
explained explained explained
Received promotion opportunity subsample
Promotion category
Monetary .18 90.8 .13 94.0 .08 96.9
Nonmonetary .21 88.1 .13 94.2 .09 96.1
Product type
Hedonic .19 89.6 .13 94.0 .09 97.0
Utilitarian .18 91.4 .12 94.9 .09 96.7
Product price
Expensive .18 90.6 .11 95.3 .08 96.8
Inexpensive .19 90.4 .13 94.5 .10 96.0

Missed promotion opportunity subsample


Promotion category
Monetary .08 98.8 .05 99.4 .04 99.6
Nonmonetary .08 98.6 .05 99.3 .04 99.6
Product type
Hedonic .07 98.9 .06 99.2 .04 99.6
Utilitarian .09 98.4 .05 99.3 .05 99.4
Product price
Expensive .08 98.5 .05 99.3 .04 99.6
Inexpensive .07 98.9 .05 99.4 .04 99.6

differentiate responses based on focus. This interpretation of an outcome externally, emotions such as anger and gratitude
the space appears quite reasonable, given the agency of will result (Schindler, 1989).
focus has been shown to be an important differentiator for Using the item locations defined by the dimensional
the experience of different types of emotions (e.g., Smith spaces in the subsample, redundant terms were eliminated
and Lazarus, 1993). Furthermore, previous research in the from certain clusters. Items retained were judged to have
domain of promotions suggests that when ascriptions re- clear meanings and to have higher likelihood of usage and
garding a deal are focused internally, emotions such as pride occurrence (as determined by Study 2). Further, two to three
or shame will likely result and when an individual attributes descriptor items were retained in each cluster to ensure less

Fig. 1. Derived stimulus configuration for Study 4 emotion descriptors.


548 H. Honea, D.W. Dahl / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 543–551

measurement error and that the diversity represented in a Table 2


The Promotion Affect Scalea
cluster was maintained. We also eliminated certain activa-
tion items that did not cluster on the outward/inward focus
dimension and appeared alongside other unrelated adjective
clusters. This reduction resulted in the elimination of 19
items and a listing of 24 adjective descriptors in the scale
battery.

7. Study 4

While Study 3 was successful in refining the list of


adjectives, the context of Study 3 involved the ‘‘anticipa-
tion’’ of receiving or not receiving a promotional offer. A
fourth study was conducted to ensure that the reduced list of
adjective descriptors provided an appropriate listing of
affective responses occurring as a result of an actual
purchase.

7.1. Methods

One hundred and sixteen undergraduate students partic-


ipated in the study. They were asked to recall a recent
promotion experience and to indicate to what extent the
identified promotion experience made them feel each of the
24 descriptors using a five-point scale (not at all/very
strongly). To control for possible order effects, the scale
items were reverse ordered for half of the sample subjects.
Subjects also responded to an open-ended question that a
Values in parentheses represent the Pearson correlation for two-item
asked them the specific details of the promotion experience scales and Cronbach’s alpha for scales with more than two items. Reported
they recalled. Subjects were asked to indicate their sex, age, statistics are based on Study 5 data.
and language spoken—responses to these questions had no
effect on the results reported below. type of emotion research (Richins, 1997). Additionally, as
indicated by Morgan and Heise (1988), an examination of
7.2. Results the dimensional structure of emotions in several studies
shows a large gap in the structure when physical states are
The stress coefficients (.07, .05, and .04 for one-, two-, eliminated, even though it has been argued that these may
and three-dimensions explaining 98.8%, 99.1%, and 99.3% not be appropriate affect measures (Ortony and Clore,
of the variance) suggested that interpretability of the space 1981). Therefore, it is expected that a gap might emerge
could be best achieved with two dimensions (Fig. 1 shows a in the negative affect side of the space. To verify that we
plot of the MDS weights for a two-dimension solution). An had not eliminated any critical inward-focused negative
assessment of the promotion experiences recalled by sub- affect responses, we examined the open-ended responses
jects indicated that they had focused almost exclusively on for negative affect adjectives that had also appeared in the
memories involving positive promotion experiences. This initial battery lists in Studies 1 and 2. Four omitted descrip-
would partially explain the minimal changes in explained tors relevant to our purposes appeared in these responses—
variance as dimensionality increased and also the lack of feelings of being embarrassed, ashamed, or cheap, and
dispersion in the negative affective descriptors (see Fig. 1). feelings of guilt regarding the promotion experience. These
However, the adjective list seems to do a reasonable job of descriptors were (re)added to the scale battery. The final
covering the stimulus space. listing of the 28-item scale is found in Table 2.
Similar to Study 3, there appeared to be certain catego-
ries of affective response differentiated by their inward/
outward focus. Affect descriptors clustered in the outward- 8. Study 5
focused categories of appreciative, fortunate, annoyance,
and distrust. The inward-focused categories included self- A final study was conducted to test the proposed PAS
confident and regret. The more sparse areas that define the instrument by comparing its effectiveness with other scale
U-shaped pattern in our analysis commonly emerge in this batteries currently used in consumer behavior research.
H. Honea, D.W. Dahl / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 543–551 549

These instruments were the CES—47 items (Richins, 1997) evaluated using the subsample sets that either included the
and the Edell and Burke Feeling Scale (EBFS)—56 items EBFS or the CES measures. Separate discriminant analyses
(Edell and Burke, 1987). were performed for each scale instrument. CES and EBFS
instruments produced significant functions for each of the
8.1. Methods different grouping variables, with the exception of the
responsibility discriminant grouping (EBFS did not yield a
Six hundred and seventy-nine adult consumers re- significant function for this variable). Impressively, even
sponded to an open-ended questionnaire that asked them though PAS involves a much smaller battery of items, it
to recount a situation when they were either able to take produced significant functions for all grouping variables
advantage/or missed taking advantage of a promotion. They except the responsibility grouping in the EBFS subsample.
then indicated their affective reactions towards the reported Results are shown in Table 3.
situation by responding to a list of adjectives. Subjects In both the CES and EBFS subsamples separating
indicated the degree to which they experienced each emo- individuals based on their assessment of the deal, the
tion by using a five-point scale (not at all/very strongly). To PAS discriminant function had high positive loadings for
control for order effects, half of the questionnaires contained items in the happy, enthused, and self-confident measures
the battery lists for PAS and the CES instrument while the and reasonably high negative loadings for the unhappy,
other half contained the battery lists for PAS and the EBFS annoyed, and self-doubt measures. Both CES and EBFS
instrument. Within those sets, PAS appeared first, half of the had high loadings for similar items such as joy, excite-
time, and the other instrument appeared first, half of the ment, pride, and sad. In both analyses using PAS measures
time. In addition, each battery list was reverse alphabetically to separate individuals based on their perceptions of the
ordered half of the time. firm’s generosity, functions had high positive loadings for
After responding to the adjective lists, subjects were items in the happy, enthused, self-confident, appreciation,
asked about their (1) assessment of the promotion, (2) and favored measures and high negative loadings for items
responsibility in receiving the promotion, and (3) perception in the annoyed, self-doubt, and unhappy measures. The
of the firm’s generosity in providing the promotion. These function for CES showed parallel results with high load-
variables were used to assess whether PAS could differen- ings for items such as joy, excitement, surprise, afraid, sad,
tiate between different promotion-related outcomes as well and discontent. The EBFS discriminant function showed
as other affect scales. Finally, subjects were asked to reasonable size loadings for feeling pleased and content. In
indicate their sex, age, and area of residence—responses the CES subsample, the PAS measures separating individ-
to these questions had no effect on the results reported uals based on their perceptions of responsibility for re-
below and therefore are not discussed further. ceiving the deal resulted in functions that had relatively
high positive loadings for items in the self-doubt and
8.2. Results unhappy measures. The function for CES showed parallel
results with high loadings for items such as humiliated and
8.2.1. The affect solution space miserable.
Consistent with Studies 3 and 4, the stress coefficients Finally, when the two PAS subsamples were combined,
suggest that a two-dimensional solution is sufficient to PAS produced significant discriminate functions for all
describe the space (stress values for the one-, two-, and grouping variables. Table 4 provides the canonical corre-
three-dimensional solution were .08, .05, and .04; explain- lations and classification accuracies for each of the derived
ing 98.5%, 99.1%, and 99.4% of the variance). discriminate functions. In addition to the measured ap-
praisal variables, subjects’ responses to the type of pro-
8.2.2. Discriminant ability of PAS motion and their ability to take advantage of the offer were
Using a median split, we broke individuals up by used to examine the ability of PAS to discriminate between
whether they had a good/bad assessment of the promotion, promotion types and outcomes. PAS was shown to effec-
perceived their responsibility in obtaining the promotion to tively discriminate between monetary (e.g., coupon, sale)
be high/low, and whether they perceived the firm providing and nonmonetary promotions (e.g., free trial, frequent
the promotion to be generous/not generous. PAS was buyer), as well as promotional outcomes. Significant

Table 3
Comparison of discriminant abilities of affect measures
EBFS PAS CES PAS
Canonical R2 P value Canonical R2 P value Canonical R2 P value Canonical R2 P value
Assessment of the deal .59 .001 .44 .001 .57 .001 .55 .001
Responsibility in obtaining the deal .44 >.10 .33 >.10 .43 .10 .43 .001
Perception of the firm’s generosity .58 .001 .47 .001 .52 .001 .49 .001
550 H. Honea, D.W. Dahl / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 543–551

Table 4 The PAS offers different categories of affect that may be


Canonical correlation and classification accuracy for the PAS scale
useful when researchers attempt to measure specific types of
Canonical R2 Classification reactions. For instance, research focusing on reactions to the
accuracy (%)
firm could incorporate more outward-focused measures,
Assessment of the deal .48 70 whereas research investigating personal satisfaction or im-
Responsibility in obtaining the deal .28 61
pulsive behavior might incorporate more inward-focused
Perception of the firm’s generosity .47 68
Get or miss promotional offer .53 78 measures from the scale. Further, the fact that the PAS
Type of promotion .27 58 differentiated between various sales promotion types and
(monetary, nonmonetary) individuals’ perceptions regarding the firm suggests that the
Total Sample N = 679, all P < .05. PAS can be used to understand the potential effectiveness of
different types of promotions and help identify instances
when a firm will or will not receive some positive benefit
canonical correlations for discriminant functions (promo- from offering a promotion to consumers.
tion type: Canonical R2=.27, P < .05; outcome of offer:
Canonical R2=.53, P < .05) showed primarily positive load- 9.1. Limitations and future research
ings on the more outward-focused positive affective
descriptors. At the subsample level, none of the scale instru- Although the methodology used in the construction of
ments (i.e., the EBFS, CES, and PAS) were able to produce the PAS was patterned after recent scale papers in the
a significant discriminant function for these grouping consumer behavior field, one limitation to the methodology
variables. of these papers is that the instruments are commonly
developed ‘‘after’’ an actual store or purchase experience.
Clearly, a real time validation of the PAS instrument using
9. General discussion the consumers’ immediate affective responses would be an
interesting next step. It would also be fruitful to test for
The purpose of this research was to develop a context- relationships between the scale items and important con-
relevant measurement instrument to assess consumers’ af- sumer attitudinal and behavioral responses. Future research
fective reactions to sales promotion activity. The PAS should test the relation shared between the PAS and theo-
indicates that promotion-related affect is a multidimensional retically related constructs such as ‘‘deal proneness’’ (Lich-
construct with both positive and negative dimensions rang- tenstein et al., 1997) and ‘‘hedonic promotion benefits’’
ing in their degree of specificity. Thus, there are general (Chandon et al., 2000).
positive and negative valenced affective responses such as Examining the interaction of promotion type and product
feeling pleased or unhappy due to a promotional purchase. price, product sales price, and product type provides other
There are also more specific valenced feelings that can be opportunities for future research. These variables will likely
differentiated by agency of focus. The more outward-fo- moderate the intensity of different affective responses.
cused positive affect items describe feelings of appreciation Perhaps, certain promotions and environmental aspects tend
and feeling favored/optimistic. The more outward-focused to elicit certain categories of affect, such as appreciation or
negative affect items describe feelings of being annoyed and distrust, while others tend to produce feelings of self-
distrustful/skeptical. The inward-focused positive affect confidence or self-doubt. Identifying the manner in which
items describe feeling self-confident and negative items these categories of affect are triggered will provide insights
describe feeling self-conscious and self-doubt. into the final impact of promotional offers and contexts on
As well as identifying the different dimensions of pro- purchase intentions, attitudes, behaviors, and satisfaction.
motion-related affect, the results of this research show that
these different dimensions can discriminate between differ-
ent promotional outcomes and experiences. The fact that Acknowledgements
both CES and the EBFS had high positive loadings on
adjective descriptors that fell in the identified positive/ The comments and helpful advice of Cristel Russell and
negative and inward/outward dimensions provides strong Raj Manchanda are gratefully acknowledged. The authors
convergent support for the validity of the PAS as a mea- would particularly like to extend their appreciation to Priya
surement instrument. The ability of the PAS affect catego- Raghubir for her part in the development of the ideas for
ries to discriminate between individuals who vary in terms this paper.
of their assessment of the firm, the promotion itself, and the
responsibility for receiving a promotion, as well as the
ability to identify the category of the promotional offer References
and whether or not an individual was able to take advantage
of the offer, suggests that the PAS will be a useful tool in Aaker DA, Stayman DM, Vezina R. Identifying feelings elicited by
promotion research. advertising. Psychol Mark 1988;5:1 – 16 [Spring].
H. Honea, D.W. Dahl / Journal of Business Research 58 (2005) 543–551 551

Batra R, Ray ML. Affective responses mediating acceptance of advertising. vances in consumer research, vol. 21. Provo (UT): Association for
J Consum Res 1986;13:234 – 49. Consumer Research; 1994. p. 16 – 22.
Breen P. Seeds of Change. Promotion Trends 2000. Annual Report of the Ortony A, Clore GL. Disentangling the affective lexicon. Proceedings of
Promotion Industry Report Compiled by Promo; 2000. p. A3. May. the Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Berke-
Burke MC, Edell JA. The impact of feelings on ad-based affect and cog- ley; 1981. August.
nition. J Mark Res 1989;26:69 – 84 [February]. Plutchik R. Emotion: a psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper;
Blattberg RC, Neslin SA. Sales promotion: concepts, methods and strat- 1980.
egies. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall; 1990. Raghubir P, Corfman K. When do price promotions affect pretrial brand
Chandon P, Wansink B, Laurent G. A benefit congruency framework of evaluations? J Mark Res 1999;36(2):211 – 23 [May].
sales promotion effectiveness. J Mark 2000;64:65 – 81 [October]. Richins ML. Measuring emotions in the consumption experience. J Con-
Cohen JB, Areni CS. Affect and consumer behavior. In: Robertson TS, sum Res 1997;24:127 – 46 [September].
Kassarjian H, editors. Handbook of consumer behavior. Englewood Schindler RM. The excitement of getting a bargain: some hypotheses con-
Cliffs (NJ): Prentice-Hall; 1991. p. 188 – 240. cerning the origins and effects of smart-shopper feelings. Adv Consum
Edell JA, Burke MC. The power of feelings in understanding advertising Res 1989;6:447 – 53.
affects. J Consum Res 1987;14:421 – 33 [December]. Scott CA. The effects of trial and incentives on repeat purchase behavior.
Holbrook M, Batra R. Assessing the role of emotions as mediators of J Mark Res 1976;13(3):263 – 70 [August].
consumer responses to advertising. J Consum Res 1987;14:404 – 20 Shaver P, Schwartz J, Kirson D, O’Connor C. Emotion knowledge: further
[December]. exploration of a prototype approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 1987;52:
Inman JJ, McAlister L. Do coupon expiration dates affect consumer 1061 – 86 [June].
behavior. J Mark Res 1994;31(3):423 – 9 [August]. Smith CA, Ellsworth PC. Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. J Pers
Izard CE. Human emotions. New York: Plenum; 1977. Soc Psychol 1985;48(4):813 – 38 [April].
Kotler P. Marketing management. Upper Saddle River (NJ): Prentice- Smith CA, Lazarus RS. Appraisal components, core relational themes, and
Hall; 2000. the emotions. Cogn Emot 1993;7(3/4):233 – 69 [May – July].
Lichtenstein DR, Netemeyer RG, Burton S. An examination of deal prone- Strahilevitz MA, Myers JG. Donations to charity as purchase incen-
ness across sales promotions types: a consumer segmentation perspec- tives: how well they work may depend on what you are trying to
tive. J Retail 1997;73(2):283 – 97. sell. J Consum Res 1998;24(4):434 – 46 [March].
Mehrabian A, Russell JA. An approach to environmental psychology. Yoo C, Park J, MacInnis D. Effects of store characteristics and in-store
Cambridge (MA): MIT Press; 1974. emotional experiences on store attitude. J Bus Res 1998;42:253 – 63
Morgan RL, Heise D. Structure of emotions. Soc Psychol Q 1988;51(1): [July].
19 – 31. Zaichkowsky JL. Measuring the involvement construct. J Consum Res
Oliver RL. Conceptual issues in the structural analysis of consumption 1985;12(3):341 – 52 [December].
emotion, satisfaction, and quality. In: Allen CT, John DR, editors. Ad-

You might also like