You are on page 1of 42

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group1

Matthias Gerner
City University of Hong Kong

ABSTRACT. This paper explores a lexicalization process in an array of Yi


(^ Tibeto-Burman) languages spoken in Southwest China. Almost every Yi
language exhibits a set of minimal pairs, called simplex-complex pairs, of the
following sort. The simplex member is an adjective or verb starting with a
voiced consonant (e.g. ve33 'sway'). The complex member is derived from the
former by devoicing the initial consonant and by augmenting its valency or
adding causative meaning to it (e.g./e33 'wave = cause to sway*). Previous
authors have treated this kind of data as a blind process of sound change and
were undecided on the type of linguistic change, i.e. whether it represents a
kind of grammaticalization or lexicalization. With pan-Yi data and statistical
figures it is demonstrated that the sound changes constitute a phonetically
and semantically motivated process of lexicalization.

1. Introduction
The aim of this paper is to report and evaluate a remarkable process of
lexicalization displayed in the lexicon of an array of Yi languages which are
Tibeto-Burman languages spoken in Southwest China (see appendix for
more spatial-genetic information). For a limited set of mainly monosyllabic
verbs - between 5 and 50 depending on the language - it is possible to
devoice the initial consonant and derive a causative verb. In Weining Neasu
(one of the Yi languages surveyed), there is for example the verb ve33
meaning 'sway* being involved as the predicate of an intransitive clause (e.g.
'the tree swayed by itself). Its devoiced counterpart fe33 'wave = cause to
sway* appears as the predicate of a monotransitive clause (e.g. 'He waved the
1

Part of this research was supported by a Strategic Research Grant (Project No.
7001921) of City University of Hong Kong.
Folia Linguistica Historica 28/1-2 (2007), 145-185.
ISSN 0165-4004, E-ISSN 1614-7308 Mouton de Gruyter
- Societas
Linguistica
Europaea
Brought
to you
by | University
of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

146

Matthias Gerner

handkerchief). Throughout this paper, I will term the voiced component as


the simplex and the devoiced member as the complex of the pair.
The correlation devoicing-causation has been recognized before, mainly
in the Chinese literature, without, however, characterizing it as a motivated
process of lexicalization. The best-known western account is MatisofPs work
on Lahu, a close genetic relative of the Yi languages (cf. Matisoff 1973: 32-34,
1975, 1976: 414-419). Matisoff accounts for this phenomenon in a
perspective of reconstructing PTB (Proto-Tibeto-Burman). He regards the
devoicing process as the vestiges of a reconstructed PTB prefix *s- then
functioning as transitivizer or causitivizer (see also Benedict 1972: 105-108).
Synchronic data of two conspicuous Tibeto-Burman languages are adduced
in this context, those of Written Tibetan2 and Jinghpaw/Kachin3 where verbs
allow the prefixation of *s- (Jinghpaw: fa- or f a - ) to derive their causative
pendants. According to Matisoff, the PTB prefix *s- had successively evolved
into a Proto-Loloish causative prefix *?-. (The Loloish or, more precisely, the
Burmese-Lolo languages constitute one of the six components of the TibetoBurman language family.) The Proto-Loloish prefix *?- in turn was realized
in particular Loloish languages through various supra-segmental phenomena
such as devoicing, aspiration and tone alterations. In another contribution,
La Polla (2000) analyzed the various strategies by which Dulong/Rawang
changes the valency of clauses. Dulong/Rawang is a Central Tibeto-Burman
language spoken on both sides of the China/Myanmar frontier. He reports
the existence of a causative prefix siu21/tiu21 (tiu21 is used before voiceless
fricative initial, while sui21 is used before all other initials), but not of any
suprasegmental phenomena as in the Yi languages. The Dulong/Rawang
prefix may actually reflect the preservation of this prefix at an early PTBstage. We will elaborate on this idea in section 5.2.
Besides a variety of individual Tibeto-Burman grammars, which mention
the devoicing-causation correlation, focused accounts are mainly offered by
Chinese linguists. The articles of Chen Shilin et al. (1962) and Chen Kang
(1990) report the devoicing-causation connection for Liangshan Nuosu
which also belongs to the 11 Yi languages surveyed in this paper. Dai (1981)
2

Written (classical) Tibetan is classified in the Western or Bodish branch of TB


languages.
3
Jingphaw/Kachin is a language spoken in northern Burma with minor extensions
into western Yunnan (China) and India. It belongs to the Sal branch of TB
languages. See also the next footnote.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 147

and Xu (1984) explore causative verbs in Jingphaw/Kachin. The article of


Dai (1981) includes the information that in Zaiwa (a Burmese language4 of
the Burmese-Lolo language group within the TB family), supra-segmental
processes such as aspiration and tone alteration are involved in the
derivation of causative verbs. These processes seem to coexist with the
causitivizer prefix fa- mentioned above, albeit on different segments of the
lexicon (see also Xu 1984). These data from the Burmese language Zaiwa
seem to expand MatisofFs view which took suprasegmental causative
processes to hold only for Loloish languages (not necessarily for Burmish
languages).
Finally there are works of Chinese linguists attempting to reconstruct and
classify the category of causative verbs for the whole range of Tibeto-Burman
languages. Du (1990) views PTB as exhibiting an agglutinative and later
inflectional morphology type. Causation belonged to the grammatical web of
the verb at an equal footing with, for example, tense and aspect. Successively,
when Old-TB shifted its morphological type to isolating, causation as a
grammatical category was shed and became a mere lexical phenomenon.
This vision is also supported by Dai (2001) who offers a reconstruction of the
causative in PTB. Similar to Matisoff, Dai establishes a proto prefix *s- for
PTB, but in contrast with the former, Dai does not involve an intermediate
glottal prefix *?- from which the suprasegmental processes would have
started. Rather, Dai regards the direct merging of *s- with Stative verbs as the
source of the processes of devoicing, aspiration, etc. Curiously, Matisoff s
earlier reconstruction is not mentioned by Dai (2001). Sun's paper (Sun
1999) is a synchronic classification of the morpho-syntactic strategies that
certain, mainly Chinese, TB languages employ to express the category of
causative verb. These strategies are prefixation, suffixation and suprasegmental processes, i.e. devoicing, aspiration and tone alteration.

Native speakers of Jinghpaw and Zaiwa share a common culture and residence
area. The superordinated ethnic group including both Jinghpaw and Zaiwa is
referred to as Kachin (in Myanmar) and Jinghpaw/Jingpo (in China). The majority
of these people live in Myanmar, where speakers of the Jinghpaw form the majority,
while in China native Zaiwa speakers constitute the largest part of the Jingpo
nationality and native Jinghpaw speakers form a tiny fraction. Nevertheless, because
of historical factors not yet explained, native Zaiwa speakers consider themselves to
be Jinghpaw, even though both languages belong to different branches of the TB
family (cf. Bradley 1997).
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

148

Matthias Gerner

The current paper differs somewhat from earlier contributions as it


attempts to argue that the devoicing-causative correlation is not the result of
a blind process of sound change, as implied by previous authors,5 but rather a
motivated process of lexicalization by which an entire lexical class came into
existence. The structure of this paper is as follows. In section 2, I discuss
some terminological issues, namely the notions of causation and lexicalization. A large part will be devoted to the description of the phenomenon from
the phonetic and the semantic angle. In section 3, I produce a number of
comprehensive tables where the causative verbs of the whole range of Yi
languages are represented and ordered with respect to the devoiced
consonant's articulation point and mode. In section 4,1 introduce the set of
Weining Neasu's causative verbs. With nearly 45 causative verb pairs,
Weining Neasu is the TB language with the highest proven number of verb
pairs. Weining Neasu's causative verbs are arranged according to the
semantic features of valency, control and situation type. In section 5,1 offer
some statistics on phonetic and semantic properties of causative verbs
suggesting that the sound changes represent a motivated process. In the
appendix, spatial-genetic information of the Yi languages surveyed in this
paper is provided.
2. Terminology
In this section I want to lay some terminological foundations. In section 2.1,
a number of semantic parameters transpiring from seminal works on
causation are introduced. I will moreover assemble some recent insights on
the issue of lexicalization (section 2.2).
2.1 Semantic parameters of causatives
There are many issues related to the concept of causation that have enriched
linguistic theories and cognitive sciences in the last 40 years. Unfortunately,
there is no way of doing justice to them in this introductory section. Below, I
briefly discuss a few outstanding works that may be involved in support of
the claims advanced in this paper, namely the hypothesis that the
lexicalization of causative verbs in Tibeto-Burman is shaped by the causation
5

Some authors even suggest the term of grammaticalization, even though it is rather
a process in the opposite direction.

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 149

type of the simplex verb. Various causation types have been proposed by
scholars depending on the semantic parameters taken into account. More
specifically, four basic factors on which causation types are coined have been
identified:
(a) a participant X, the causer,
(b) a participant Y, the causee,
(c) an event e called by some as "precipitating event" (Frawley 1992:
159), "causing event" (Shibatani 1976:1) or simply "cause" (Song
1990: 10, 1996: 19-20),
(d) a situation s labelled as "result" (Frawley), "caused event" (Shibatani)
or "effect" (Song 1990: 10, 1996: 19).
These parameters led to a widely agreed distinction between so-called
single-event and double-event causatives. Single-event causatives refer to a
unique transitive event e(X, Y) in which the caused situation s is an integral
part of the event e: s(Y) ^ e(X, Y). Double-event causatives dissociate e and s:
s(Y)<Ze(X,Y).
Generative Semantics, a formal linguistic theory popular in the 1960s and
1970s, basically interpreted single-event causatives as double-event
causatives. The examples that were under scrutiny again and again include
c
john killed Bill' (single-event causative) and 'John caused Bill to die'
(double-event causative). From a point of view of semantic interpretation,
both clauses entail the resultative state of Bill's death for which reason they
were taken as equivalent. However, successive linguists dismissed this
identification wishing to separate a cause from its effects. For example, time
adverbials pointing to different cause and effect time frames pose problems
for the Generative Semantics account (see Fodor 1970). It is possible to say
(1) (a) 'John caused Bill to die on Sunday by stabbing him on Saturday', whereas
(b) *'John killed Bill on Sunday by stabbing him on Saturday' is ungrammatical.

Some scholars (for example Shibatani) have further distinguished


between single-event causatives and ordinary transitive clauses, a difference
not upheld in this paper. The following minimal pair of sentences was
adduced to serve as evidence for this distinction (Shibatani 1976:2).

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

150
(2)

Matthias Gerner
(a)
(b)

'John kicked the ice but nothing happened to it/


*7ohn melted the ice but nothing happened to it/

Both events constitute single-event causative events, but Shibatani


classified (2a) as uncausative and (2b) as causative. Yet, the acceptability of
(2a) depends on how narrow one defines the notion of 'happen'. If it
includes the affectedness of the patient, then the state of 'being kicked' leads
to a contradiction in (2a). Consequently, transitive clauses that imply the
affectedness of the patient cannot be distinguished from single-event
causatives. Nevertheless, there appears to be some ground for discriminating
between single-event causatives and transitive situations with no palpable
impact on participants (e.g. 'I know him').6
As we will see in the course of this paper, all causative verbs presented in
sections 3 and 4 are involved in single-event causatives and the distinction
single/double does not yield a sufficiently explanatory backbone for the
process of lexicalization featured herein. A different finely graded list of
parameters is proposed by Dixon (2000: 62) who suggests nine semantic
features that causative constructions react to or show sensibility to:

Comrie (1975, 1976) takes a multi-stratal approach on causatives (in spite of his
typological mono-stratal inclinations) by posing a deep uncausative structure from
where causatives are derived through addition of a causer argument. The mapping of
the grammatical relation of the causee is proposed to proceed from a case hierarchy:
subject > direct object > indirect object > oblique. The case hierarchy predicts the
grammatical relation of the causee in the derived causative construction. The causee
will be mapped on the first grammatical relation of the case hierarchy that has no
argument status for the underlying uncausative verb. The main problem with
Comrie's theory is the empirical lack of languages that follow this type of case
assignment (cf. Palmer 1994: 219). A typological work of a different type is Song's
cross-linguistic endeavour based on 408 languages, cf. Song (1989, 1990, 1996). He
distinguished three causation types: the COMPACT type, the AND type and the
PURP type. The COMPACT type corresponds to single causatives and the AND
type is comparable to double-event causatives. The PURP type refers to a third type
of causatives wherein the causing event is performed purposefully for obtaining the
caused situation.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group


(I)

Relating to
verb

(i)

151

State/action.

(ii)
(II)

Relating
to causee

(III)

Relating to
causer

Transitivity
(intransitive, monotransitive, ditransitive).
(iii) Control over caused situation
(lacking or having control),
(iv) Volition or willingness of cause
to perform in the caused event,
(v)
Affectedness of causee
by the causing event
(vi) Directedness: does the causer act
directly or indirectly?
(vii) Intention of causer
(accidental or intentional achievement of result),
(viii) Naturalness
(caused event happens fairly natural or with efforts)
(ix) Involvement
(causer involved or not involved in activity)

As it will become obvious in section 4, most causative verb pairs of the Yi


group that have undergone the process of lexicalization display for the
causee of the simplex verb a lack of control over the caused situation. These
observations will be made plain and exploited in section 5.
2.2 On the notion of lexicalization process
Lexicalization is acknowledged in the literature in a synchronic as well
diachronic perspective. Synchronically, it mainly refers to the encoding of
conceptual categories by words of the lexicon. This perspective is basically
pursued by Generative and more recently by Cognitive Semantics. The
current trend of this line of research is cross-linguistic investigation on
possible conflation patterns of complex conceptual structures into one lexical
item (e.g. McCawley 1968; Jackendoff 1990, 2002). Moreover, some scholars
have explored productive word formation such as compounding and
derivation as cases of synchronic lexicalization (e.g. van der Auwera 2002:
20). Diachronically, lexicalization is "the adoption into the lexicon of items
that have fallen outside the productive rules of grammar" (Brinton and
Traugott 2005: 18; Anttila 1989 [1972]: 151). Diachronic lexicalization can

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

152

Matthias Gerner

follow two directions: (i) fusion of parts in a compound or complex word;7


(ii) change from bound morpheme to simple lexeme8 (Brinton and Traugott
2005: 61). As for fusional lexicalization, scholars have distinguished two
major subtypes:
-

Unproductive word formation processes: These are formerly productive


processes of word formation which have become frozen, irregular and
unproductive (e.g. Bauer 1978: 6). The English derivational suffix
-dom might be cited as an example of a formerly productive now
frozen word formation process.
Demorphologization: Hopper (1990: 154) defines demorphologization
as a process by which "a morpheme loses (most of) its grammaticalsemantic contribution to the word and becomes an indistinguishable
part of the construction of the word, while retaining part of its
original phonological substance." The English word alone (all + one)
may be cited as example of demorphologization (Brinton and
Traugott 2005: 52).

As we will see in the following sections, the devoicing-causation process


of this paper is a formerly productive now unproductive word formation
process generating during its course of fossilization some supra-segmental
alterations.

3. The causative verbs of the (other) Yi languages ordered by the


point and mode of articulation
I surveyed the devoicing-causation correlation in 11 Yi languages representing
the whole range of Yi groupings. It turned out that the correlation in question
is attested to various degrees in the lexicon of these languages. As mentioned
above, Weining Neasu is the language with the highest number of causative
7

Brinton and Traugott feature this type of diachronic lexicalization as "increase of


dependency" (2005: 61). For example, 'cobweb' is a lexicalized noun that in Old
English consisted of two components: coppe 'spider' and web Veb' (cf. Brinton and
Traugott 2005: 50).
8
This type produces lexical items through an increase of autonomy of a formerly
bound morpheme. However, the examples that scholars have adduced to justify this
kind of lexicalization are controversial (cf. Brinton and Traugott 2005: 57-61 for
more details).
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 153

pairs (nearly 45). On the other end of the spectrum lies Luoping Nase (see
appendix) with only one or two attested cases of devoiced causative pairs. Yet,
in Luoping Nase a set of ambitransitive verbs can be used either as intransitive
or as transitive predicate. Some of these verbs are monosyllabic with voiced
initial consonant for the simplex as well as for complex forms. The other Yi
languages lie in between these extremes.
For all Yi languages, mono-syllabicity is a predominant lexical phenomenon, while two-syllabic words also make up a significant part of the lexicon.
Multi-syllabicity, however, is much rarer. Yi languages and, more generally,
Loloish languages are what Matisoff (1994: 115-116) calls omni-syllabic
tonal languages with an extremely simple core syllabic canon: (C) VT.
Devoicing is the basic phonetic process involved in the production of
causative verbs. Additional suprasegmental changes such as aspiration, tone
alteration, and sometimes even alteration of the point of articulation, often
join in. In this section, the data are arranged according to the point of
articulation of the initial consonant (bilabial, labio-dental, alveolar, retroflex,
alvelo-palatal and velar) and secondarily by the mode of articulation (stop,
nasal, fricative and lateral). It appears that the devoicing-causation
correlation has effectively spread across the whole range of consonants.

3.1 Devoicing of bilabial stops


Example (3) displays the causative verb pair 'carry' / 'make carry' in Gejiu
Nesu. The (a) version always incorporates a simplex verb, the (b) version
employs a complex verb.
(3)

Gejiu Nesu

(a) k3>55 a55ko33 t<?he21 tV3 ba55 sa33.


3PSG chd

NUM:1 CL

carry

DUR

'She carries a child on the back/


(b) qo33 a55ko33 gu21 to55 pa55.
1PSG child

PAT 3PSG make carry

make him carry the child/

There is one interesting exception in this group of causative verb pairs.


For Yongren Lolo, we have one deaspiration-causative correlation (not a
devoicing-causative correlation), which is phui33 'explode' (intr.) / pui33
'explode' (tr.). For illustration, I have also represented the ambitransitive

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

154

Matthias Gerner

'peel off
'scrape off

Simplex
Complex

'flow'

Simplex

'cause to flow'

Complex

'collapse'

Simplex

2
:2

1
ff

2
l

o i l

ba33

be13

pha33

'

ba

55

pa55
21

br
bv21

br33

be21
h 21

Complex

pe

Simplex

'loosen'

Complex p h u 21

Open' (intr.)

Simplex

Open' (tr.)

Complex

bu

bu21

p h u 21
mbo

'roll' (tr.)

Complex po
'explode, crack' (intr.) Simplex bi33

21

'explode, crack' (intr.) Complex pi

33

33

33

bu33
pu55
bi13
h 13

pi

_P9 3 _ 3 __pV 3 _
pho33 pV 3
pui2'
pui21
bi2121

bi

bu33 po33
phu33 po33

phui33 bi55
pui33

pi55

55

'appear'

Simplex

'reveal'

Complex pi55

bi

PY

21

'loose'

Simplex

(7) Mile Azhee

! 't-

=3
.fl

Mile Axi

4>

(5) Shizong Kopho

(2) Weining Neasu

t1
Qu

Complex

'make carry'

'roll' (intr.)

JG

Simplex

'carry on back'

'make collapse'

Liangshan Nuosu

Name of Language

cases, namely the verb pairs with identical simplex and complex
components. Besides Luoping Nase mentioned above, this concerns also one
pair in Longlin Ngopho and two pairs in Mile Azhee.

Figure 1: Devoicing of bilabial stops in the Yi group

3.2 Devoicing of labio-dental fricatives


The pair 'wear' / 'dress' has a devoicing process for almost every Yi language.
In most languages of the sample it sounds like v/* / fi*. Only in Liangshan
Nuosu has the corresponding pair a velar onset: ga55 Vear' / ka55 'dress'
(see subsection 3.6).

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 155


'wear'
'sway'
'dress'
Name of Language
Simplex
Simplex
Complex
vi13
Weining Neasu
ve33
fi13
55
55
Longlin Ngopho
vei
fei
VJ'3
Luoping Nase
vi13
13
vi
(5) Shizong Kopho
fi13
33
(6) Mile Axi
vi
fi55
21
Mile Azhee
vi
fi21
21
vi
Gejiu Nesu
fi21
33
Wuding Aluphu
vi
fi33
Figure 2: Devoicing of labiodental fricatives in the Yi group

'wave'
Complex
fe33

3.3 Devoicing of alveolar consonants


The group of alveolar consonants is well represented allowing the devoicing
of both alveolar stops and laterals. Stops are the mode of articulation where
the lexicalization process has spread and concentrated most of its output.
3.3.1 Devoicing of alveolar stops
Again, there is one pair in Longlin Ngopho, and one in Luoping Nase where
both simplex and complex forms are of the same voice level. From the wide
spectre of causative verbs starting with alveolar stops, I have selected two
examples, one illustrating the pair 'say* / 'tell' (Luoping Nase),9 the other
demonstrating the couple 'study' / 'teach' (Weishan Lalo).10

There is no direct causative relation between 'say' and 'tell' in the sense that the
causee of 'tell' would be the agent of saying. Rather, 'tell' can be derived from 'say'
through a valency increase of a different type. The new semantic role added to the
frame of 'tell' is not the causer but rather a new role for recipient or beneficiary.
10
Besides Weishan Lalo, the causative verb pair 'study' / 'teach' is elsewhere attested
in Lahu, a language related to the Yi languages spoken in parts of Yunnan Province
(P.R. of China) and Thailand, see Matisoff (1975).
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

Matthias Gerner

S = Simplex
C = Complex

'drink'
'give to

Name of Language

156

1
c

I^ fP
rt

S ndo33
C to21

-2

1
Z

I
f
f
?o
"
o
b
'

1 ZJ
^

>

|
o

1
>->

-f !
>-i
<

3a

1
2

5P

g >

ndo33

to33

nt fi o 21

do13

du33

da21

du55

do33

do33

to21

to55

nt fi o 21

to33

tu33

te 21

tu33

to33

to33

dze33

dza21

dzo33

dzo33

drink'

'eat'
'feed'

'say'
'tell'
'be left

S dzui33 dzu33
C t$a33 tu33
S
C
S dzi33 dz21

dz21
t^u
55

die
t h i 2i

di

dzo21 dzo21

55

t^o

33

55

t?o

21

tse

33

jtqa

21

t^o

33

t?o33

33

dzo

t h ie 2i

t h e 2i

dze33

dze33

tse33

tse33

over'
'leave over' C tshi33
'stand'
'build'/

33

tsi13
33

S dzu
dzo
C tshu33 tso33

'place'

'sit'
'make sit'
/'build'

di33

ti33

s
c
s

dui

'break'

thui33

(tr.)
'split'
(intr.)

dz55

'lie down'
'place'
'break'
(intr.)

'split' (tr.)
'collapse'
'make
collapse'
'study,
learn'
'teach'

c
s
c

da21
ta 21
33

tsi55
55

de33

55

the33

d3>
h

t g-

dzu55

tsu33

Figure 3: Devoicing of alveolar stops in the Yi group

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

'ascend'
'lift up'
'enter'
'bury
= place in'

i
Z

o
z0

=>

If
I

! t s? 2 1 !
I I t

>

S
C
S
C

'wear'
S ndi55
(hat, shoe)

>

di33
ti33
dy21
ty21

di13

d321

dy21

dv55

t921

ty21

tY55

'dress'
(hat, shoe)

C ti55

ti13

'bright,
shine'

do13

'light,
kindle'

to13

'appear,
exit'

dr55do3 dou55

'reveal'

'be
perforated'
'perforate'
'burn'
(intr.)
'burn' (tr.)
'peel off
'scrape off
'turbid,
muddy'
'stir up'

1
*

Wuding Aluphu

C = Complex

zc

Yongren Lolo

S = Simplex

Name of Language

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 157

S
C
S ndu55

thY55

du

tu55

21

t h u 21

du21

C tu55
S
C
S

tu21

de

te21

dui21
t h uj 21
21

Figure 3: Devoicing of alveolar stops in the Yi group (contd.)

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

158

Matthias Gerner

(4)

Luoping Nase
(a)

tgra21 ?u21

tha55 mi55?u21 die55.

3PSG

with

1PSG

speak

word

'He is talking to me/


(b) ?u21

no55

ta 55

1PSG

affair

NUM:1 CL

thhie2

tar) 33

2PSG tell

tell you something/


Weishan Lalo (Bj rverud 1998:139)

(5)
(a)

de21 dzu55 li33.


hit

study

(b) de21
hit

IMP

qa55

tsu33 li 33

1PSG

teach

IMP

'Teach me how to play1/

'Learn to play!*
3.3.2 Devoicing of alveolar lateral

There is only one pair in this category that constitutes a genuine case of a
de voicing-causative correlation. The other two cases exhibit ambitransitive
verbs with intransitive as well as transitive uses (of the type S = O). The
Weining Neasu pair lr33 'move' (intr.) / tr33 'move' (tr.) is illustrated by the
following examples:
(6)

(a)

Weining Neasu
si 33 ga55

dze13 ko55

lY33.

tree

CL

move

DEM:DIST

DUR

'That tree is moving [e.g. its branches and leaves by the wind]/

(b)

33

SI
tree

-?a55

dze13 Qi21

le55

tr33.

)EM:DIST

CL

PASS

move

3P SG

'That tree is moved by him/her [e.g. by shaking the stem]/


'move'

'make move'

'flow'

'make flow'

'roll'

'make roll'

Name of Language

Simplex

Complex

Simplex

Complex

Simplex

Complex

Weining Neasu

hr33

4r33
lui33

lui33

122

122

Luoping Nase
M eAxi

Figure 4: Devoicing of alveolar lateral in the Yi group

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 159


3.4 Devoicing of retroflex stops

Name of Language

The next group regroups causative verbs starting with a retroflex stop. This
kind of stop belongs to the consonant canon of most Yi languages. The
majority of these stops are affricated. Only Weining Neasu (out of the set of
Yi languages surveyed in this paper) has genuine unaffricated stops (i.e. < [
and *). The following example illustrates the causative pair (fee55 'be at' /
t$he55 'place' in Weining Neasu.

'be at'
'put at, build'
'shine'
'project'
*be afraid'
'frighten'
'come out'
'liberate'
'collapse'
'make collapse'
'torn'
'tear'
'explode'
'make explode'
'break' (intr.)
'break' (tr.)
'peel off
'scrape off
'burn' (intr.)
'burn' (tr.)
'melt' (intr.)
'melt' (tr.)

Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex

I
?

1 1 4
4
* I 1 j 2 5
i s

ff

SP

1 1 ! 1 1 1 I

<foe55 c&o33 cjzp21


the55 tshou55 tsu53

4P

tho55

4sp3333 ^sy3333

teo teu

dY555

tV33

<te3333
t?9

tha33
ta33

l^a33

433

tehu33
dr13
tv13
21
tu13

Figure 5: Devoicing of retroflex stops in the Yi group

fei21
t?i21

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

160

Matthias Gerner

(7)
(a)

WeiningNeasu
se21vu33 mo21 tha21
peach tree

fruit

mo13 se21vu33 khje55 c&e55.

NUM:1 CL

peach tree

LOOon bear, beat

Teaches are on the peach tree/


(b) tol3b13 kje33 <?i21
big stone

PAT

3P SG

Ie21be21 k hj e 55 tV5.
neck

LOCat

place

'Put a big stone around his neck!'

3.5 Devoicing of alvelo-palatal consonants


There are three types of alvelo-palatal consonants that have developed a
devoicing-causation correlation: alvelo-palatal stops, nasals and fricatives.
For this first type of consonant, a fair number of exponents have undergone
the devoicing-causation process.
3.5.1 Devoicing of alvelo-palatal stops
The consonants of this category are affricated and are represented in six
languages of the sample surveyed in this paper. Example (8), originating
from Liangshan Nuosu, exemplifies the causative pair 'graze' / lead to
pasture'.
(8)

Liangshan Nuosu
(a)

Ira33 tsi55;t<?e33 <]Uo33 bo33 o44.


ox
oneself
graze go
DP
'The oxen are grazing by themselves/

(b)

lui33
ox

tshi33 de33
3PSG PAT

ndi21p33 ko33
meadow
LOG

jteo44
pasture

ta33.
STP

'He led the oxen to pasture/

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

'be lacking'
'take off
'left over'
'leave over'
'be afraid'
'frighten'
'go across'
'make go across'
'graze, pasture'
'turn out to grass'
'burn' (intr.)
'burn' (tr.)
'melt' (intr.)
'melt' (tr.)

Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
Simplex
Complex
33
Simplex dso
33
Complex Jteo
33
Simplex d?
Complex jt(?933
Simplex dzi33
Complex Jtei33

d?e13
jt<?he13
Jt<?855

55

jt<?e
d*o33 Jtpi33
33
*<?033 JtQi
21
Dtdo
Jt<?o13

Yongren Lolo

Gejiu Nesu

(5) Shizong Kopho

Luoping Nase

(D Weining Neasu

Name of Language

Liangshan Nuosu

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 161

d^e13 d^e21
jtge33 jtqe55

dzi21

d^i21
Jt<?i
d^i21
Figure 6: Devoicing of alvelo-palatal stops in the Yi group
13

d?i33
JtQi33

3.5.2 Devoicing of alvelo-palatal nasals


The sole nasal pair of the whole sample comes from Liangshan Nuosu and
represents a valency extension from one to two places of the type csit' / *make

sif.
(9)

Liangshan Nuosu
(a)nui 33 me21
n,i33.
2P SG in front sit

(b) a44?!33 a44mo33 de33


child

mother

i55go33 ko33

mother bed

ta,33

LOC:on make sit STP

'Mother made the child sit on the bed/

'Sit at the front!'


'sit'

'make sit'

Name of Language
Simplex
Complex
33
(D Liangshan Nuosu *i
: iii21
Figure 7: Devoicing of alvelo-palatal nasals in the Yi group

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

162

Matthias Gerner

3.5.3 Devoicing of alvelo-palatal fricatives


Within this devoicing category there is one uncorrupted example (that of
Yongren Lolo) and one tainted example (that of Mile Axi whose complex is
not a fricative but an affricated stop). The following two sentences illustrate
Yongren Lolo's causative verb pair.

(10)

Yongren Lolo

kV3 si21 do33.

be33tsi33 tS833lu33 t h i 21

(a)
3P SG

garment

beautiful

NUM: 1 CL

wear

DUR

'She wears a beautiful garment.'

(b) o21mo33 ^p21


mother

<si55.

t h ie 21 be33tsi33 ge55

3P SG BEN

garment

PAT

dress

'Mother dressed her with a garment.'


'melt* (intr.)
Simplex
tV2?i55

Name of Language
(6) Mile Axi
Yongren Lolo

'melt* (tr.)
Complex

'wear*
Simplex

'dress*
Complex

tho22tci55

?i21

Qi55

Figure 8: Devoicing of alvelo-palatal fricatives in the Yi group


3.6 Devoicing of velar stops

Finally, velar stops as the target of the devoicing process are particularly well
attested in Liangshan Nuosu and Weining Neasu, the two northernmost
languages of the sample. In Yongren Lolo, an interesting pair exists where
the directional verb 'return* has the lexicalized causative relative 'return,
bring back'. In (lib) the verb 33 'bring' serves as an optional support of
the causative verb.
(11)

Yongren Lolo

(a) go33
IP SG

xi33kV3 go21
home

return

:33

go

bring the book back home.'

return home.'

(b) go 33

IP SO

5S

33

33

su b3

book

bring

:33uh 33

Al

JV c>

home

kh

21

-33

IV 7

4>1

bring back

go

bring the book back home.'


Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

'wear'
Simplex ga55
55
'dress'
Complex ka
'be afraid'
Simplex gu33
gu33
33
'frighten*
Complex ku
ku33
33
'hear'
Simplex gin
'tell'
Complex km33
'enclosed'
Simplex
gv33
'enclose, arrest'
Compkx
k*33
'return'
Simplex
'bring back'
Complex
go' 3
'curved, bowed, bent' Simplex
'curve, bend, bow'
Complex
kh33
o13
'split, burst' (intr.)
Simplex gI33
'split, burst' (tr.)
Complex ki33
ki33
'melt' (intr.)
Simplex
g355
'melt' (tr.)
Complex
ka55
Figure 9: Devoicing of velar stops in the Yi group

163

Yongren Lolo

(9) Weishan Lalo

Weining Neasu

Liangshan Nuosu

Name of Language

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group

go21
ko21

gi

4. Causative verbs in Weining Neasu ordered by semantic parameters


Weining Neasu occupies a remarkable position among the 11 Yi languages
surveyed in this paper. I counted a total of nearly 45 causative verb pairs in
field data available to me. As mentioned above, the complex component of
these verb pairs points to single-event causatives. Recalling Dixon's nine
parameters of causatives mentioned in section 2.1 (cf. Dixon 2000:62), we will
organize the Neasu causative verb pairs around two semantic traits: the kind of
valency extension, namely from intransitive to monotransitive (cf. section 4.1)
or from monotransitive to ditransitive (cf. section 4.2), and the degree of
control the causee has over the caused situation. Let us mark by S the sole
argument role of the simplex verb in case that it is intransitive and by A the
main argument of the simplex verb if and only if it is monotransitive. (For the
semantic macro-roles S and A, see Dixon 1979,1994).

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

164

Matthias Gerner

4.1 Causation as valency extension from one to two places


Causative verb pairs that exhibit a valency extension from one to two places
make up the lion's share of all causative verb pairs with ca. 35 occurrences.
SemanticaUy, the simplex verb may be involved in states, processes and
actions with a unique participant S. We distinguish the case where S lacks
control (section 4.1.1) and where it is in control of the situation (section
4.1.2).
4. LI S of simplex verb lacks control
Intransitive simplex verbs that incorporate an S argument lacking control
over the situation may be involved in states (section 4.1.1.1) and processes
(section 4.1.1.2). Both subcategories comprise the majority of all causative
verb pairs.
4.1.1.1 S of simplex verb lacks control of state
About seven simplex verbs in my sample are stative and describe a
positional, structural or appearance state. The corresponding complex verbs
add a generally human causer who is directly and intentionally involved in
the activity. The S argument of the simplex verb equals the causee of the
complex verb and both lack control in the respective situation they are
engaged in.
Simplex
Complex
the55
'put at' (lit. cause to be located)
c(255 'be at' (locational)
33
'build'
dzo
'stand' (for immobile objects) tso33
kr33
'enclose, arrest'
gr33
'enclosed'
d^e13 'be lacking'
;fc<?he13
'takeoff
h 55
55
t
r
'make
loose, untie'
(fr
'loose'
do13
'bright'
to13
'light, kindle'
clo55
'shine'
tho55
'project'
Figure 10: Pairs with uncontrolled stative simplex verb

In the subsequent illustrations, the simplex verb in example (a) is


contrasted with the derived complex verb in example (b). The sole argument
S of the simplex is inanimate in general but may be animate as in (13a). In
any case S does not exert control over the situation. In (14b), the complex
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group

165

verb tee13 'take off should be analysed as 'cause something to be missing'


originating from the simplex verb (foe13 'be missing'. The causative verb pair
in (16a+b) refers to a state of appearance.
(12)(a)lu 2 1
city

dzD33 bo21
be built

khV5

te13.

mountain LOCron

placed

The house is built on a mountain.'


(b) 2155 hi21mo21 t h a 21 dzr33 tso33.
IP PL

house

NUM:1 CL

build

"We build a house.'


(13) (a) mu33 ko55 gv33
horse

lo33.

inside enclosed ADV

The horse is enclosed inside.'


(b)qo 21 tgho55je21
1PSG

friend

tha21

jo 21

kje33

kv33 13.

NUM:1

CL

COV

arrest

SEND

"I have arrested a friend.'


(14) (a) se33la13
cup

na55

kV 3

<jUe13 lo33.

DEM:MED CL

lack

ADV

The cup is missing.'


(b)ba 33 ba 33 tha21
bread

pha21

NUM:1 half

ice13

na21 t?a33.

takeoff

2P SG feed

'Half of the bread was taken off for you.'


(15) (a) i<?hi55nde3V33 jo l3 mu 33 <&55 lo33.
shoelaces

oneself

loose ADV

The shoelaces loosened themselves.'


33

kje33

3PSG shoelaces

COV

loosen

SEND

'He loosened the shoelaces.'


(16)(a)ti 3 3
lamp

ga55

dze13 do13 pje13.

DEM:DIST CL

bright very

That lamp is very bright.'


(b)mi 33 k h i 13 go 13 ,r)o 21
sky

dark

DP

1PSG

ti33 to13.
lamp light

'It is dark now, so I will light the lamp.'


Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

166

Matthias Gerner

4.1.1.2 S of simplex verb lacks control of process


Intransitive simplex verbs that refer to an uncontrolled process constitute the
most productive group of verbs targeted by the causative lexicalization
process. Causative verbs pairs of this kind can be subsumed in turn under a
number of labels, namely verbs of motion, of disposition and verbs of
physical affectedness. The first subgroup comprises simplex verbs that are
verbs of motion.
Simplex
bu33

Complex
'roll (tr.), incite'
'roll' (intr.)
pu55
33
'wave'
ve
'sway'
fe33
ir33
'shake, move' (intr.)
lY33
'shake, move' (intr.)
33
h 33
da
'cause to collapse, destroy'
'collapse'
ta
'collapse, come down'
fa'3thr33 'cause to collapse'
:21
'stretch out' (tr.)
'stretch out' (intr.)
tH921
be21
phe21
'throw down, cause to fall'
'fall down'
Figure 11: Pairs with uncontrolled simplex verb of motion (1st subgroup)

The motion that is expressed by the simplex verbs is basically preserved


in the complex verbs with a few secondary meanings such as 'cause to roll' >
'incite, provoke' (for pu55) in (17b) or 'cause to collapse' > 'destroy' (for
tha33} in (19b). The complex verb appends a causer who directly and
intentionally performs the activity in each case.
(17) (a) mo21ta13 ?o13mu33 bu33 lo33. (b) go2
ball

oneself

roll

l P SG

ADV

The ball rolls by itself.'


(18) (a) si33thu33 tha21 33 ve33.
tree leave

NUM: 1 CL

sway

Tree leaves are swaying.'


(19) (a) hi

21

h 33

to

33

(la

33

lo .

house ART:PROX collapse ADV

The house collapsed.'

vu33

mo21ta13 pu55.

snow

ball

roll

am rolling a snow ball.'


(b)

<?i21
3PSG

la13
hand

fe33
wave

rjo21 mo55.
1PSG point to

'He waved his hand towards me.'


(b) <?i21 hi21 tha55

dzr33 tha33.

3P SG house DEM:PROX CL

destroy

'He destroyed this house.'

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 167


(20) (a)

se21vu33 si33

khje55 be21 za13

li21.

peach

LOC:on fall

come

tree

descend

The peach has fallen from the tree/


(b) b33di33 tha21
bowl

NUM:1

kV3 ?i2ltha55

ci21

CL

3P SG throw

top of building

phe21 za13

ka55.

descend

add

'He threw that bowl down from the top of the building/
A few simplex verbs refer to states of disposition a subject may be found
in. Two causative verb pairs in my data set belong to this subtype and are
listed in figure 12 below. The related complex verbs point to intentional
activities performed by a causer and endured by a causee.
Simplex
Complex
bu21
Open' (intr.) phu21
'open' (tr.)
dz21
left over'
tsi13(tu33)
leave over'
Figure 12: Pairs with uncontrolled simplex verb of disposition (2nd subgroup)
One specific comment on the use of the complex verb tsin leave over' in
(22b) needs to be made. Even though it can occur as the sole predicate of the
clause, it is often complemented by a beneficiary NP marked by the verb bi55
'give1. The predicational frame of tsi13 is therefore basically monotransitive.
(21) (a) rjgo21 ?o13mu33 bu21 lo33.
door

oneself

(b) Qi21 ggo21 kje33 phu21 33.

open ADV

3P SG door

'The door opened itself/


(22) (a) Jtea33 dz21
food

lo33.

leftover ADV

COV

open

SEND

'He/she opened the door/

(b) na21 tea33 tsi13


2P SG food

'Some food has been left over/

tu33 rjo21 55 21 .

leave over FIX

1PSG give SEND

'Keep some food for me.'

The main subgroup of uncontrolled simplex verbs is displayed in figure


13 and refers to processes of physical affectedness and alteration. The voiced
version hints at an intransitive process of physical change, and the devoiced
version at a transitive activity. In English, by contrast, the causative verbs in
figure 13 are often translated as ambitransitive verbs of the S = type
(Dixon and Aikhenvald 2000: 4-5) where a noun phrase may occupy both
the S and roles of the ambitransitive verb as exemplified in 'the newspaper
burnt' and 'he burnt the newspaper'.

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

168

Matthias Gerner

Complex
thu21
'perforate'
'be perforated'
13
13
'scrape
off (tr.)
dv
'peel off (intr.)
tr
h 33
33
t
u
'break'
(tr.)
'break'
(intr.)
<teP
'stir up, cause to be turbid'
de21
'turbid, muddy"
te21
13
'explode, crack' (intr.)
bi
p h i 13 'cause to explode, crack' (tr.)
'curved, bowed, bent'
kho13 'curve, bend, bow'
go13
21
'melt' (intr.)
d?i
ici13 'cause to melt' (tr.)
21
'burn' (intr.)
'burn' (tr.)
du
tu13
33
33
'tear'
'torn'
ki
gi
'gather = cause to be finalized'
gv21 'used up' (as resultative versatile) kr21
'poison'
do13 'poisoned'
to13
Figure 13: Paks with uncontrolled simplex verb of physical affectedness (3rd
subgroup)
Simplex

du21

These verb pairs are illustrated in the examples (23-28) below. Some of
the simplex or complex verbs of figure 13 cannot be used as the main
predicate of the sentence but must complement another predicate as
resultative versatile. Versatile is a term coined by Matisoff (1969) to refer to
grammaticalized verbs whose products lie in the realm of aspect and
resultative state. The string gr21 'used up' for example cannot function as
sole predicate of a sentence but appears as a resultative versatile in the
postfield of other verbs. A similar situation holds for the complex verb kho13
in (26b) which is bound to co-occur with some subsequent adjective or verb.
(23) (a) rjo21 mbu33 du21
IP
SG

lo33.

clothes perforated ADV

'My clothes got a hole.'


(b) <?i21 dzu33tV3 k hj e 55 du21na33 tha21 na21 thu21.
3PSG table
LOC:on hole
NUM:1 CL perforate

'He made a hole on the table/


(24) (a) va13 bi55se21 bi13 lo33.
pig

gallbladder burst

ADV

'The pig's gallbladder burst/ *


(b)

va13
pig

bi55se21 21
ndu21 pi13
gallbladder 3P SG beat
burst

13.
SEND

'He beat the pig's gallbladder to burst it/


Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 169


21 33
(25) (a) vu33 dUi21
lo3

snow melt

ADV

'The snow melted/


(b) vu33 kje33 tci13 dze33 ji 21 13.
snow COV

melt

then

water get

'Melt the snow and get water/


(26) (a) do21 mo21 na55
stick CL

go13.

DEM:MED bent

'The stick is bent/


(b) go21
1PSG

do21 mo21 na55


stick CL

kV3 *i33to13.

DEM:MED bend

turn

have bent the stick/


(27)(a)ci 2 1

do13

zo21.

3P SG poisened endure

'He endured intoxication/


(b) Qi 21

mo55

su21 le55

to13.

3PSG COV:pointto others PASS

poison

'He was poisoned by others/


(28) (a) mbu33 ?o13mu33 tse33 gi33 lo33.
clothes

oneself

then

torn

ADV

'The clothes are torn by themselves/


(b) Qi21

mbu33 go21

3PSG clothes

IP SG

le55

ki33 13.

PASS

tear

SEND

'His clothes were torn by me/


The fourth and last subgroup of uncontrolled simplex verbs has one
member, the pair fao33 'be afraid' / ieo33 'frighten' which has an
uncontrolled simplex verb with mental meaning. The following sentence
may serve as illustration. The simplex dzo33 'afraid' must be completed by
fGi33 that occurs after the noun phrase that indicates the source of fear.
(29) (a) go21
1PSG

*^33 pi21
afraid

ipi33.

3P SG afraid

am afraid of him/her/

(b) go21 <?i21


1PSG 3P SG

jfc*>33

pje13.

frighten

very

frightened him/her very much/

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

170

Matthias Gerner

4.1.2 S of simplex verb is in control of action


In the Neasu data set there is a small group of intransitive simplex verbs
whose sole argument is in control of the action referred to. The simplex
verbs in question are verbs of movement for which the moving entity exerts
control over the movement. As for all causative pairs displayed in this paper,
the corresponding complex verbs add a generally human causer who is
directly and intentionally involved in the activity, though without moving
himself.
Simplex
dr55do33
iwio21
5

Complex
thY55
'appear*
'reveal'
tco13
'cause someone to go across*
'go across'
'come out*
'set free*
dV
tV5
Figure 14: Pairs with controlled simplex verb of movement
Below are provided examples. The complex verb^o 13 'cause someone to
go across' in (32b) manifests a restriction of usage in that it must co-occur
with the polysemous verb ^V 55 'set free, allow' in a serial verb construction.
(31) (a) rja33 ga55
bird

kV3

DEM:DIST CL

jo13mu33

<\?55

lo33.

oneself

come out

ADV

'The bird came out by itself


(b)rja 33 ga55
bird

kV 3 <?i21

DEM:DIST CL

ihv55

3P SG

13.

set free SEND

'He set the bird free.'


(32) (a) ci21 wUo" na55pha21
3P SG pass over

that side

lr55.
go

'He passes over to the other side.'


(b) Qi21 tN55

921

3P SG set free 1PSG

t*50*3

ma55

make pass NEG

ctau55.
willing

'He was not willing to let me pass.'

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 171


4.2 Causation as valency extension from two to three places
Besides intransitive verbs, monotransitive verbs were also targeted by the
devoicing process of lexicalization. Causative verb pairs that constitute a
valency extension from two to three places occur with significantly lower
frequency. I numbered less than ten occurrences in the field data available to
me.11 We will again organize the causative verbs by whether or not the main
argument A exerts control over the situation referred to by the simplex verb.
4.2.1 A of simplex verb lacks control of state
There are two causative verb pairs where the main argument of the simplex
verb has no control over the situation as provided in figure 15. Both are
similar in meaning referring to the pair 'hear' / 'tell'. In both cases, the
simplex verb 'hear' is arguably a non-control verb. This kind of causative
pair is elsewhere only attested in Liangshan Nuosu (refer to pair gut33 'hear'
and kui33 'tell' in figure 9).
Simplex

Complex
'hear'
iCO33
'hear, understand'
tu55
Figure 15: Pairs of the 'hear' / 'tell' type

cUo33
du55nu13

'tell, cause to hear'


'tell'

In Neasu, a difference appears for the complex verb of both causative


pairs. The verb^o 55 'tell' can make up the sole and independent predicate
of a clause, while tu55 'teil' must co-occur with the verb hi55 'say' in a serial
construction. Further, the pair du55nu13 'hear, understand' - \u55 'tell' is
somewhat special. The voiced syllable du55 is a noun meaning 'word'. In
combination with the verb nu13 'notice' it means 'understand'. The
devoicing takes place here between a noun and a verb.

11

There are also pairs that fail to fully qualify for a simplex-complex pair. They
devoice the initial consonant but do not display a causative meaning. These pairs
include ,33 'estimate' /fact33 'calculate' and nde33 'equip' / te33 'put on'.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

172

Matthias Gerner

^ 1 21 too33.

(33) (a) du55 tho33


word

ARTrPROX yesterday

IP SG get

hear

heard the word yesterday/


(b) Qi21 kr55 a3W3 du
3P SG PUT

aunt

55

i^o33.

word

tell

'He will tell his aunt/


(34) (a) go21
1PSG

a33ma33 du55nu13.
mother

21
(b) go21 Io.21_13
pi

1PSG story

hear

'I heard Mom's voice/

hl 55 Q I 21

say

55

3P SG tell

I told him a story/

4.2.2 A of simplex verb is in control


Two types of monotransitive simplex verbs exhibit the main argument A in
control of the situation: stative verbs (section 4.2.2.1) and action verbs
(section 4.2.2.2). For both types I counted more than six exponents.
4.2.2.1 A of simplex verb is in control of state
There are three stative causative verb pairs of the 'wear' / 'dress' type. The
pair vi13 / fi13 is the general term compatible with a wide range of vesture;
the couple din I ti13 is restricted to the wearing of hats, gloves and shoes but
excludes textiles like trousers. Finally, gu551 ku55 refers to the state of being
covered by cloth or clothing. All three simplex verbs refer to situations
controlled by the person who is in the wearing position.
Simplex
vi *3
'wear' (for clothes)
di13
'wear* (for hat, gloves, shoes)
gu55
Covered' (with cloth or clothes)
Figure 16: Pairs of the 'wear' / 'dress' type

Complex
ftl 3
'dress someone with clothes'
ti13
'dress' (for hat, gloves, shoes)
ku55
'cover' (with cloth or clothes)

For a number of three-place predicational frames, the particle kjs33 can


and should be employed to mark patient NPs. The order is fixed as
BV,12 as illustrated in (35a+b) for the pair vi13 / fi13. For the
12

As stated in part at the start of section 4, the labels A and are the macro-roles
inspired from Dixon (1979,1994). The abbreviation B stands for 'beneficiary'.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group

173

simplex verb gu55 'cover' in (37a) the covering entity is not marked with any
particle or preposition.
(35)

(a) Ci21
3PSG

mbu33

?a33?Y55 tha21 tgV1 vi13.

garment

beautiful

NUM:1 CL

wear

'He/she wears a beautiful garment/


(b) da13
every

,2133
morning

4?i55 a33ma33
all
mother

mbu33 kV3 pi21 ft13.


garment PAT 3 P SG dress

'Every morning mother dresses him/


(36) (a)

Qi21

mo55tsi33 di13.

3P SG

hat

wear

'He wears a hat/


(b)

th'55Je21

mo55tsi33 kje33 Qi21

friend

hat

PAT

ti13.

3P SG dress

'The friend put a hat on him/


(37) (a)

<?i21

mbu33 ma21

gu55.

3P SG

clothes

covered

NEG

'He is not covered with clothes/


(b)

a33ma33

?i13mbu33 rjgo21 <?i21

mother

cloth

pull

ku55.

3P SG cover

'Mother pulled out some cloth to cover him/


4.2.2.2 A of simplex verb is in control of action
In Weining Neasu there are three monotransitive simplex verbs referring to
actions controlled by an agent. This category comprises two verbs of
consumption of the eat and drink type as well as the causative pair 'ride' /
'make ride'. Verbs for eating and drinking have lexicalized across the whole
board of Yi languages constituting the most widespread examples.
Simplex
Complex
dzu33
'eat'
'feed'
tu33
33
ndo
'drink'
to21
'give to drink'
dze33
'ride (a horse)'
tse33
'make ride (a horse)'
Figure 17: Pairs with controlled simplex verb of action

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

174

Matthias Gerner

As in the previous section, the particle kje33 is generally employed to


mark the patient with occasional exceptions such as in (39b). If the particle is
not involved in the sentence, the order in Neasu is fixed as ABOV. The use of
kjs33 achieves a topical foregrounding of the patient NP.
(38) (a) rjo21
1PSG

d?a33 dzu33 go13.


food

eat

(b) Qi 21

DP

3P SG

eat food.'
(39) (a) na21 ijcfe
2PSG wine

ndD21 ndo33?
drink

ride

PREP

PAT

child

feed

mu33 zi21

3P SG horse
c

water

tD21.
let drink

He lets the horse drink some water.'

jtV3rjga13.
go to street market

I ride a horse to go to the market/

(b)qo 21 m33 kje33 ei21


1PSG horse PAT
C

(b) d 21

drink

dze33 kr55

1PSG horse
C

food

'He fed the child/

'Do you drink wine?*


(40) (a) rjo21 m33

<t?a33 kJV3 a33ga55 tu33.

3P SG

tse33

JLi21.

cause to ride come

I cause him/her to come here on a horse/

5 The lexicalization of simplex/complex pairs driven by phonetic


and semantic factors

In this section I propose that the causative verbs in the Yi group (and
beyond) have come into existence through a motivated (not blind) process of
lexicalization. The argumentation proceeds as follows. In subsection 5.1, I
show with statistics that the causative verb pairs have developed through a
motivated process of phonetic and semantic change. In subsection 5.2, I
propose an incremental lexicalization path for the devoicing-causation
correlation.

5.1 Statistics on phonetic and semantic properties of causative verbs


I have counted the number of attested causative pairs in function of the place
and mode of articulation. The distilled results are presented in figure 18
below (the number of ambitransitive verbs is presented in square brackets).
While I claim that for Liangshan Nuosu and Weining Neasu the figures

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 175

provided below represent near-complete numbers, I cannot exclude that for


some of the remaining Yi languages the actual number of causative pairs
could be slightly higher if one pushed hard for finding more exponents.
c

Js
"3

<

B abial

1 f I ! 1 .^ i l l I

u-,
0
<

"C

:gv

Stops

Labio-dental

Fricatives

Alveolar

Stops

13

1[1] 3

Lateral

0[1] 0

0[1] 0

Retroflex

Stops

10

0[1] 0

Alvelo-palatal Stops

Velar

"5

1[1] 0[2] 1

0[3] 1

0[2] 2

*j

2[2]

53[1]

1[2]

15(1]

15[3]

18[7]

Nasals

Fricatives

Stops

0 1 3

Total-

23

42

6[1] 2[7] 7

10

11[2] 8

4[1] 3[3] 12

128[14]

Figure 18: Statistics of attested causative pairs across the phonetic spectre in the Yi
Group

Nevertheless, the distribution shown in the figure seems to reveal some


interesting phonetic and sound-structural tendencies of the supposed
lexicalization process. The place and mode of articulation that comes down
with the highest number of lexicalized pairs is alveolar stops (a good portion
of them being alveolar affricated stops). Recalling from section 1 the putative
PTB prefix *s- which shares with those causative pairs the alveolar place of
articulation, it appears not far-fetched to proclaim that mainly verbs with an
alveolar initial consonant had been targeted by the unproductive lexicalization process. This would let the process look like an assimilative process13
and would considerably rebalance the account of previous authors who have
spoken of a blind sound change.
Further, I claim that the lexicalization of causative verbs is not only
driven by an assimilative ("aware" or "motivated") phonetic process, but by
13

The process would be assimilative in the sense that the majority of targeted verbs
share with the PTB prefix *s- the same place of articulation.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

176

Matthias Gerner

the semantics of the target simplex verbs as well. In figure 19,1 have counted
the number of causative pairs in Weining Neasu in dependence of the
semantic categories employed in section 4, i.e. the type of valency extension
(1 > 2 or 2 > 3), whether the main argument exerts control over the
situation and finally the Aktionsart of the situation referred to by the simplex
verb (state, process or action).
Valency Extension Control of S/A
Situation Type
Number of Pairs
l->2
State
[- control]
7
l->2
Process
24
[- control]
l->2
Action
[+ control]
3
2-^3
State
2
[- control]
2->3
State
[+ control]
3
Action
JL 7 J
[+
[ T control]
V^UIILIUIJ
-OX.UU11
3J
2-3
Figure 19: Statistics of attested causative pairs across the semantic spectre in
Weining Neasu

It is striking that intransitive verbs (34/42) have been the target of the
lexicalization process much more often than monotransitive verbs (8/42).
However, the more sensible parameter for the lexicalization process is [
control]. In Weining Neasu, 33 simplex verbs refer to uncontrolled situations
and 9 to controlled situations. Especially uncontrolled intransitive processes
(24/42) have been the target of the proposed lexicalization process. This basic
statistical distribution reflects a cross-linguistic tendency of (at least certain
forms of) causatives not to apply to verbs in an iterative fashion (cf. Dixon
2000: 59). As control verbs already express some kind of manipulation or
causation, it is not surprising that they had been less targeted by the
devoicing process. On the other hand, non-control verbs encode a low
degree of causation and the devoicing process more likely seeks out such
verbs as it reacts less to their inherent semantics. I claim therefore that the
semantic nature of the simplex verb has also been a decisive factor for the
selective process of devoicing these verbs have undergone.

5.2 Steps in the lexicalization process


It is thus proposed that the devoicing-causation correlation came historically
into existence in the Lolo-Burmese branch of TB languages through a
motivated process of lexicalization. The proposed steps in this lexicalization
process are as follows:
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group

177

Step 1 (morphological valency-increasing prefix *s- in PTB): As


numerous authors remarked (see section 1), there seems to have been the
transitive prefix *s- at the stage of PTB (cf. Benedict 1972: 105-108; Dai 2001;
Matisoff 1973: 32-34,1975,1976: 414-419). It appears that the language with
written documentation in which the PTB-causative prefix *s- is preserved
most faithfully is the Central TB language Dulong/Rawang (La Polla 2000).
The causative prefix is described in this language as a highly grammatical
bound morpheme without selectional restrictions. All intransitive and
monotransitive verbs may attach this valency-increasing productive prefix,
while only marginal verb categories such as applicative verbs, ditransitive
verbs and verbalized nominals disallow its preposing (La Polla 2000: 296).
Step 2 (mixed segmental and supra-segmental valency-increasing
strategies in Proto-Burmese-Lolo (PEL)): The branch of South-Eastern
Tibeto-Burman languages mainly consists of the Burmese-Lolo language
group (Bradley 1997: 38). In this branch of the TB family, large sound
changes seem to have occurred. In various BL languages, the original
causative prefix *s- has undergone sound changes when attached to certain
intransitive and monotransitive verbs. Matisoff (1973: 32) mentions
glottalization as a transition point between the segmental PTB-prefix *s- and
subsequent supra-segmental phenomena such as described in section 3 and 4
of this paper. While no synchronic data with the glottal causative prefix *?are available, there might be at least one Burmese-Lolo language which has
preserved this transitional hybrid picture: the Burmish language Zaiwa
spoken in China near the Myanmar border (see section 1). Dai (1981)
reports that in Zaiwa supra-segmental processes such as aspiration and tone
alteration contribute to the production of causative verbs, whilst the
existence of a causative prefix fa- is also attested. Both causativizing
strategies are compatible with different segments of the lexicon. The
important feature of these supra-segmental alterations is that they exhibit
selectional restrictions on the verbs that might undergo them. These suprasegmental alterations seem to have lost the productivity of the PTB prefix *swhich is typical for lexicalization. The split between verbs that derive
causative forms with a morphological prefix and those which form causative
versions through supra-segmental alterations has been arguably generated by
motivated lexicalization. At the onset, (intransitive and monotransitive)
verbs with a voiced initial consonant were targeted. The selection of simplexverbs with voiced initials has not been arbitrary, even though not every
voiced-initial verb has been selected. In the following chart there are mono-

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

178

Matthias Gerner

syllabic verbs from Weining Neasu, which have not been sought out by the
process of legalization: intransitive verbs with a voiceless initial and
mono/ditransitive verbs with a voiced initial.
Voiceless initial-low transitivity
t$hu55 'become bad' (intransitive)
<?e55
'drown' (intransitive)
te 2 1
'run' (intransitive)

Voiced initial-high transitivity


bi55
'give, bring' (ditransitive)
de33
'tap' (monotransitive)
dr55
'push' (monotransitive)
ga55
'cause' (mono-, ditransitive)
lr33
'shake' (monotransitive)
Figure 20: Voiceless intransitive and voiced mono/ditransitive verbs in Weining
Neasu

On the other hand, voiced-initial verbs are the only candidates that may
undergo the process of devoicing/causation as described in the previous
sections. During this process of lexicalization, predominantly non-control
verbs have been aimed at. For a few verbs, aspiration is a secondary process,
which goes along with the devoicing process.
Step 3 (supra-segmental processes as the only valency-increasing strategy
in Proto-Loloish (PL)): Burmese-Lolo languages consist of Burmish and of
Loloish languages (Bradley 1997: 38). Zaiwa as Burmish language (see step 2)
has a mix of segmental and supra-segmental means to derive causative verbs.
In languages of the Loloish subbranch, the causative prefix attested in
Burmish and other TB languages has completely disappeared and suprasegmental processes on a narrow range of verbs appear to be the sole vestiges
of this formerly productive process. The number of simplex/complex verbs
varies between one or two (Luoping Nase has, in addition, a number of
ambitransitive verbs) and 45 (Weining Neasu); all pairs belong to the section
of basic vocabulary.

6. Conclusion
The current contribution offers a new account for a phenomenon that was
otherwise recognized some 40 years ago (cf. Chen Shilin et al. 1962). It
reports with pan-Yi data on a lexicalization process in a portion of the
Tibeto-Burman family whereby a verb with voiced initial consonant derives
a causative form by devoicing the consonant. It demonstrates with statistical
figures that the phonetic and semantic changes these verbs underwent are
not arbitrary but motivated. This paper contrasts with earlier accounts,
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group

179

which treated this brand of lexicalization as a blind process of linguistic


change. Furthermore, previous papers were ambiguous on the type of
linguistic change these verb derivations refer to, i.e. whether it is
grammaticalization or lexicalization. In this paper I identify the changes as
lexicalization.
Appendix: Spatial-genetic information on the Yi group
In the linguistic classification literature, the Yi languages are included in the
larger Burmese-Lolo language group (inside the Tibeto-Burman languages).
Some Yi languages, especially in the Chuxiong and Dali prefectures of
Yunnan province, approach linguistically varieties of Lisu, which is another
nationality in China. In China there are 7.5 million ethnic Yi (2000 census)
distributed over Yunnan, Sichuan, Guizhou and Guangxi provinces. If
unintelligibility were a criterion to separate languages, then I would estimate
the total number of Yi languages to be 100-150. By unintelligibility I mean
substantial lack of comprehension that cannot be overcome by two months
of language learning. The 11 Yi languages surveyed geographically represent
all branches of the Yi group. The lexical data presented in this paper
originate from primary field research (conducted during 1996-2000), with
the exception of Weishan Lalo where I rely on a simplex-complex list in
BjorvenuTs grammar (Bjrverud 1998: 66).

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

180

Matthias Gerner

Language

Location

Number of

References

Speakers
Liangshan Nuosu China/Sichuan/Liangshan

2.2 Million

China/Yunnan/Lij iang/Ninglang
@ Weining Neasu

Chen &Wu (1998)

China/Guizhou/Bijie/Weining, Hezhang,
Nayong, Zhijin
China/Guizhou/Liupanshui/Shuicheng

Longlin Ngopho

Luoping Nase

Shizong Kopho

Fu ([1950] 1997)

China/Guangxi/Baise/Longlin;
China/G uizhou/Liupanshui/Pan

300,000600,000
10,000-40,000

China/Yunnan/Qujing/Luoping
China/Yunnan/Qujing/Luoping, Fuyuan,

100,000-

Shizong, Luliang, Qujing

300,000

China/Guizhou/Liupanshui/Pan
China/Yunnan/Qujing/Shizong, Luliang;

20,000-40,000

China/Yunnan/Honghe/Luxi, Me
China/Yunnan/Kunming/Shilin
Me Axi

China/Yunnan/Honghe/Me, Luxi

ca. 82,000

China/Yunnan/Wenshan/Qiubei
Me Azhee
Gejiu Nesu

Liotard
(1909,1911)

China/Yunnan/Honghe/Me
China/Yunnan/Honghe/Gejiu, Jinping,

ca. 60,000
ca. 370,000

Weishan Lalo

Jianshui, Lchun
China/Yunnan/Dali/Weishan,
Nanjian

565,000

Yongren Lolo
Wuding Aluphu

China/Yunnan/Chuxiong/Yongren, Dayao
China/Yunnan/Chuxiong/Wuding, Luquan

50,000-150,000
ca. 41,000

Bjrverud
(1998)

Figure 21: An overview of the Yi languages surveyed

The following map locates the Yi languages surveyed in this paper on a


map of Southwest China.

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 181

Distribution of the Yi languages investigated


Llangshan Nuosu

Laos

H Shizong Kopho $; Weishan Lalo

Weining Neasu

^=3. Mile Axl

-.... Yongren Lolo

Longlln Ngopho

= Mile Azhee

^JV Wuding Aluphu

Luoping Nase

44 Gejiu Nesu

Map 1: Geographical distribution of the Yi languages investigated

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

182

Matthias Gerner

List of abbreviations
IP PL
1PSG
2PSG
3PSG
ADV
ART
ARTiPROX
BEN
CL

cov

COV:point to
DUR
DEM
DEM:DIST
DEM:MED
DEM:PROX
DP
FIX
IMP
LOG
LOC:on
NEG
NUM
NUM:9
PASS
PAT
PREP
SEND:away
STP

First person plural


First person singular
Third person singular
Third person singular
Adversative modality particle
Article
Article: proximal distance to Speaker
Beneficiary
Classifier
Coverb
Coverb:with gloss
Durative Aspect
Demonstrative
Demonstrative: distal to Speaker
Demonstrative: medial to Speaker
Demonstrative: proximal to Speaker
Dynamic Perfect particle
FIX versatile
Imperative
Location particle
Location particle with gloss
Negation
Numeral
Numeral with its value
Passive postposition
Patient
Preparative phase particle
SEND versatile with English gloss
Stative Perfect

References
Anttila, Raimo (1989[1972]). Historical and Comparative Linguistics (Current Issues
in Linguistic Theory 6). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Bauer, Laurie (1978). On lexicalization (neither a lexicalist nor a transformationalist
be). Archivum Linguisticum 9, 3-14.
Benedict, P.K. (1972). Sino-Tibetan: A conspectus (James Matisoff, contributing
editor). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bjrverud, Susanna (1998). A grammar ofLalo. Ph.D. dissertation. Lund University.

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group 183


Bradley, David (1997). Tibeto-Burman languages and classification. In Papers in
Southeast Asian linguistics No. 14: Tibeto-Burman languages of the Himalayas.
Pacific Linguistics A-86, David Bradley (ed.) 1-72. Canberra: Australian National
University.
Brinton, Laurel and Elisabeth Gloss Traugott (2005). Lexicalization and Language
Change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chen Kang (1990). Yiyu zidongci yu shidongci de xingtai biaozhi jiqi youlai [On
morphological marking of simplex and causative verbs in the Yi language].
Minzu Yuwen (2), 40-45.
Chen, Kang and Wu Da (1998). Yiyu yufa [Yi grammar]. Beijing: Zhongyang Minzu
Daxue Chubanshe [Central University of Nationalities Press].
Chen Shilin, Bian Shiming, Li Xiuqing and Luohong Waku (1962). Liangshan Yiyu
de shidong fanchou [The category of causative in Liangshan Yi]. Zhongguo
Yuwen (8-9), 412-426.
Comrie, Bernard (1975). Causatives and universal grammar. Transaction of the
Philological Society (1974), 1-32.
Comrie, Bernard (1976). The syntax of causative constructions: Cross-language
similarities and divergencies, In Syntax and Semantics Volume 6: The grammar of
causative constructions, Masayoshi Shibatani (ed.), 261-312. New York:
Academic Press.
Dai Qingxia (1981). Zaiwayu shidong fanchou de xingtai bianhua [On the
morphology of the causative category in the Zaiwa language]. Minzu Yuwen (4),
36-41.
Dai Qingxia (2001). Zangmian yuzu yuyan shidong fanchou de lishi yanbian [The
historical development of the causative category in Tibeto-Burmese languages].
Journal of Chinese Linguistics 29(1), 1-10.
Dixon, Robert M.W. (1979). Ergativity. Language 55(1), 59-138.
Dixon, Robert M.W. (1994). Ergativity. Cambridge studies in linguistics 69.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dixon, Robert M.W. (2000). A typology of causatives: Form, syntax and meaning. In
Changing Valency: Case studies in transitivity, R.M.W. Dixon and Alexandra
Aikhenvald (eds.), 30-83. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dixon, Robert M.W. and Alexandra Aikhenvald (2000). Introduction. In Changing
Valency: Case studies in transitivity, R.M.W. Dixon and Alexandra Aikhenvald
(eds.), 1-29. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Du Ruoming (1990). Zangmianyu dongci shidong fanchou de lishi yanbian [On the
evolution of the causative category of Tibeto-Burman languages]. Yuyan Yanjiu
18(1), 133-148.
Fodor, J. A. (1970). Three reasons for not deriving 'kill' from 'cause to die'. Linguistic
Inquiry 1,429-438.
Frawley, W. (1992). Linguistic semantics. Hillsday, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

184

Matthias Gerner

Fu, Maoji (1997 [1950]). A descriptive grammar of Lolo. (Ph. D. Thesis August 1950,
Cambridge University.) Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area, 20(1), 1-227.
Hopper, Paul (1990). Where do words come from? In Studies in Typology and
Diachrony: Papers Presented to Jospeh H. Greenberg on his 75th Birthday

(Typological Studies in Language 20), William Croft, Keith Denning and


Suzanne Kemmer (eds.), 151-160. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
JackendofF, Ray (1997). The Architecture of the Language Faculty (Linguistic Inquiry
Monograph 28). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Jackendoff, Ray (2002). Foundation of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar,
Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
La Polla, Randy (2000). Valency-changing derivations in Dulong/Rawang. In
Changing Valency: Case studies in transitivity, R.M.W. Dixon and Alexandra
Aikhenvald (eds.), 282-311. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lietard, A. (1909). Notions de grammaire Lolo (dialecte A-hi). Bulletin de VEcole
Francaise d'Extreme Orient 9(2), 285-314.
Lietard, A. (1911). Notions de grammaire Lolo, dialecte A-hi. ToungPao 13,1-42.
Matisoff, James (1969). Verb concatenation in Lahu: The syntax and semantics of
'simple' juxtaposition. Acta Linguistica Hafniensia (International Journal of
Structural Linguistics K0benhavn) 12(1), 69-120.
Matisoff, James (1973). The grammar of Lahu. Publications in Linguistics 75.
Berkeley: University of California.
Matisoff, James (1975). A new Lahu simplex/causative pair: "study/train". Linguistics
of the Tibeto-Burman Area 2(1), 151-154.
Matisoff, James (1976). Lahu causative constructions: Case hierarchies and the
morphology/syntax cycle in a Tibeto-Burman perspective, In Syntax and
Semantics Volume 6: The grammar of causative constructions, Masayoshi
Shibatani (ed.), 413-442. New York: Academic Press.
Matisoff, James (1994). Tone, intonation, and sound symbolism in Lahu: loading the
syllable canon, In Sound symbolism, Leanne Hinton, Johanna Nichols and John J.
Ohala (eds.), 115-129. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
McCawley, James (1968). Lexical insertion in a transformational grammar without
deep structure. In Papers from the 4th Regional Meeting, Bill J. Darden, CharlesJames N. Bailey and Alice Davison (eds.), 71-80. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic
Society.
Palmer, F. (1994). Grammatical roles and relations. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Shibatani, Masayoshi (1976). The grammar of causative constructions: A conspectus,
In Syntax and Semantics Volume 6: The grammar of causative constructions,
Masayoshi Shibatani (ed.), 1-40. New York: Academic Press.
Song, Jae Jung (1989). A universal typology of causative constructions: Functional
and diachronic perspectives. Ph.D. dissertation. Melbourne: Monash University.

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

The lexicalization of causative verbs in the Yi group

185

Song, Jae Jung (1990). On the rise of causative affixes: a universal-typological


perspective. Lingua 82,151-200.
Song, Jae Jung (1996). Causatives and causation. London: Longman.
Sun Hongkai (1999). On the category of causative verbs in Tibeto-Burman
languages. Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area 22(1), 183-199.
van der Auwera, Johan (2002). More thoughts on degrammaticalization. In New
Reflections on Grammaticalization - Proceedings from the International
Symposium on Grammaticalization 17-19 June 1999, Potsdam Germany
(Typological Studies in Language 49), Ilse Wischer and Gabriele Diewald (eds.),
19-29. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Xu Xijian (1984). Jingpoyu de shidong fanchou [The causative category of the
Jingphaw language]. Minzu Yuwen (1), 40-45.
Author's address
Matthias Gerner
Department of Chinese, Translation & Linguistics
City University of Hong Kong
83 Tat Chee Avenue
Kowloon, Hong Kong
e-mail: mgerner@cityu.edu.hk

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

Brought to you by | University of Sussex


Authenticated | 172.16.1.226
Download Date | 8/7/12 7:38 PM

You might also like