You are on page 1of 15

Journal of Marketing Communications

Vol. 11, No. 3, 215228, September 2005

The Effects of Locus of Control on


Word-of-mouth Communication
DESMOND LAM* & DICK MIZERSKI**
*Faculty of Business Administration, University of Macau, Taipa, Macau, China, **Department of
Information Management and Marketing, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia

ABSTRACT Word-of-mouth can be a powerful tool for and against marketing a brand. The
effect of personality can have a significant effect on an individuals word-of-mouth behaviour.
One of the most popular personality constructs is the locus of control. This research studied the
influence of the locus of control on consumer word-of-mouth communications. The results showed
that individuals who scored high on their internal locus of control were more likely to engage in
word-of-mouth communication with their out-groups. In addition, individuals who scored high on
their external locus of control were more likely to engage in word-of-mouth communication with
their in-group. Out-groups are defined as people with a weaker ties relationship, while in-groups
are defined as people with a stronger ties relationship (i.e. close friends and family). These
findings would help marketers in directing their promotional programmes more effectively.
KEY WORDS: Word-of-mouth, locus of control, in-group, out-group

Introduction
There has been a considerable amount of documentation (see Katz and Lazarfeld,
1955) on the power of word-of-mouth since the 1960s. Word-of-mouth is generally
more credible than any salesperson and has the advantage of rapid diffusion and a
broad reach. It has been widely reported to be many times more influential than
information from newspapers and magazines, personal selling and radio advertising
(Katz and Lazarfeld, 1955; Herr et al., 1991). Despite the importance and influence
of word-of-mouth it has remained one of the most neglected marketing areas
(Silverman, 2001). In fact many companies are still struggling to develop effective
marketing programmes that encourage consumer word-of-mouth communication
(Gremler et al., 2001). According to Mangold et al. (1999) only a small percentage of
word-of-mouth communications were stimulated by active corporate promotional
efforts. To date relatively few companies have tried to harness the full potential of
Correspondence Address: Desmond Lam, Faculty of Business Administration, University of Macau,
Av. Padre Tomas Pereira S. J., Taipa, China. Fax: +853 838320; Tel.: +853 397 4880. Email:
DesmondL@umac.mo
1352-7266 Print/1466-4445 Online/05/03021514 # 2005 Taylor & Francis Group Ltd
DOI: 10.1080/1352726042000333180

216

D. Lam & D. Mizerski

the power of word-of-mouth communication (Buttle, 1998). It is a common belief


among many companies that consumer word-of-mouth communication is uncontrollable (Wilson, 1994; Lovelock, 2001) and that it is sufficient to stimulate positive
word-of-mouth behaviour simply by positive product experiences (Gremler et al.,
2001). This study refutes these misconceptions. The authors of this current study
believe that a sound understanding of the factors influencing word-of-mouth
communication may help to create more proactive and targeted promotional
programmes towards stimulating consumer word-of-mouth communication.
Generally, the reasons why an individual engages in word-of-mouth communication have been extensively researched (e.g. Arndt, 1967; Brown and Reingen, 1987;
Wilson and Peterson, 1989; Bansal and Voyer, 2000; Ennew et al., 2000). Previous
research has found that consumers engage in word-of-mouth communication for
altruistic motives, anxiety reduction, advice seeking, product involvement and selfenhancement reasons (Sundram et al., 1998). Word-of-mouth communication
among consumers is also affected by other external factors, such as incentives,
social network structures, business climates, cultures and individual personalities
(Buttle, 1998). The influence of individual personality in particular is often cited as a
very important factor.
One of the most intensively and consistently studied individual personality
constructs is the concept of locus of control (see Matsumoto, 2000; Hoffman et al.,
2003). The locus of control construct captures individuals general and daily
expectancies about the causes of their reward and punishment (Rotter, 1966).
Original research into the construct discovered two basic dimensions, namely
internal and external. Individuals with a high internal locus of control generally
believe they are in control of their lives and events affecting their lives, while those
with a high external locus of control see the outcomes of events as being due to
uncontrollable external variables such as luck, fate and powerful others. This study
will explore whether individuals locus of control can affect their word-of-mouth
communications with others. An important part of this study is to investigate
individuals word-of-mouth communications with their in-groups and out-groups.
In-group word-of-mouth communication is communication between people who
share a close relationship or strong ties such as between close friends and family,
whereas out-group word-of-mouth communication is communication between
people with weaker ties such as people other than friends and family (Matsumoto,
2000). Ultimately, it is the intention of this study to help companies in identifying
communities, markets or societies that may be more receptive to word-of-mouth
marketing. The following sections will examine each construct in greater detail and
then develop and test research hypotheses.
Word-of-mouth Communications
The research literature on word-of-mouth communications largely began after the
Second World War (e.g. Katz and Lazarsfeld, 1955;Arndt, 1967). Previous research
on word-of-mouth communications has primarily focused on the antecedents and
consequences of communication, in particular on negative information (Wilson and
Peterson, 1989; Mangold et al., 1999). This study will focus on both negative
and positive information about products or brands.

The Effects of Locus of Control on Word-of-mouth Communication

217

Generally, word-of-mouth communication can be defined as an oral, person-toperson communication between a receiver and a communicator whom the receiver
perceives as non-commercial regarding a brand, product or service (Arndt, 1967). It
is a group phenomenon, an exchange of thoughts or ideas among two or more
individuals (Bone, 1992). Word-of-mouth activity has been shown to influence
a variety of buyer conditions, including awareness, expectations, perceptions,
attitudes, behavioural intentions and behaviours (Reingen, 1987). According to a
study conducted by Herr et al. (1991), face-to-face word-of-mouth communication
was much more persuasive than printed advertising. It is the most important source
of influence in the purchase of household goods and food products (Katz and
Lazarsfeld, 1955). Because consumers generally cannot process all of the information that is available to them for purchase decisions, they often engage in simple
guides for simplifying their information-seeking and decision-making processes.
Word-of-mouth communication helps to reduce the amount of information that
must be processed in order to make a decision (Duhan et al., 1997).
Sundram et al. (1998) found that consumers engaged in word-of-mouth communications for altruistic, product involvement and self-enhancement reasons. For example, a
consumer may make a product recommendation to a friend out of goodwill and a desire
to help or because of their positive product consumption experience with the product.
On the other hand, he or she may also complain (negative word-of-mouth) to other
consumers if he or she is dissatisfied with their consumption experience with a product
or company. Others may engage in positive word-of-mouth communications in order to
show their expertise in a certain product area such as computers and fashion or negative
word-of-mouth communications in order to project their social status and power.
Gatignon and Robertson (1986) cited decision support, decision justification, social
status and social power as the main factors that motivated word-of-mouth
communications. The need for information and a relief of decision anxiety motivated
word-of-mouth seeking. Another study performed by Mangold et al. (1999) found the
three key factors most likely to stimulate word-of-mouth communications were a
strongly felt need on the part of the word-of-mouth receiver, coincidental communication between word-of-mouth communicator and receiver relating to a broader subject
and a high level of satisfaction or dissatisfaction with the product on the part of
the word-of-mouth communicator. This study attempts to investigate individuals
general word-of-mouth communication, which may include both positive and negative information about products or brands and examine it in the context of both
word-of-mouth giving and seeking.
The frequency and intensity of word-of-mouth communications may also depend
on the situation, type of products and markets, social network, individual
personality, availability of a physical infrastructure and culture of individuals
(Buttle, 1998). In terms of social network consumers in general interact with people
that are associated with them with varying degrees of tie strength, ranging
from strong (e.g. family and close friends or in-groups) to weak (e.g. acquaintances
or out-groups). According to Triandis (1995), in-groups are
groups of individuals about whose welfare a person is concerned, with whom
that person is willing to cooperate without demanding equitable returns, and
separation from whom leads to anxiety (p. XX).

218

D. Lam & D. Mizerski

In-group relationships are characterized by a good degree of belongingness,


familiarity, intimacy and, very importantly, trust (Watkins and Liu, 1996;
Matsumoto, 2000). These characteristics create strong ties between members of ingroups. On the other hand, out-group relationships lack the above characteristics,
thereby leading to weaker ties between its members (Matsumoto, 2000). Previous
research has shown that strong ties are more likely to be activated for information
flow than weak ties (Reingen and Kernan, 1986; Brown and Reingen, 1987).
Moreover, the amount of word-of-mouth communication generated is generally
higher within groups with many strong tie relations than within groups with many
weak tie relations (Bone, 1992). These studies seem to suggest that individuals in
general engage in more word-of-mouth communication with their strong ties (i.e. ingroups) than with their weak ties (i.e. out-groups).
While the influence of social networks on word-of-mouth communications has
been investigated in much detail in previous studies, relatively little research (e.g.
Rubin and Rubin, 1989) has examined the influence of individuals locus of control
on the subject area. This study hopes to take the issue a step further by examining the
influence of individuals locus of control on their product or brand word-of-mouth
communication with both their in-groups and out-groups. The next section will
review the construct of locus of control and develop the research hypotheses in this
study.
The Locus of Control and Research Hypotheses
The locus of control is an important construct describing individual differences. It is
one of the most widely studied personality concepts (Matsumoto, 2000) and has
often been used for predicting employees behaviour (Spector, 1988; Spector et al.,
2002) in organizations. The locus of control can be defined as
the degree to which the individual perceives that the reward (obtained)
follows from or is contingent upon his own behavior or attributes (Rotter,
1966).
First operationalized by Rotter (1954, 1966) and with many subsequent studies
adopting its use (Lefcourt, 1981), the construct was originally conceptualized as unidimensional, with the internal and external loci of control on either end of its axis. In
fact, previous research has found the internal and external loci of control to be
mutually exclusive (Rotter, 1966). Since its initial development Rotters (1966) locus
of control scale has undergone several changes. For example, Levenson (1974)
developed a multi-dimensional scale as an alternative to Rotters (1966) scale. This
scale, which is now widely accepted as an alternative, includes three dimensions:
internal, powerful others and chance. Generally, people differ in terms of the amount
of control they believe they have over their own behaviour and environment
(Lefcourt, 1966; Rotter, 1966; Levenson, 1974). Those with a high internal locus of
control or internals believe they have control over their own behaviour and
environment. These people believe they have considerable influence over the
outcomes in their lives. Those with a high external locus of control or externals

The Effects of Locus of Control on Word-of-mouth Communication

219

believe they are dominated by external forces such as fate, luck or powerful others,
factors that are beyond their control.
Demographically there are some differences between internals and externals as
documented by previous studies. According to previous research internals tended to
be more educated (Lachman and Leff, 1989) and have higher household incomes
(Rotter, 1969; Hoffman et al., 2003) than externals. In addition, men and those in
senior positions were found to be more internal than females and those in junior
positions, respectively (Smith et al., 1997).
Hoffman et al. (2003) stated that internals are more action oriented than externals.
In line with this statement, the research of Brockhaus (1975) found internals to be
more oriented towards activities and more likely to possess entrepreneurial qualities
such as risk taking. Internals tend to initiate new activities and undertake efforts or
actions in order to manage events around them actively and, hence, are more action
oriented. At work internals often perform beyond their basic job requirements such
as by actively taking initiatives for controlling outcomes (Spector, 1982; Blau, 1993).
Internals are likely to take up more risk in businesses (Miller et al., 1982; Howell and
Avolio, 1993) and engage in a greater degree of information search than externals
(Srinivasan and Tikoo, 1992), which they use for making their decisions (Lefcourt,
1982). Internals risk-taking aptitude in businesses and corporate settings generally
reflect their beliefs in controlling the outcomes of their lives and events around them.
This risk-taking nature of the internals in controlling outcomes can affect how they
communicate with others. Generally, communication among people often involves
taking some risks such as the risk of information transmitted uncontrollably and the
social risk of a negative remark made by the sender. Given the activity-oriented and
risk-taking characteristics of internals, one may naturally expect internals to seek out
and engage actively in word-of-mouth communication with people around them.
This behaviour is in essence manifested by internals desire to take initiatives in
controlling their lives despite the possibility of any associated risks. Therefore, it
seems unlikely that internals will engage solely in active communication with their ingroups. Based on their nature and craving for information, they may be more likely
to engage in communication with their out-groups than the externals. Hence, the
following hypotheses were formulated for this study.
H 1:

Individuals who score high on their internal locus of control are more likely
to engage in word-of-mouth communications with their out-groups compared to individuals who score low on their internal locus of control.

H 2:

Individuals who score high on their internal locus of control are less likely to
engage in word-of-mouth communications with their in-groups compared
to individuals who score low on their internal locus of control.

Externals, on the other hand, often engage in avoidance behaviour (Janssen and
Carton, 1999) such as withdrawal (Storms and Spector, 1987). According to Steinfatt
(1987) externals also have greater needs for affiliation. As such they are more likely
to engage in companionship and entertainment behaviour compared to internals
(Flaherty et al., 1998). Externals often feel a lack of personal control and believe

220

D. Lam & D. Mizerski

their actions do not necessarily lead to their desired results. As such they are likely to
fall back on their in-group members, who provide a sense of safe companionship
and certainty. Due to their avoidance attitudes and needs for relationship, externals
may be more likely to engage in word-of-mouth communication with their ingroups. At the same time, aggravated by their risk-avoiding nature, externals would
feel uncomfortable with the unfamiliar and unknown associated with their out-group
members. Thus, they may also be less likely to engage in word-of-mouth
communication with their out-groups. Hence, the following two hypotheses were
formulated.
H3: Individuals who score high on their external locus of control are more
likely to engage in word-of-mouth communications with their in-groups
compared to individuals who score low on their external locus of control.

H4: Individuals who score high on their external locus of control are less likely to
engage in word-of-mouth communications with their out-groups compared
to individuals who score low on their external locus of control.

Research Methodology
Sample and Data Collection
In order to test the hypotheses, a set of 200 questionnaires was distributed to a
convenience sample consisting of business undergraduates at a university in Perth,
Australia. Incomplete data reduced this sample to 197. The age of the respondents
ranged from 18 to 34 years old, with a median age of 21 years. Approximately 20%
of the respondents were working adults studying their first degree and 43% of the
respondents were male.
Questionnaire
Altogether there were 20 items in this study. The word-of-mouth communication
construct was operationalized using eight items, with four items each for the ingroup and out-group word-of-mouth communications, respectively. For exploratory
purposes the original items comprised two items each for word-of-mouth seeking
and giving, each with an attitudinal and a behavioural component. These items were
pre-tested for validity during the preliminary study on a focus group of 15
individuals (mostly undergraduates and postgraduates) prior to the main survey. A
few of the items were reworded for clarity through feedback. Very importantly, the
focus group participants did not find any differences between the seeking, giving,
attitude and behaviour items beyond the in-group/out-group partition. As such the
original items were retained for the main survey.
Nine items on the locus of control were chosen from Levensons (1974) original
scale. Three of these items measured the internal locus of control, while the others
measured the external (i.e. powerful others and chance) locus of control (see the

The Effects of Locus of Control on Word-of-mouth Communication

221

Appendix). All of the above items were measured on a five-point Likert scale ranging
from 1 representing strongly disagree to 5 representing strongly agree. Three
additional items were questions about age, gender, nationality and the number of
years spent in Australia.
Data Analysis and Results
A factor analysis with principal components extraction and varimax rotation was
performed on the eight items that were used for measuring word-of-mouth
communications. The process only yielded two factors successfully: in-group
word-of-mouth communication and out-group word-of-mouth communication.
This result was in accordance with the focus group findings, where the participants
did not find significant differences between the various word-of-mouth items beyond
the in-group/out-group partition. In essence, the respondents to the survey did not
find significant differences in items for either category (i.e. in-group word-of-mouth
communication and out-group word-of-mouth communication).
Another factor analysis on the nine items for the locus of control discovered two
factors instead of the expected three dimensions: internal, powerful others and
chance (Levenson, 1974). These factors were renamed the internal locus of control
and the external locus of control. An item reliability test was also conducted, yielding
satisfactory Cronbachs a coefficient values for all factors (see Table 1). According to
Nunnally (1967), reliabilities in the range of 0.50.6 are satisfactory in the early
stages of research. Hence, the coefficients obtained were deemed sufficient given the
exploratory nature of this study. Finally, all variables were created by weighted
summation based upon their respective rotated factor loading scores. The results of
the factor and reliability analysis are shown in Table 1 along with their items.
A preliminary analysis of the sample using paired-sample t-tests found that the
mean internal locus of control score (M53.74) of the respondents was significantly
(t517.94, df5193 and p,0.001) higher than their mean external locus of control
score (M52.44). This was consistent with previous research on higher education
students, who tended to have higher internal locus of control scores (Rotter, 1966).
Moreover, there was a significant negative correlation (R520.208 and p,0.01)
between the internal locus of control and the external locus of control.
Next, a multi-regression analysis was conducted with in-group and out-group
word-of-mouth communications as the dependent variables and the internal as well
as external loci of control as the predictors. Referring to Table 2, the results showed a
significant relationship between the internal locus of control and out-group word-ofmouth communications (p,0.001). At the same time, the external locus of control
appeared to predict the in-group word-of-mouth communications (p,0.001). Given
the findings, is it possible that simple demographics could explain the results more
readily than the locus of control variables? To test for the effects of demographics,
age and number of years in Australia were added as predicted variables in a second
multi-regression model (see Table 2). The results showed that, despite adding these
two variables, the overall predictive abilities of the locus of control variables
remained strong (p,0.001).
The results in Table 3 showed that the higher the internal locus of control,
the higher the out-group word-of-mouth communications (R250.280). Contrary to

222

Table 1. Factor and reliability tests results

Word-of-mouth
In-group

Out-group

Cronbachs a

Items

0.697

I like introducing new brands and products only to my close friends


or family
I only provide information about new brands and products to my
close friends or family
I like to seek advice or information only from my close friends or
family when making purchase decision
I only gather information about a product before I buy from my
close friends or family
I like to provide people other than my close friends or family with
information about new brands or products
I share information about new brands and products with people
other than my close friends or family
I seek out the advice of people other than my close friends or family
regarding which brand to buy
I like to seek information and advice of people other than my close
friends or family before making a purchase decision

0.699

Locus of control
Internal locus of control

0.612

External locus of control

0.717

My life is determined by my own actions


When I get what I want it is usually because I worked hard for it
I can pretty much determine what will happen in my life
To a great extent my life is controlled by accidental happenings
When I get what I want it is usually because I am lucky
It is not always wise for me to plan too far ahead because many
things turn out to be a matter of good or bad luck
I feel like what happens in my life is mostly determined by powerful
people
My life is chiefly controlled by powerful others

Rotated factor
loading

Eigenvalue

0.667

2.199

0.726

2.199

0.707

2.199

0.737

2.199

0.779

2.132

0.775

2.132

0.643

2.132

0.650

2.132

0.630
0.781
0.481
0.646
0.561
0.484

2.356
2.356
2.356
1.518
1.518
1.518

0.798

1.518

0.761

1.518

D. Lam & D. Mizerski

Variables

The Effects of Locus of Control on Word-of-mouth Communication

223

Table 2. The effects of locus of control on word-of-mouth dependent variables


Dependent variable
Predictor variable
Internal locus of control
External locus of control
Model adjusted R2

In-group word-of-mouth

Out-group word-of-mouth

0.054
0.374**
0.125

0.259**
20.019
0.060

Standardized regression coefficients are shown: *p,0.05, **p,0.001.

Table 3. The effects of locus of control and other predictors on word-of-mouth dependent
variables
Dependent variable
Predictor variable
Internal locus of control
External locus of control
Age
Years in Australia
Model adjusted R2

In-group word-of-mouth

Out-group word-of-mouth

0.082
0.352**
20.092
20.172*
0.135

0.280**
0.000
20.147
20.078
0.065

Standardized regression coefficients are shown: *p,0.05, **p,0.001.

expectation, the relationship between the internal locus of control and in-group
word-of-mouth communications was positive, although the strength was not
statistically significant (p.0.05). Hence, hypothesis 1 is supported but hypothesis
2 is not. In addition, the study found that the higher the external locus of control, the
higher the in-group word-of-mouth communications (R250.352 and p,0.001).
However, there was no significant relationship between the external locus of control
and out-group word-of-mouth communications (p.0.05). Hence, hypothesis 3 is
supported but hypothesis 4 is not. Interestingly, the number of years spent in
Australia had an effect on the in-group word-of-mouth communications. Generally,
the shorter the duration of stay in Australia, the higher the in-group word-of-mouth
communications. Perhaps uncertainty associated with being in a less familiar
environment (i.e. measured by the number of years spent) promoted or encouraged
more in-group communication and sharing.
Discussion and Implications
The major finding in this study was that word-of-mouth communication was
influenced by an individuals locus of control. The study found that individuals with
a high internal locus of control were more likely to engage in word-of-mouth
communications with their out-groups. Conversely, individuals with a high external
locus of control were more likely to engage in word-of-mouth communications with

224

D. Lam & D. Mizerski

their in-groups. Previous research has found that externals are more risk averse and
desire companionship, while internals are more action oriented and engage in more
information search. These characteristics of internals and externals appeared to have
a significant influence on the individuals (i.e. in-group versus out-group) with which
they shared product or brand information.
The findings are interesting and insightful. The results from this study have
enhanced our existing knowledge of consumer behaviour. It provides valid support
for the use of the locus of control as the means for market segmentation. As
mentioned earlier, previous research on the locus of control has found externals to be
less educated, have a low income, tend to be women and hold lower corporate
positions, whereas internals, on the other hand, have a higher income, are more
educated, tend to be men and hold higher corporate positions. Inferring from the
current findings, the former groups of individuals (i.e. externals) may be more likely
to engage in product or brand word-of-mouth communications with their close
friends and families (i.e. in-groups) while the latter groups of individuals (i.e.
internals) may be more likely to engage in communication with people other
than their in-groups (i.e. out-groups). The knowledge of the locus of control
characteristics of these groups of individuals may become highly valuable for
companies, potentially enabling alternative promotional strategies. For example,
when directly targeting less educated or lower income consumers, marketers may
find a new product diffusion process slower and less effective because these groups of
consumers tend not to engage in product word-of-mouth communication beyond
their in-groups. As such markets may have to choose to create awareness of their
new products by promoting through the families of these market segments. On the
other hand, when targeting more educated or higher income consumers, less
promotional efforts may be needed in order to reach the desired awareness since
their groups of consumers tend to engage in substantial word-of-mouth communication beyond their in-groups. For companies who can successfully identify,
estimate or measure the locus of control characteristics of their target markets,
targeted word-of-mouth marketing can be incorporated into their marketing
campaign as a highly viable promotion tool.
Both word-of-mouth communications and mass media influence the diffusion of
innovation. Knowing the potential impacts of the locus of control may also enable
multinational companies to understand their global markets better and take that into
account when planning their marketing strategies. Quite interestingly, a number of
research studies on the locus of control have found that Americans often score
higher on their internal locus of control, whereas non-Americans such as Asians
tended to score higher on their external locus of control (Chiu, 1986; Lee and
Dengerink, 1992). Ralston and Gustafson (1993) conducted a study on the
differences in managerial values between subjects from the USA, Hong Kong and
the Peoples Republic of China. They found that the subjects from the Peoples
Republic of China and Hong Kong scored higher on their external locus of control
than the USA subjects. Since the external locus of control relates positively to ingroup word-of-mouth communication, one may then infer that in-group word-ofmouth communication may be more proliferate in Asian societies than in Western
societies. Past research has consistently found that personal sources play an
influential role in affecting purchases. Opinion leaders in particular are often

The Effects of Locus of Control on Word-of-mouth Communication

225

believed to be highly influential and help generate word-of-mouth communications


(Dawar et al., 1996). This has led many marketers to focus their promotional efforts
on opinion leaders in a bid to encourage product information diffusion. The results
from this study and research on cross-cultural differences in the locus of control may
yield some insights for international marketers. For marketers targeting the Asian
markets such as the Peoples Republic of China additional promotional efforts at the
families and friends of opinion leaders may greatly enhance the overall effectiveness
of product information diffusion and, ultimately, their marketing campaign.
Limitations and Future Research
One of the major limitations in this study was the use of a student sample. Nevertheless,
the selected sample frame provided a pool of individuals who shared a common lifecycle background for the testing of the research hypotheses, hence resulting in better
validity. Future research should attempt to replicate the same study by expanding the
sample frame and sizes for better generalization. In addition, the current research only
focused on one of the many personality constructs and did not examine other external
factors that may potentially affect word-of-mouth communication. Consumers cultural
values in particular may influence word-of-mouth behaviour (Dawar et al., 1996; Buttle,
1998). In this case, the personality effects on individuals word-of-mouth communication could be moderated by their cultural values. Future research should investigate
these areas for a better and more complete understanding of the subject area.
Conclusion
Word-of-mouth communication is indeed a major force in the marketplace that
cannot be taken for granted. While many studies have investigated its antecedents
and consequences, few have actually examined the influence of the locus of control
on word-of-mouth communication. This study found that individuals with a high
internal locus of control were more likely to engage in word-of-mouth communication with their out-groups. At the same time, those with a high external locus of
control were more likely to engage in word-of-mouth communication with their ingroups. The findings will help marketers in better identifying and promoting
segments within their markets that may be more receptive to word-of-mouth
marketing.
References
Arndt, J. (1967) Role of product-related conversations in the diffusion of a new product, Journal of
Marketing Research, 4, pp. 291295.
Bansal, H. S. & Voyer, P. A. (2000) Word-of-mouth processes within a services purchase decision context,
Journal of Service Research, 3(2), pp. 166177.
Blau, G. (1993) Testing the relationship of locus of control to different performance dimensions, Journal of
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 66(2), pp. 125138.
Bone, P. F. (1992) Determinants of word-of-mouth communications during product consumption,
Advances in Consumer Research, 19, pp. 579583.
Brown, J. J. & Reingen, P. H. (1987) Social ties and word-of-mouth referral behavior, Journal of Consumer
Research, 14, pp. 350362.

226

D. Lam & D. Mizerski

Buttle, F. A. (1998) Word of mouth: understanding and managing referral marketing, Journal of Strategic
Marketing, 6, pp. 241254.
Chiu, L. H. (1986) Locus of control in intellectual situations in American and Chinese school children,
International Journal of Psychology, 21, pp. 167176.
Dawar, N., Parker, P. M. & Price, L. J. (1996) A cross-cultural study of interpersonal information
exchange, Journal of International Business Studies, 27(3), pp. 497516.
Duhan, D. F., Johnson, S. D., Wilcox, J. B. & Harrell, G. D. (1997) Influence of consumer use of word-ofmouth recommendation sources, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 25, pp. 283295.
Ennew, C. T., Banerjee, A. K. & Li, D. (2000) Managing word of mouth communication: empirical
evidence from India, The International Journal of Bank Marketing, 18(2), pp. 7583.
Flaherty, L. M., Pearce, K. J. & Rubin, R. B. (1998) Internet and face-to-face communication: not
functional alternatives, Communication Quarterly, 46, pp. 250268.
Gatignon, H. & Robertson, T. S. (1987) An exchange theory model of interpersonal communication,
Advances in Consumer Research, 13(1), pp. 534538.
Gremler, D. D., Gwinner, K. P. & Brown, S. W. (2001) Generating positive word-of-mouth
communication through customeremployee relationships, International Journal of Service Industry
Management, 12(1), pp. 4159.
Herr, P. M., Kardes, F. R. & Kim, J. (1991) Effects of word-of-mouth and product information on
persuasion: an accessibilitydiagnosticity perspective, Journal of Consumer Research, 17, pp. 454462.
Hoffman, D. L., Novak, T. P. & Schlosser, A. E. (2003) Locus of control, web use, and consumer attitudes
toward Internet regulation, Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 22(1), pp. 4157.
Howell, J. M. & Avolio, B. J. (1993) Transformational leadership, transactional leadership, locus of
control, and support for innovation: key predictors of consolidated-business-unit performance,
Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, pp. 891902.
Janssen, T. & Carton, J. S. (1999) The effects of locus of control and task difficulty in on procrastination,
Journal of Genetic Psychology, 160, pp. 436442.
Katz, E. & Lazarfeld, P. F. (1955) Personal Influence (Glencoe, IL: Free Press).
Lachman, M. E. & Leff, R. (1989) Perceived control and intellectual functioning in the elderly: a 5-year
longitudinal study, Developmental Psychology, 25, pp. 722728.
Lee, V. K. & Dengerink, H. A. (1992) Locus of control in relation to sex and nationality: a cross-cultural
study, Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 23, pp. 288397.
Lefcourt, H. M. (1966) Internal versus external control of reinforcement: a review, Psychological Bulletin,
64(4), pp. 206220.
Lefcourt, H. M. (1981) The construction and development of the multidimensional/multiattributional
casuality scales, in: H. M. Lefcourt (Ed.) Research with the Locus of Control Construct, pp. 245277
(New York: Academic Press).
Lefcourt, H. M. (1982) Locus of Control: Current Trends in Theory and Research (NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates).
Levenson, H. (1974) Activism and powerful others: distinctions within the concept of internalexternal
control, Journal of Personality Assessment, 38, pp. 377383.
Lovelock, C. H. (2001) Service Marketing: People, Technology, Strategy, 4th edn (Upper Saddle River,
NJ: Prentice Hall).
Mangold, W. G., Miller, F. & Brockway, G. (1999) Word-of-mouth communication in the service
marketplace, The Journal of Services Marketing, 13(1), pp. 7383.
Matsumoto, D. (2000) Culture and Psychology: People Around the World (USA: Wadsworth).
Money, R. B. & Gilly, M. C. (1998) Explorations of national culture and word-of-mouth referral behavior
in the purchase of industrial services in the United States and Japan, Journal of Marketing, 62(4), pp.
7687.
Nunnally, J. C. (1967) Psychometric Theory (New York: McGraw-Hill).
Ralston, D. A. & Gustafson, D. J. (1993) Differences in managerial values: a study of U.S., Hong Kong
and PRC managers, Journal of International Business Studies, 24(2), pp. 249276.
Reingen, P. H. (1987) A word-of-mouth network, Advances in Consumer Research, 14(1), pp. 213217.
Reingen, P. H. & Kernan, J. B. (1986) Analysis of referral networks in marketing: methods and
illustration, Journal of Marketing Research, 23, pp. 370378.
Rotter, J. B. (1954) Social Learning and Clinical Psychology (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall).

The Effects of Locus of Control on Word-of-mouth Communication

227

Rotter, J. B. (1966) Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement,
Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80(1), pp. 128.
Rubin, A. M. & Rubin, R. B. (1989) Antecedents of interpersonal communication motivation,
Communication Quarterly, 40(3), pp. 305317.
Ryff, C. D. (1989) Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological wellbeing, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, pp. 10691081.
Silverman, G. (2001) The power of word of mouth, Direct Marketing, 64, pp. 4752.
Smith, P. B., Dugan, S. & Trompenaars, F. (1997) Locus of control and affectivity of gender and
occupational status: a 14 nation study, Sex Roles, 36(1/2), pp. 5177.
Spector, P. E. (1988) Development of the work locus of control scale, Journal of Occupational Psychology,
61(4), pp. 335340.
Spector, P. E., Cooper, C. L., Sanchez, J. I., ODriscoll, M., Sparks, K., Bernin, P., Bossing, A., Dewe, P.,
Hart, P., Lu, L., Miller, K., De Moraes, L. R., Ostrognay, G. M., Pagon, M., Pitariu, H. D.,
Poelmans, S. A. Y., Radhakrishnan, P., Russinova, V., Salamatov, V. & Salgado, J. F. (2002) Locus
of control and well-being at work: how generalizable are Western findings?, Academy of Management
Journal, 45(2), pp. 453466.
Srinivasan, N. & Tikoom, S. (1992) Effect of locus of control on information search behavior, Advances in
Consumer Behavior, 19, pp. 498504.
Steinfatt, T. M. (1987) Personality and communication: classical approaches, in: J. C. McCroskey & J. A.
Daly (Eds) Personality and Interpersonal Communication, pp. 42126 (Newbury Park, CA: Sage).
Storms, P. L. & Spector, P. E. (1987) Relationships of organizational frustration with reported
behavioural reactions: the moderating effect of locus of control, Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology, 60(3), pp. 227235.
Sundram, D. S., Mitra, K. & Webster, C. (1998) Word-of-mouth communications: a motivational
analysis, Advances in Consumer Research, 25, pp. 527531.
Triandis, H. C. (1995) Individualism & Collectivism (USA: Westview).
Watkins, H. & Liu, R. (1996) Collectivism, individualism and in-group membership: implications for
consumer complaining behaviors in multicultural contexts, Journal of International Consumer
Marketing, 8(3/4), pp. 6975.
Wilson, J. R. (1994) Word-of-mouth Marketing (New York: John Wiley & Sons).
Wilson, W. R. & Peterson, R. A. (1989) Some limits on the potency of word-of-mouth information,
Advances in Consumer Research, 16, pp. 2329.

Appendix
Word-of-mouth Items (In-group)
1.
2.
3.
4.

I like introducing new brands and products only to my close friends or


family.
I only provide information about new brands and products to my close
friends or family.
I like to seek advice or information only from my close friends or family
when making a purchase decision.
I only gather information about a product before I buy from my close
friends or family.

Word-of-mouth Items (Out-group)


1.

I like to provide people other than my close friends or family with


information about new brands or products.

228

D. Lam & D. Mizerski


2.
3.
4.

I share information about new brands and products with people other than
my close friends or family.
I seek out the advice of people other than my close friends or family
regarding which brand to buy.
I like to seek information and advice of people other than my close friends
or family before making a purchase decision.

Internal Locus of Control


1. My life is determined by my own actions.
2. When I get what I want it is usually because I worked hard for it.
3. I can pretty much determine what will happen in my life.

External Locus of Control (Chance)


1. To a great extent my life is controlled by accidental happenings.
2. When I get what I want it is usually because I am lucky.
3. It is not always wise for me to plan too far ahead because many things turn
out to be a matter of good or bad luck.

External Locus of Control (Powerful Others)


1. I feel like what happens in my life is mostly determined by powerful people.
2. My life is chiefly controlled by powerful others.
3. People like me have little chance of protecting our personal interests when
they conflict with those of strong pressure groups. (Rejected because of low
rotated factor loading.)

You might also like