You are on page 1of 9

Understanding and Treating ''Pusher Syndrome''

Hans-Otto Karnath and Doris Broetz


PHYS THER. 2003; 83:1119-1125.

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, can be
found online at: http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/83/12/1119
Collections

This article, along with others on similar topics, appears


in the following collection(s):
Balance
Balance Training
Neurology/Neuromuscular System: Other
Perspectives

e-Letters

To submit an e-Letter on this article, click here or click on


"Submit a response" in the right-hand menu under
"Responses" in the online version of this article.

E-mail alerts

Sign up here to receive free e-mail alerts

Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on January 1, 2015

Perspective

Understanding and Treating


Pusher Syndrome

Pusher syndrome is a clinical disorder following left or right brain


damage in which patients actively push away from the nonhemiparetic
side, leading to a loss of postural balance. The mechanism underlying
this disorder and its related anatomy has only recently been identified.
Investigation of patients with severe pushing behavior has shown that
perception of body posture in relation to gravity is altered. The
patients experience their body as oriented upright when the body
actually is tilted to the side of the brain lesion (to the ipsilesional side).
In contrast, patients with pusher syndrome show no disturbed processing of visual and vestibular inputs determining visual vertical. These
new insights have allowed the authors to suggest a new physical therapy
approach for patients with pusher syndrome where the visual control
of vertical upright orientation, which is undisturbed in these patients,
is the central element of intervention. [Karnath H-O, Broetz D.
Understanding and treating pusher syndrome. Phys Ther. 2003;83:
1119 1125.]

Key Words: Hemiparesis, Pusher syndrome, Spatial neglect, Spatial orientation, Thalamus.

Hans-Otto Karnath, Doris Broetz

Downloaded
from http://ptjournal.apta.org/
by guest on January 1, 2015
Physical Therapy . Volume 83 . Number
12 . December
2003

1119

A new physical therapy approach is

n 1985, Patricia Davies first described the surprising


behavior of some patients with stroke who use their
nonparetic extremities to push toward the paretic
side.1 When sitting or standing, these patients use
their nonparetic extremities to push away from the
nonparetic side, leading to a loss of lateral postural
balance (Fig. 1). If not prevented, they would push
themselves laterally to the point where they would fall
toward the hemiparetic side. There is forceful resistance
against interventions aiming to correct their tilted posture. Davies1 termed this behavior the pusher syndrome. An investigation of 327 patients with acute
stroke and hemiparesis revealed that the disorder was
present in 10.4% of the patients.2
The purpose of this article is to summarize the recent
literature related to pusher syndrome, including its
clinical diagnosis, related anatomy, and a description of
the mechanism found to be responsible for the disorder.
Further, a new physical therapy approach is suggested
based on these new insights into the nature of pushing
behavior.
Pusher SyndromeDistinctive Disorder or
Catch-all for Different Expressions of Postural
Instability Following Stroke?
Pushing actively with nonparetic extremities to the side
contralateral to the brain lesion (Fig. 1), which is termed
contraversive pushing, differentiates the clinical picture of pusher syndrome from the loss of equilibrum
that can occur in other patients with hemiparesis.
Because of their paresis, patients who do not exhibit
pusher syndrome may show deficits in balance and may
fall toward their paretic side.3,4 In contrast to patients
who exhibit contraversive pushing, these patients recognize when they lose equilibrum but are unable to
support themselves because of their paresis. They usually
cling to something with their nonparetic hand (ie, they
tend to pull, not push).
Use of the term pusher syndrome for a number of
different postural instability symptoms that occur in
patients after brain damage (for an overview, see Scha dler
and Kool5) should be avoided. The term should be used
for the distinctive disorder of actively pushing away from
the nonhemiparetic side as defined by Davies1 and
illustrated in Figure 1. Until recently, the pathophysio-

suggested based on new insights into


the nature of pushing behavior.
logical mechanism leading to pusher syndrome and the
particular brain structure damaged were unknown.
Is Contraversive Pushing Caused by
Hemineglect and Thus a Typical Disorder of
the Right Hemisphere?
Davies1 assumed that contraversive pushing frequently
occurs when left-sided neglect is present after lesions of
the right hemisphere. Similar assumptions have been
put forward by other authors.6,7 Thus, there has been
speculation about whether pushing behavior might be
caused by spatial neglect or might reflect a severe right
hemisphere syndrome.8
Davies,1 however, also observed that pushing behavior is
not almost exclusively associated with right brain damage, as is the case for patients who exhibit spatial
neglect.9,10 Pusher syndrome frequently occurs also with
lesions of the left hemisphere and is not related to
neglect but rather to aphasia.1 A study of 327 patients
with acute stroke and hemiparesis who were investigated
within the first couple of days after onset of stroke led to
the observation that left and right hemisphere damage
occurs with equal frequency in patients with contraversive pushing (left brain damage: 47%; right brain damage: 53%).2 Moreover, there was no evidence for a
regular co-occurrence of pathological pushing with spatial neglect, anosognosia, aphasia, or apraxia.2
In agreement with Pedersen et al,2 Karnath and
co-workers11 found that hemispatial neglect is not the
cause of contraversive pushing. In their sample of 23
patients with pusher syndrome, they found a large
proportion who had left brain damage and thus aphasia
but no neglect. Sixty-five percent of their patients with
contraversive pushing had right-side lesions, and 35%
had left-side lesions. Although contraversive pushing
within the group of patients with right-side lesions was
highly associated with spatial neglect (80% of these
patients also had neglect), neglect did not appear to be
the cause of pushing behavior. The reason for this
observation was that 20% of the patients with right brain
damage who exhibited contraversive pushing and 100%

H-O Karnath, MD, PhD, is Associate Professor for Neurology, Department of Cognitive Neurology, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research,
University of Tuebingen, Hoppe-Seyler-Str 3, D-72076 Tuebingen, Germany (Karnath@uni-tuebingen.de). Address all correspondence to Dr
Karnath.
D Broetz is Physical Therapist, Department of General Neurology, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tuebingen.
Both authors provided concept/idea, writing, subjects, and facilities/equipment.
This work was supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft awarded to Dr Karnath (Ka 1258/23).

1120 . Karnath and Broetz

Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest


on Therapy
January .1,Volume
2015 83 . Number 12 . December 2003
Physical


Figure 1.
A patient with right-side brain damage and pusher syndrome. The characteristic feature of the
disorder is that these patients, while sitting (left) or standing (right), spread the nonparetic
extremities from the body to push away actively from the nonparetic side. The result is the
typical tilted body posture of these patients. If not assisted by the examiner, the patients push
themselves into a lateral inclination until they fall toward the hemiparetic side.

of the patients with left brain damage who exhibited


contraversive pushing showed no symptoms of spatial
neglect. All of the patients had pusher syndrome due to
left-sided brain lesions rather had aphasia.11
We conclude that both neglect and aphasia are highly
associated with pushing behavior after right-side brain
damage (3 neglect) and after left-side brain damage (3
aphasia), but that both disorders neglect and aphasia
cannot be the underlying cause of pusher syndrome.
Symptoms such as neglect and anosognosia after rightsided lesions and aphasia after left-sided lesions frequently exist with contraversive pushing because the
relevant brain structures associated with these functions
lie in close proximity to each other. Neither neglect nor
aphasia, however, is causally related to contraversive
pushing.
What Is the Brain Structure Typically Damaged
in Patients With Pusher Syndrome?
Based on the traditional assumption that pushing behavior is caused by spatial neglect, it has been assumed that
the lesion location typically found in patients with spatial
neglect also must be responsible for pushing behavior.
Many therapists and physicians, therefore, have been
taught that pusher syndrome is most common in
patients with strokes in the parietal lobe of the right
hemisphere.
When this assumption was studied for the first time, the
data showed that the parietal cortex is not the neural
correlate of pusher syndrome.11 In a sample of 23

patients with severe contraversive pushing who were consecutively admitted to


a neurology department, the authors
identified brain lesions by magnetic resonance imaging or computed tomography. The overlap area of infarction in
the patients with pusher syndrome was
determined and compared with that of a
sample of 23 patients with stroke admitted in the same period who did not
exhibit contraversive pushing but were
similar with regard to age, etiology of
lesion, presence of hemiparesis, spatial
neglect, and aphasia. The analysis
revealed that the brain structure typically damaged in patients with pusher
syndrome is the left or right posterolateral thalamus. This finding suggests
that the posterolateral thalamus is
involved in our control of upright body
posture.

Traditionally, the posterolateral part of


the thalamus was thought to serve simply as a relay structure of the vestibular pathway on its
way from the brain stem to the cortex. The findings of
Karnath and colleagues,11 however, showed that this is
not the only task of the posterolateral thalamus. The
ventral posterior and lateral posterior nuclei of the
posterolateral thalamus rather seem to be fundamentally
involved in our control of upright body posture. Patients
exhibiting severe contraversive pushing showed a clear
overlap of their infarctions in this portion of the thalamus.11 This structure is anatomically distinct from the
vestibular cortex identified by Brandt and co-workers12
in the posterior insula. In addition, the clinical findings
in patients with such posterior insula lesions are different. While a lesion of the human vestibular cortex
leads to a tilt of the perceived visual vertical but not to
contraversive pushing,12 a lesion of the posterolateral
thalamus in patients with pusher syndrome induces the
opposite pattern. The patients with contraversive pushing show normal perception of visual vertical, but they
exhibit a severe tilt of perceived body posture in relation
to gravity.13 Thus, both graviceptive systems not only
appear to use distinct anatomical structures but also
seem to process afferent sensory information from
peripheral input sources differently.
Future studies are needed to investigate the possible role
of diaschisis.14 Lesions of those thalamic nuclei (ventral
posterolateral, ventral posteromedial, and lateral posterior) that were found to be affected in patients with
contraversive pushing11 might lead to additional functional or metabolic abnormalities in some of the structurally intact regions of the cortex. Thalamocortical

Downloaded
from http://ptjournal.apta.org/
by guest on January 1, 2015
Physical Therapy . Volume 83 . Number
12 . December
2003

Karnath and Broetz . 1121

processing of visual and vestibular inputs for the determination of visual vertical was unaffected by the lesion.
Consequently, when patients with pusher syndrome sit
upright, they experience a mismatch between visual
vertical, based on vestibular and visual inputs on the one
side, and their perception of tilted body orientation
relative to the vertical (Fig. 2).

Figure 2.
Sitting on a tilting chair, patients with pusher syndrome were required to
indicate when they reached upright body orientation.13 (a) With
occluded eyes, the patients experienced their body as oriented
upright when actually tilted 18 degrees to the side of the brain lesion.
(b) While viewing the laboratorys structured surroundings, the same
patients could align their longitudinal body axis to earth vertical.

axons arising in the ventral posterolateral and ventral


posteromedial nuclei project to the primary somatosensory cortex in the postcentral gyrus (Brodmann areas 3a,
3b, 1, and 2), to the secondary somatosensory cortex in
the parietal operculum, and to the insula.15 The lateral
posterior nucleus projects to the posterior parts of areas
5 and 7 of the superior and inferior parietal lobules.15
Imaging and other metabolic studies might help to
assess whether additional critical substrates in the cortex
are present and relevant in patients with pusher syndrome.
Which Mechanism Leads to Pusher Syndrome?
Recently, the mechanism leading to contraversive pushing has been investigated.13 The authors examined the
ability of patients with pusher syndrome to determine
upright position while their eyes were occluded. The
researchers found an altered perception of the bodys
orientation in relation to gravity by using a seating device
that allowed tilting the patient to the right or to the left
without ground contact. After a random tilt to the left or
to the right of at least 35 degrees, the patients were
required to indicate when they reached upright body
orientation. On the average, patients with contraversive
pushing experienced their body as oriented upright
when actually tilted 18 degrees to the side of the brain
lesion (Fig. 2a). Surprisingly, the same patients showed
no disturbed orientation perception of the visual world
(visual vertical). Thus, in contrast to their disturbed
perception of upright body posture (Fig. 2a), patients
with pusher syndrome could align their longitudinal
body axis to earth vertical when using visual cues from
the laboratorys surroundings13 (Fig. 2b). In addition,
without a visual surround in complete darkness, these
patients could correctly determine visual vertical when
sitting upright as well as when tilted in the position that
is perceived as upright. The latter results indicate that
1122 . Karnath and Broetz

Generally, a conflict between 2 reference systems is


either resolved by suppressing one of them, or both, or
by a compromise (eg, by weighted summation). However, neither happens in the present case.13 Under
normal bedside conditions, the patients with contraversive pushing do not align their body with the visual
vertical, with their perceived postural vertical, or with an
intermediate posture. They instead move the body in the
opposite direction. Karnath et al13 speculated that the
patients, by pushing their longitudinal body axis toward
the contralesional side, might be trying to compensate
for the mismatch between visual vertical and the tilted
orientation of body verticality. The clinical observation
that patients with contraversive pushing diminish their
pushing behavior when visual input is excluded (with
eyes closed) supports this notion. Accordingly, contraversive pushing did not occur when the patients with
contraversive pushing were tilted to the ipsilesional side
(the perceived upright position) and structured visual
input was excluded.13 Thus, the therapists attempt to
correct the patients body posture toward the upright
position (undertaken with eyes open) seems to contradict the patients effort to compensate the mismatch
between visual vertical and the tilted orientation of
perceived body verticality, and it induces the feeling of
lateral instability and their fear of falling and provokes
their active resisting against such attempts.13
Alternatively, it is possible that the pushing behavior is a
secondary response to the patients unexpected experience that they lose lateral balance when trying to get up
and sit upright. The experiment of Karnath et al13
revealed that the patients perceived upright orientation was tilted about 18 degrees toward the ipsilesional
side. Thus, when patients try to get up and orient the
body to what they perceive to be upright, they become
laterally instable because the center of mass is shifted too
far to the ipsilesional side. Pushing the body to the
opposite (contralesional) side might be the ensuing
reaction to this experience.13 Therefore, no pushing
occurs when patients sit immobilized by lateral stabilization in the cushioned safety of the experimental chair
that was used in the study.13
In the future, researchers may want to further investigate
these possible interpretations. Nevertheless, the study of
Karnath et al,13 clearly showed for the first time that
contraversive pushing is due to a severe misperception of

Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest


on Therapy
January .1,Volume
2015 83 . Number 12 . December 2003
Physical

ral representation of this second graviceptive system in humans.

Figure 3.
Patient with left-sided hemiparesis and pusher syndrome. Because orientation perception of the
visual surround is not impaired in these patients,13 they realize they are not in an erect position
by looking at their structured surroundings and can align their body with these earth-vertical
structures. In addition, the therapist may give visual aids (eg, use of an arm) to demonstrate the
earth-vertical, upright orientation (left). With little help, the patient is now able to orient his
body upright (right).

body orientation in relation to gravity. Moreover, the


data suggest that, for the purpose of rehabilitation, the
preserved ability to align the body axis to earth vertical
with the help of visual cues might be helpful. Although
patients with pusher syndrome are not spontaneously
able to use the visual input to control upright body
posture, this might become possible when training procedures apply this ability as part of conscious strategies
to control posture in these patients.
The discrepancy of a pathologically tilted postural vertical
concurrent with an unimpaired perception of the visual
vertical shows that patients with contraversive pushing
manifest a selective disturbance of control of upright
body posture.13 Although they are no longer able to
determine when their body is oriented in an erect
position, they have no problems correctly determining
the orientation of the visual world around them. Patients
with lesions of the vestibular system behave exactly the
opposite. They show visual-vestibular dysfunction with
a perceptual tilt of the visual vertical but have no
problems orienting their body to an earth-vertical,
upright position.12,16,17
These dissociations provide evidence for a separate
pathway in humans for sensing the orientation relative
to gravity that is apart from the well-known pathway for
orientation perception of the visual world. For this
reason, Karnath et al11 posited that the brain structure
typically damaged in patients with pusher syndrome
the posterolateral thalamusmight constitute the neu-

Diagnosis of Pushing Behavior


Our daily clinical experience leads us to
suggest 3 variables important in the
examination of patients with contraversive pushing: (1) spontaneous body posture, (2) increase of pushing force by
spreading of the nonparetic extremities
from the body, and (3) resistance to
passive correction of posture. We determine these variables with the patient
both sitting (feet with ground contact)
and standing. The examiner sits or
stands on the paretic side of the patient
to prevent falling.

Spontaneous Body Posture


The most striking feature of patients
with contraversive pushing is their spontaneous posture while sitting and standing. Their longitudinal body axis is
tilted toward the paretic side. This
behavior is best observed without prior instructions,
right after changing position (eg, from a supine position
to sitting at the bedside). To quantify pathological body
posture, we differentiate among 3 intensities (see
Appendix): severe contraversive tilt with falling to the
side contralateral to the brain lesion, severe contraversive tilt without falling, and mild contraversive tilt without falling. A tilted logitudinal body axis must occur
regularly, not just occasionally, due to the normal insecurity in balancing when patients become hemiparetic
after stroke.
Abduction and Extension of the Nonparetic Extremities
Another feature of contraversive pushing is the use of
the nonparetic extremities to bring about the pathological lateral tilt of the body axis. With the patient sitting
on the bedside, we observed that the ipsilesional hand is
abducted from the body searching for contact with the
surface and the elbow is extended (Fig. 1). In our
experience, if the feet have ground contact, the ipsilesional leg will be abducted, and the knee and hip joints
will be extended as well. To quantify this characteristic
feature, we use visual assessment of abduction and
extension of the extremities (see Appendix), depending
on whether the movements occur spontaneously even at
rest or only on changing position (eg, on moving the
patient from the wheelchair to the bed or on standing
up after sitting).
Resistance to Passive Correction of Tilted Posture
Evaluating a patients behavior on being corrected by
the investigator to an upright position is the third

Downloaded
from http://ptjournal.apta.org/
by guest on January 1, 2015
Physical Therapy . Volume 83 . Number
12 . December
2003

Karnath and Broetz . 1123

diagnostic feature for determining the presence of contraversive pushing. It is known that any attempt by the
examiner to move the tilted body axis to an upright
position by shifting the weight toward the nonparetic
side elicits active resistance from the patient. The patient
increases the force in the already extended nonparetic
extremity. During our clinical examination (see Appendix), we evaluate the occurrence or nonoccurrence of
active resistance to being interventionally corrected.
The Appendix summarizes the 3 variables (ie, spontaneous body posture, increase of pushing force by spreading
of the nonparetic extremities from the body, and resistance to passive correction of posture) in the form of a
scale, published as the so-called Clinical Scale for
Contraversive Pushing (SCP).13,18 The authors13,18
intended the scale to aid clinicians in diagnosing the
presence of pushing behavior and determining its severity. The weighted values that were tentatively assigned to
each finding of the examination in the Appendix are still
in the process of being validated. For a firm diagnosis of
contraversive pushing, we suggest a value of 1 or more
(summed over the results for sitting and standing;
maximum2 per variable) for each of the 3 variables.
However, further investigation of the scale is needed;
lower or higher values might turn out to be more
adequate for a firm diagnosis.
Prognosis of the Disorder
At the time of admission to the hospital following the
stroke, patients with contraversive pushing have a more
severely impaired level of consciousness and ability to
walk, paresis of the upper and lower extremities, and
lower initial function in activities of daily living than
patients with hemiparesis but without contraversive
pushing.2 However, in contrast to other neuropsychological deficits such as aphasia or spatial neglect, we found
that contraversive pushing is a disorder that can be well
compensated for by the brain. Only 6 months after a
stroke, pathological pushing behavior is rarely still evident.19 Pusher syndrome thus has a good prognosis19
and does not seem to negatively influence the outcome
of rehabilitation. However, we also know that patients
with contraversive pushing take 3.6 weeks (ie, 63%)
longer than patients without pusher syndrome to reach
the same functional outcome level.2 Thus, physical therapy for contraversive pushing should aim to shorten this
period. Patients with contraversive pushing should
become independent of help from other people in less
time and should be discharged from inpatient care
earlier.
Suggestion for a New Strategy for Treating
Pusher Syndrome
From the recent finding that patients with contraversive
pushing have impaired perception of the bodys orien-

1124 . Karnath and Broetz

tation in relation to gravity,13 it follows that pathological


pushing should not be treated in a horizontal position,
but in an earth-vertical position (ie, while the patient is
sitting, standing, or walking). Moreover, because perception of the visual surround turned out to be unimpaired
in patients with contraversive pushing,13 they can see
that they are not in an erect position by looking at their
structured surroundings (Fig. 2b). The patients appear
to be unable, however, to spontaneously make use of this
preserved ability; they have to be trained to do so.
Because the patients feel erect when they see that they are
tilted, and vice versa,13 we believe the first goal of
physical therapy should be to demonstrate this, showing
the patients that visual information corresponds to reality. While sitting or standing, the patients should be
asked to see whether they are oriented upright. We also
provided an experience that showed patients that it is
beneficial to use visual aids (eg, the therapists arm as
shown in Fig. 3) to give patients feedback about their
body orientation. It is our observation that the experience of not falling after attaining the corrected position,
combined with seeing that they are upright, increases
the patients confidence and lowers both the presence
and the extent of the reaction to abduct and extend the
nonparetic extremities to push toward the paretic side.
In our clinical experience, the intervention plan that is
most effective is the one that is designed in such a way
that the patients learn the following in sequential order:
Realize the disturbed perception of erect body
position.
Visually explore the surroundings and the bodys
relation to the surroundings. Ensure that the
patient sees whether he or she is oriented upright.
We suggest that the physical therapist use visual aids
that give feedback about body orientation (eg, the
therapists arm as shown in Fig. 3) and work in a
room containing many vertical structures, such as
door frames, windows, pillars, and so on.
Learn the movements necessary to reach a vertical
body position.
Maintain the vertical body position while performing other activities.
In our day-by-day clinical management of patients with
pusher syndrome, we see that this procedure produces
successful results. However, research is needed involving
controlled studies of this new approach to examine the
effects of the intervention and whether it shortens the
time for inpatient care and accelerates independence in
daily living.
References
1 Davies PM. Steps to Follow: A Guide to the Treatment of Adult Hemiplegia.
New York, NY: Springer; 1985.

Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest


on Therapy
January .1,Volume
2015 83 . Number 12 . December 2003
Physical


2 Pedersen PM, Wandel A, Jorgensen HS, et al. Ipsilateral pushing in
stroke: incidence, relation to neuropsychological symptoms, and
impact on rehabilitationthe Copenhagen stroke study. Arch Phys Med
Rehabil. 1996;77:2528.
3 Bohannon RW, Smith MB, Larkin PA. Relationship between independent sitting balance and side of hemiparesis. Phys Ther. 1986;66:
944 945.
4 Dettmann MA, Linder MT, Sepic SB. Relationships among walking
performance, postural stability, and functional assessment of the
hemiplegic patient. Am J Phys Med. 1987;66:7790.
5 Scha dler S, Kool JP. Pushen: syndrom oder symptom? eine literaturu bersicht. Zeitschrift fur Physiotherapeuten. 2001;53:716.
6 Bateman A, Riddoch MJ. Neuropsychological perspectives on
pusher syndrome. Eur J Phys Rehabil. 1996;6:9396.

11 Karnath H-O, Ferber S, Dichgans J. The neural representation of


postural control in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97:
1393113936.
12 Brandt T, Dieterich M, Danek A. Vestibular cortex lesions affect the
perception of verticality. Ann Neurol. 1994;35:403 412.
13 Karnath H-O, Ferber S, Dichgans J. The origin of contraversive
pushing: evidence for a second graviceptive system in humans. Neurology. 2000;55:1298 1304.
14 Feeney DM, Baron J-C. Diaschisis. Stroke. 1986;17:817 830.
15 Jones EG. The Thalamus. Neew York, NY: Plenum Press; 1985.
16 Bisdorff AR, Wolsley CJ, Anastasopoulos D, et al. The perception of
body verticality (subjective postural vertical) in peripheral and central
vestibular disorders. Brain. 1996;119:15231534.

7 Premiselli S, Cesana L, Cerri C. Pusher syndrome in stroke: clinical,


neuropsychological, and neurophysiological investigation. Eur Med
Phys. 2001;37:143151.

17 Anastasopoulos D, Haslwanter T, Bronstein A, et al. Dissociation


between the performance of body verticality and the visual vertical in
acute peripheral vestibular disorder in humans. Neurosci Lett. 1997;233:
151153.

8 Punt TD, Riddoch MJ. Towards a theoretical understanding of


pushing behaviour in stroke patients. Neuropsych Rehabil. 2002;12:
455 472.

18 Karnath H-O, Broetz D, Goetz A. Klinik, Ursache und Therapie der


Pusher-Symptomatik. Nervenarzt. 2001;72:86 92.

9 Mesulam M-M. Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal, and


cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional
targeting of salient extrapersonal events. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B.
1999;354:13251346.

19 Karnath H-O, Johannsen L, Broetz D, et al. Prognosis of contraversive pushing. J Neurol. 2002;249:1250 1253.

10 Karnath H-O, Zihl J. Disorders of spatial orientation. In: Brandt T,


Caplan LR, Dichgans J, et al, eds. Neurological Disorders: Course and
Treatment. 2nd ed. San Diego, Calif: Academic Press; 2003:277286.

Appendix.
Clinical Assessment Scale for Contraversive Pushing (SCP)13,18
Examination Form
Clinical Scale for Contraversive Pushing (SCP)
Name

Date of birth

Ward

Examination date
Physician in charge

Diagnosis
Physical therapist in charge

(A) Spontaneous body posture


Value 1severe contraversive tilt with falling to that side
Value 0.75severe contraversive tilt without falling
Value 0.25mild contraversive tilt without falling
Value 0inconspicuous

Sitting

Standing

Sum total (max2):


(B) Abduction and extension of the nonparetic extremities
Value 1performed spontaneously, already when at rest
Value 0.5performed only on changing the position (eg, on transferring
from bed to wheelchair)
Value 0inconspicuous
Sum total (max2):
(C) Resistance to passive correction of tilted posturea
Value 1resistance occurs
Value 0resistance does not occur
Sum total (max2):
a

Touch the patient at the sternum and the back. Instruction: I will move your body sidewards. Please permit this movement.

Downloaded
from http://ptjournal.apta.org/
by guest on January 1, 2015
Physical Therapy . Volume 83 . Number
12 . December
2003

Karnath and Broetz . 1125

Understanding and Treating ''Pusher Syndrome''


Hans-Otto Karnath and Doris Broetz
PHYS THER. 2003; 83:1119-1125.

References

This article cites 15 articles, 6 of which you can access


for free at:
http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/83/12/1119#BIBL

Cited by

This article has been cited by 13 HighWire-hosted


articles:
http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/83/12/1119#otherarticles

Subscription
Information

http://ptjournal.apta.org/subscriptions/

Permissions and Reprints http://ptjournal.apta.org/site/misc/terms.xhtml


Information for Authors

http://ptjournal.apta.org/site/misc/ifora.xhtml

Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on January 1, 2015

You might also like