You are on page 1of 7

Caregivers’ Socialization of Gender Roles in a

Children’s Museum
Michael Abrahamy, Elizabeth Blake Finkelson,
Connie Lydon, Kathleen Murray

Literature has shown that parents are the primary source of gender role
socialization. Through modeling, play, and other familial interactions, chil-
dren internalize parental messages regarding gender. Given these findings,
this study explores the question of whether, in play settings where children
have a choice of occupational costumes to wear, caregivers will reinforce
societal gender stereotyping of occupations by urging their children to try on
costumes that are gender congruent or gender neutral. While past research
has indicated that caregivers exhibit more gender role stereotypes to boys
than girls, our findings suggest that there is no significant difference in the
amount of gender opposite occupational costumes given to boys and girls. It
was also argued that male caregivers would be more traditional in display-
ing gender role stereotypes through the offering of occupational costumes
than female caregivers and the statistical data supports this conclusion.
Implications of the effects of gender role stereotypes are also discussed.

INTRODUCTION will obtain much of their behavior. As Witt points


out, developing children internalize the subtle mes-
Through the process of gender role socializa- sages that parents send regarding what is acceptable
tion, children learn from a very early age what it for each gender. (Arliss, 1991, as cited in Witt,
means to be a boy or a girl in our society. They learn 1999).
their gender roles and develop a sense of gendered Socialization begins at birth and children's
self from social and environmental cues such as the internalization of parental messages about gender is
behaviors, ideas, attitudes and beliefs that surround not far behind. Many studies cited by Fagot,
them each day. A potentially important context for Leinbach, & O'Boyle (1992) attest to the presence of
the construction and the socialization of gender is gender labeling, stereotyping, and sex-typed behav-
the family, where parents are major socializing ior in very young children and verify that both par-
agents (Leaper 2002). ents influence these early displays of gender typed
It is widely held that parents play a signifi- behavior. The year between a child's second and
cant role in "shaping" a child's gender role behavior. third birthdays has been identified as the period dur-
According to Snow, Jackson, & Maccoby (1983), ing which sex stereotypes for toys, clothing, tools,
parents socialize boys and girls differently, though it household objects, games, and work are acquired
may be unintentional. Parents form their children's (Huston, 1983). During this time, children are not
ideas of what it means to be masculine or feminine only aware of adult gender role differences, but also
through both overt and covert socialization. This of their own sexual identity, discovering that they
socialization of gender roles is in accordance with themselves are either male or female (Thompson,
the social-learning theory, which explains the acqui- 1975).
sition of behavior through reinforcement and model- From birth, parents treat sons and daughters
ing (Lytton & Romney, 1991). The parents will be differently, dressing infants in gender specific out-
the first and perhaps most significant sources of such fits, painting their rooms in gender-specific colors,
reinforcement and the people from whom children giving them gender differentiated toys, and expect-

Perspectives in Psychology Spring 2003 Ψ 19


ing different behaviors from boys and girls (Thorne, al costumes to wear, caregivers will reinforce socie-
1993). It appears that parents also treat boys and tal gender stereotyping of occupations by urging
girls differently with regard to how strongly they their children to try on costumes which are gender
enforce gender roles. In a study examining parents' congruent or gender neutral. Specifically, we make
gender typing of toys, Campenni (1999) found that, the following hypotheses: caregivers will choose
in general, feminine toys were stereotyped more than masculine and neutral occupational costumes more
masculine toys. For example, people are more likely often than feminine occupational costumes for boys,
to say that a boy should not play with a Barbie doll, but will choose feminine, neutral, and masculine
but that it is acceptable for a girl to play with a GI occupational costumes for girls, thus displaying
Joe doll. Campenni(1999) also found that parents more gender specific stereotyping for boys than girls
tended to choose masculine and neutral toys more between the ages of 3-6 years old. We also hypothe-
often than feminine toys for their sons, but for their size that male caregivers will reinforce gender
daughters chose neutral toys more often than femi- stereotypes more than female caregivers by choosing
nine or masculine ones. fewer gender opposite occupational costumes for
Some studies suggest that it is not only the both boys and girls.
sex of the child that is salient in gender role social-
ization, but also the characteristics of the parent. It
appears that males tend to be more traditional in their METHODS
enforcement of gender-roles than females (Kulik,
2002). Snow et al. (1983) found that fathers use toys The Please Touch Museum (PTM), located in
to socialize their male and female children different- Center City Philadelphia, is a child-oriented muse-
ly by giving them gender specific toys. Women, um open to the public seven days a week. The PTM
however, often have less traditional attitudes toward encourages children to interact with exhibits that
gender roles than men and are more likely to use provide hands-on learning experiences through cre-
behavioral modeling as a means of gender socializa- ative and imaginative play. The museum is designed
tion. for children who range in age from infancy to seven
Gender role stereotypes are also found to be years old. The children are usually accompanied by
at the core of gender segregation of occupations. As adults who guide them through the different activi-
Gatten et al. (1999) observe, a number of studies ties. Our data was collected exclusively on weekends
(Shepard & Hess, 1975; Shinar, 1975; Panek, Rush at the "Me on TV" exhibit in the PTM.
& Greenawalt, 1977; Freedman, Podsakoff, & The "Me on TV" is a 25 square foot interac-
Mackenzie, 1993; St. Pierre, Herendeen, Moore tive exhibit that gives children the opportunity to see
&Nagle 1994) have examined occupational gender themselves on television. In a newsroom setting,
stereotyping, all concluding that gender stereotypes "Unique costumes and a variety of stage props help
of occupations do exist. In fact, some of the clearest to set the scene as children use cameras and monitors
sex differences in adolescence and adulthood seem to capture their pretend play on live TV"(PTM
to occur in domains such as occupational choice and Brochure). The set up consists of a video camera in
interest (Lytton & Romney, 1991), where job selec- the center of the room, which focuses on a reporter's
tion by males and females corresponds with beliefs desk in front of a backdrop of Philadelphia and a
about appropriate gender role behaviors weatherboard. To the right of the camera is a green
Although there is extensive research on gen- screen that allows children to appear in front of var-
der specific stereotyping of toys and parents' influ- ious backgrounds and locations that can be changed
ence on their children's conception of gender, no at the push of a button. There are three monitors that
observational study has yet examined how care- the children can use to see themselves. To the right
givers shape the gender stereotypes of children of the green screen is the rack of costumes that can
through their selection of gender typed occupational be used for dress up in front of the cameras.
costumes for imaginary play. The present study was To test our hypotheses we first conducted a
undertaken in an attempt to help fill that void. We are pictorial survey on the occupational costumes avail-
exploring the general question of whether, in play able to the children in the " Me on TV " exhibit to
settings where children have a choice of occupation-
Perspectives in Psychology Spring 2003 Ψ 20
determine the gender specificity of each costume. offered to the child, and the sex of the parent. This
Before we conducted our survey we excluded three process continued until we had observed 10 male
costumes in order to establish that every costume caregiver-boy, 10 male caregiver-girl, 10 female
was equal in style and ease of wear. A sample of 49 caregiver-boy and 10 female caregiver-girl pairs. In
college students were given pictures of each costume situations where there was a caregiver who present-
and asked to place them into piles of masculine, fem- ed a costume to more than one child, only the first
inine, and neutral according to their perception of the child was noted as data.
gender of the costume. Costumes that were placed in
one of the three gender categories at least 70% of the
time were used in the study. Furthermore, the gen- RESULTS
der categories in which the 70% fell determined
whether it would be used as masculine, neutral, or Analysis Techniques of Gender Socialization
feminine in the study. To analyze our data we used non-parametric
In order to ensure reliability, we conducted tests of significance due to our small sample size,
two pilot studies to establish inter-observer agree- possible " litter effects" from collecting data at only
ment. In the first study we observed one subject and one site (Martin & Bateson, 1986), and our inability
had an inter-observer agreement of 75%. After col- to meet the high number of observations required to
lecting this data we determined that there was a dif- run a parametric analysis (Siegel & Levine, 1956).
ference of opinion in what constituted "giving" a
costume to the child. Having established this criteria Gender Socialization According to Gender of
we conducted our second pilot study consisting of Caregiver
two subjects in which we obtained an inter observer To test whether the number of caregivers
agreement of 100%. who will choose gender opposite costumes for boys
We used focal sampling (Martin & Bateson, is the same as the number of caregivers who will
1986) of caregivers in the " Me on TV " exhibit. choose gender opposite costumes for girls, we used
When the observer was sure that at least one of each a two way chi-square. This test yielded a chi-square
type of costume was available for use, the first par- (X2) value of 1.111. The chi square value of 1.111
ent who took a costume off the rack for his/her child is well below the critical value of 3.84 required for
became the focal subject. We watched the caregiver significance at the .05 level of significance. The
until they handed the costume to the child, at which probability that the random variable is greater than
point we recorded the sex of the child, the costume chi-square was 0.2918, with df=1 (degree of free-

Figure 1: Results of Occupational Costume Selection Figure 2: Results of Occupational Costume Selection
by Male Caregivers by Female Caregivers

4.5 6
4
5
Number of Time Given
Number of Times Given

3.5
3 4
Female Caregiver/
2.5 Male Caregiver/ Boy Boy
3
2 Male Caregiver/ Girl Female Caregiver/
Girl
1.5 2
1
1
0.5
0 0
Construction Worker

Construction Worker
Postal Service

Nurse
Postal Service
Doctor
Firefighter

Doctor
Nurse

Veterinarian
Firefighter
Veterinarian

Type of Costume Type of Costume

*A total number of 40 caregivers were observed composed of the following: 10 male caregivers to boy, 10 male caregivers to
girl, 10 female caregivers to boy, and 10 female caregivers to girl.
Perspectives in Psychology Spring 2003 Ψ 21
dom). Thus, in spite of visual differences, there were becoming less acceptable in our society.
no statistically significant differences found between Furthermore, studies of college students and adults
caregivers offering of gender opposite costumes to indicate a trend toward egalitarian attitudes regard-
boys and girls. ing women's roles. As compared to past studies
reporting that adolescents expect mothers of young
Gender Socialization According to Gender of Child children to remain in the home full time, our findings
To test whether the number of male care- indicate a continuing egalitarian trend (Braito &
givers who will choose gender opposite costumes for Klundt, 1984).
their children is the same as the number of female Our second hypothesis was that male care-
caregivers who will choose gender opposite cos- givers would reinforce gender stereotypes more than
tumes for their children, we used a two way chi- female caregivers by choosing fewer gender oppo-
square. This test yielded a chi-square (X2) value of site costumes for both girls and boys. Our statistical
4.444. The probability that the random variable is data is significant for this hypothesis because no
greater than chi-square was 0.0350, and df=1. This male caregivers gave gender opposite costumes and
result indicates a statistically significant difference female caregivers gave gender opposite costumes
between gender socialization of children by male four times. This finding was not surprising given the
caregivers compared to female caregivers. Female extensive research suggesting this trend. Male and
caregivers had a higher tendency than males to go female caregivers treat their children differently with
against gender norms and provide their child with a regard to gender socialization (Ruble, 1998; Siegal
gender opposite occupational costume. 1987; Kulik, 2002).
One of our largest challenges in testing our
hypothesis was that male caregivers tended to be less
DISCUSSION interactive with the costumes than were female care-
givers. Male caregivers often gravitated toward the
Overall, we did not find that our sample of camera and ignored the other attractions such as the
caregivers socialized boys and girls differently by costumes, even though they were just as active with
their costume choice. This was a surprising finding their children. Often, while these male caregivers
as most of the literature supports the conclusion that were exploring the technical aspects of the exhibit,
parents do socialize boys and girls differently. There female caregivers were interacting with their chil-
are several possible reasons that our data differs dren by giving suggestions on how to use the weath-
from this previous research. First, our sample size erboard or in some cases moving toward the costume
was small, consisting of 40 subjects, which area.
increased the influence each subject had on our data. Although these observations may not direct-
In addition, we performed our study in one museum ly affect our data, they offer some interesting impli-
that had a patronage made up of mostly white, mid- cations as they suggest that males and females do
dle class families as the location itself was in a large exhibit different behaviors with their children. This,
economically developed city. Previous research has in turn, may " shape" the child's view of gender by
shown that older persons, unemployed women and demonstrating that men are supposed to work with
members of low socio-economic status groups tend the technical and more physical tools and women are
to be more traditional in their enforcement of gender supposed to dress up and be more creative. As
role than employed females, young persons, and Lindsey & Mize (2001) have found, fathers engage
members of middle socio-economic groups (Kulik, in more physical play and mothers engage in more
2002). pretend play. Even more importantly, they found that
Another potential reason for these surprising the type of play that a child engaged in with his par-
findings is that our culture is moving slowly toward ents reflected the type of play he would exhibit with
equality between the sexes. As Idle et al. (1993) sug- his peers. This research illuminates the importance
gest, there appears to be a change in parents' percep- of our observed difference in socializing behavior
tion of what is acceptable gender typing behavior as between mothers and fathers, as children go on to
they begin to realize that gender stereotyping is internalize and exhibit these behaviors.
These observations not only presented chal-
Perspectives in Psychology Spring 2003 Ψ 22
lenges in our data collection, but also posed some have found that job related stress increases for
interesting ideas for further research on gender women working in male dominated occupations and
socialization. Men seem to use technology more than men in female dominated occupations (Evans &
women in socializing their children. An interesting Steptoe, 2002; Yoder & Aniakudo, 1996).
study could look at what factors cause this and what In order to make lasting strides in occupa-
the possible effects are on the child. Females seem tional equality the focus should be turned toward
to support pretend play more than males by dressing children. As some researchers comment, "The role
their children up in costumes and using this as a tool of direct socialization appears to be crucial, then, not
for socializing. In light of both of these observa- only in its own right but also in establishing the
tions, additional studies may provide useful informa- foundation upon which later self- socialization is
tion about the causes and effects of these contrasting based" (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1987). In addition,
methods of socialization. For example, a study many studies have tested whether or not parents'
could look at the specific tools that male caregivers gender schemas affect child development. For exam-
utilize to socialize their children in comparison to ple, Martin (2000) argues that gender schemas
the methods female caregivers use. Such a study acquired in childhood are very important as they
could focus on parental interactions in the home, guide behavior during adolescence and later on in
observing the amounts of physical versus verbal play adulthood. Consequently, it is these learned gender
exhibited by each sex parent. Perhaps future schemas that will guide children and restrict or
researchers could observe parent-child interaction broaden their views of gender roles and their behav-
within an experimental play setting containing both iors throughout their lives. Given this finding, it is
technical toys like video games and more creative important to continue looking at the socialization of
games like dress-up. It would be necessary to have gender roles in children, and specifically the social-
both stereotypically male and female video games ization of gender stereotypes in occupations as a way
and dress up clothes, so that both game options to better understand how this process affects society.
would be attractive to both sexes of children. A study
like this would have important implications, as par-
ents could be made aware of the types of behaviors APPENDIX
they are modeling as representative of certain gender
behaviors. Figure A: Costume Selection by Gender of
Another implication for future research Male Caregivers Compared to Female Caregivers
stems from the fact that our small sample size of 40 9
subjects and the limited demographic did not ade- 8
quately represent the United States population. 7
Number of Times Given

Since Kulik (2002) has found that personal back- 6


ground, as well as gender, contributes to how care-
5 Male Caregiver
givers display gender stereotypes, it would be inter-
4 Female Caregiver
esting to study the effects of race, socioeconomic
3
status, age and other factors that may contribute to
2
parental socialization of gender roles.
Our culture is in a transition period where it 1

is striving to attain equality between genders. This 0


Masculine Feminine Neutral
desire to change gender role stereotypes, specifical-
Type of Occupational Costume
ly in regard to occupation, is so ubiquitous that it has
even affected our language. For example the term
"fireman" is slowly changing to the more gender
Figures A and B are additional graphs that were not included
neutral "firefighter"( Liben, Bigler, Krogh, 2002). in the results section of the paper because they were not statis-
Most research on women firefighters and male nurs- tically analyzed. These two graphs are merely a depiction of
es demonstrates that even though their numbers are the breakup of type of occupational costume from gender spe-
increasing, they have overcome and continue to face cific and gender opposite into masculine, feminine and neutral.
challenges posed by stereotypical ideology. Many
Perspectives in Psychology Spring 2003 Ψ 23
fathers with their sons and daughters.” Sex Roles,
Figure B: Costume Selection by Gender for Boys
28(11-12), 679-691.
Compared to Girls
Kite, M.E. (2001). “Changing times, changing gender roles:
Who do we want our women and men to be?” In
14
Number of Times Given R.K. Unger (Ed.), Handbook of the Psychology of
12 Women and Gender. New York: Wiley.
10 Kulik, L. (2002). “The impact of social background on gen-
8 Caregiver to boy der-role ideology.” Journal of Family Issues, 23(1),
6 Caregiver to girl 53-73.
Leaper, C. (2002). “Parenting girls and boys.” In M.H.
4
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting: Vol. 1.
2
Children and Parenting (2nd ed,. pp. 189-225).
0 Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Levin, J. & Fox, J. (2000). Elementary Statistics in Social
ne

al
line

utr
ini

Research. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.


Ne
cu

m
Fe
as
M

Liben L.S., Bigler, R.S., & Krogh H.R. (2002). “Language at


Type of Occupational work: Children's gendered interpretations of occu-
Costume pational titles.” Child Development, 73(3), 810-828.
Lindsay, E. W. & Mize, J. (2001). “Contextual differences in
parent-child play: Implications for children's gender-
role development.” Sex Roles, 44(3-4), 155-176.
Lytton H., & Romney, D.M. (1991). “Parents' differential
REFERENCES socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis.”
Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267-269.
Arliss, L.P. (1991). Gender Communication. Englewood Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1987). “Parent-child inter-
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. action in dyads and triads, at age 6 1/2 years.”
Braito, R. & Klundt, K. (1984). “The adolescent's view of Unpublished manuscript.
conditions under which women should work: A Mallon, S.C. (1995). “Leadership position and sex role
gender role socialization perspective.” Western stereotyping of Hispanic gifted and talented chil-
Social Science Association, (pp. 142-153). dren.” Dissertation Abstracts International, 56( 2).
Campenni, C. E. (1999). “Gender stereotyping of children’s Martin, C. L. (2000). “Cognitive theories of gender develop-
toys: A comparison of the parents and nonparents.” ment.” In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The
Sex Roles, 40(1-2), 121-138. Developmental Social Psychology of Gender (pp.91-
Eisenberg, N., Wolchik, S.A., Hernandez, R., & Pasternack, 121). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
J.F. (1985). “Parental socialization of young chil- Martin, P. & Bateson, P. (1986). Measuring Behaviour: An
dren's play: A short-term, longitudinal study.” Child Introductory Guide. Cambridge: Cambridge
Development, 56, 1506-1513. University Press.
Evans, O. & Steptoe, A. (2002). “The contribution of gender- Please Touch Museum Brochure. (2003).
role orientation, work factors and home stressors to Ruble, D. N. (1988). “Sex role development.” In M.H.
psychological well-being and sickness absence in Barnstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental
male and female dominated occupational groups.” Psychology: An Advanced Textbook, 2nd ed.
Social Science & Medicine, 54(4), 481-492. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fagot, B. I., Leinbach, M.D., & O'Boyle, C. (1992). “Gender Siegal, M.(1987). “Are sons and daughters treated more dif-
labeling, gender stereotyping, and parenting behav- ferently by fathers than by mothers?”
iors. Developmental Psychology, 28, 225-230. Developmental Review, 7, 183-209.
Gatten, D. S., Dubois, C. L. Z., & Faley, R.H. (1999). “The Siegal, S. (1956). Nonparametric Statistics for the
effects of organizational context on occupational Behavioral Sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.
gender-stereotyping.” Sex Roles, 40(7/8), 567-582. Snow, M. E., Jacklin, C. N., & Maccoby, E. E. (1983). “Sex-
Heilman, M.E. (2001). “Description and prescription: How of-child differences in father-child interaction at one
gender stereotypes prevent women's ascent up the year of age.” Child Development, 54, 227-232.
organizational ladder.” Journal of Social Issues, Tenenbaum, H.R., & Leaper, C. (2002). “Are parents' gender
57(4), 657-674. schemas related to their children's gender-related
Huston, A. C. (1983). “Sex-typing.” In E. M. Hetherington & cognitions? A meta-analysis.” Developmental
P. H. Mussen (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology, 38, 615-630.
Psychology: Vol. 4, Socialization, Personality, and Thompson, S. (1975). “Gender labels and early sex role
Social Development (4th ed., pp. 387-467). New development.” Child Development, 46, 339-347.
York: Wiley. Thorne, B. (1993). Gender Play: Girls and Boys in School.
Idle, T., Wood, E. & Desmarais, S. (1993). “Gender role New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
socialization in toy play situations: Mothers and

Perspectives in Psychology Spring 2003 Ψ 24


Weinraub et al. (1984). “The development of sex roles stereo-
types in the third year: Relationships to gender label-
ing, gender identity, and sex-typed toy preference,
and family characteristics.” Child Development, 55,
1493-1503.
Witt, S. D. (1997). “Parental influence on children's social-
ization to gender roles.” Adolescence, 32(126), 253-
259.
Yoder, J.D. & McDonald, T.W. (1998). “Measuring sexist
discrimination in the workplace: Support for the
validity if the schedule of sexist events.” Psychology
of Women Quarterly, 22, 3, 487-491.
Zarate, M.A., Bonilla, S. & Luevano, M. (1995). “Ethnic
influences on exemplar retrieval and stereotyping.”
Social Cognition, 13(2), 145-162.

Perspectives in Psychology Spring 2003 Ψ 25

You might also like