You are on page 1of 22

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning CENTRE FOR APPLIED LINGUISTICS MA ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET

Student I/D: 1163612

STUDENT ID NUMBER: 1163612 PROGRAMME: MA IN ELTMM/ICT

MODULE NAME: SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AND CLASSROOM LANGUAGE TEACHING ET990-2 MODULE TUTORS: EMA USHIODA / ANNA-MARIA PINTER

WORD COUNT: 3,257 ASSIGNMENT QUESTION:

DATE DUE: 12 NOON on 12 JANUARY 2012 DATE SUBMITTED: 10 JANUARY 2012

In completing the details on this cover sheet and submitting the assignment, you are doing so on the basis that this assignment is all your own work and that you have not borrowed or failed to acknowledge anyone elses work

Please X this box if you agree to this statement

X
0

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning INTRODUCTION

Student I/D: 1163612

There has been a paradigm shift in motivation research in respect of second language acquisition (SLA) over the last twenty years. In the early 1990s, there was a sense that the social-psychological tradition, which framed methods of enquiry, had run its course and that alternative perspectives were needed to revitalise and refocus the L2 motivation field ((Drnyei and Ushioda, 2011:46). There was a subsequent phase, the cognitive-situated period, drawing on cognitive theories from educational psychology. Out of this came a process-oriented approach, focusing on changes in individual involvement over time. This, in turn, has evolved into (or perhaps merged with) a new socio-dynamic phase (Drnyei and Ushioda, 2011:69). I will discuss several key empirical studies into L2 motivation, roughly arranged in order of whether they take a quantitative, qualitative or mixed methods approach. It must, however, be noted that despite the obvious distinctions and the preference to view these on a continuum (Drnyei, 2007:25), they have ideological differences, a contrast in categorization and a contrast in the perception of individual diversity which permeates the methodology used. I will follow this with my own commentary, the implications which can be drawn for my own research and in what ways I have reflected about SLA in the last few months. QUANTITATIVE APPROACH Quantitative social research grew out of the desire to emulate the objective procedures found in the natural sciences. The use of measurable, statistically based evidence, a priori categorization, variables rather than cases, and standardized procedures seeks to eradicate researcher subjectivity. It is systematic and rigorous, and uses in-built cross-checking indices (Drnyei, 2007:32-34). Language learners attitudes and motivation have traditionally been measured by means of quantitative methods, typically using large-scale questionnaire surveys, to account for the attitudes of whole speech communities. Most empirical research up until the 1990s was dominated by a social-psychological approach initiated in bilingual Canada by Robert Gardner, Wallace Lambert and Richard Clment. This is understandable, according to Drnyei (1998:122): since learning the language of another community simply cannot be separated from the learners social dispositions towards the speech community in question the students attitudes towards the specific language group are bound to influence how successful they will be in incorporating aspects of that language (Gardner, 1985, in Drnyei, 1998:122).

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

Gardner et al (1985) developed a standardized motivation battery, the AMTB (Attitude/Motivation Test Battery) to measure integrative and instrumental orientation, via an AM Index. The former reflects a sincere interest in the target language culture and a desire to interact with the members of that community, while the latter emphasizes the pragmatic gains or practical advantages, such as getting a job with a higher salary. Students with dormant French were used deliberately, in one particular study (Gardner and MacIntyre, 1991), as explained:

The reason for using subjects who had not studied French for some time was not taken lightly. Given the materials to be learned, it was known on the basis of previous research (Gardner, Lalonde, & Moorcroft, 1985) that such individuals would not know any of the vocabulary items at the outset of the study, and that consequently the rate of learning would reflect factors operating in the learning task itself as opposed to transfer from previous knowledge. Since the major focus was on the effects that attitudes and motivation, on the one hand, and monetary rewards, on the other, had on the French vocabulary learning (and other relevant behaviors), such control seems mandatory. MacIntyre, 1991:60) (Gardner and

Instrumental motivation of a $10 reward was offered randomly to half of the subjects if they achieved a superior level of success in the learning task (Gardner and MacIntyre, 1991:59). Timed responses to eight different attitudinal/motivational characteristics (subscales) were obtained using a microcomputer (see Appendix A). Subjects were subsequently given six attempts to learn 26 English/French word pairs using an anticipation method. Results were presented by a series of graphs plotting the two prescribed types of motivation against performance and a correlation table using the subscales. Conclusions were drawn that instrumentally motivated students, where there was an opportunity to profit, studied longer than those who were not. In addition, there was an indication that a reaction time index might provide a method of identifying social desirability (Gardner and MacIntyre, 1991:57).

Gardner (1985:169) had previously stated that the source of the motivating impetus is relatively unimportant, provided that motivation is aroused. However, the source of this motivation is important for practising teachers who need to stimulate motivation. Researchers have claimed that both integrative and instrumental orientations play a part in performance and that the results could be generalized to actual language acquisition. These orientations, however, do not adequately summarise all possible reasons, as pointed out by Oxford and Shearin (1994) when highlighting the (1991) case of 218 American high school students who were asked to write an essay explaining their motivation for studying Japanese: 2

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

more than two-thirds of the teenagers also had additional [non-orientational] reasons for learning Japanese These reasons included: receiving intellectual stimulation, seeking personal challenge, enjoying the elitism of taking a difficult language, showing off to friends, developing greater cultural tolerance through language study, aiding world peace, satisfying curiosity about cultural "secrets," pursuing a fascination with Japanese writing systems, and having a private code that parents would not know. (Oxford and Shearin, 1994:12)

In 1991, Crookes and Schmidt, amongst others, called for a new approach, which heralded what has retrospectively been described as the cognitive-situated period (Drnyei and Ushioda 2011:46): From a conceptual point of view, much of the work on motivation in SL learning has not dealt with motivation at all. Consequently, we have adopted here a definition of motivation in terms of choice, engagement, and persistence, as determined by interest, relevance, expectancy, and outcomes. We suggest that this will [provide] a more satisfactory connection to language-learning processes and language pedagogy. the role of motivation ought to be general and not restricted to particular contexts or groups. In brief, we seek to encourage a program of research that will develop from, and be congruent with the concept of motivation that teachers are convinced is critical for SL success. Crookes and Schmidt (1991:502) There followed a significant shift towards adopting a more pragmatic, education-centred approach to motivation research which would be consistent with the perceptions of practising teachers and, thus, more directly relevant to classroom application (Drnyei and Csizr, 1998:204). Some research (e.g. Julkunen, 1989) hypothesized that situation-specific factors closely related to the classroom reality played a greater role in L2 motivation. How to operationalize this complexity was a question posed by Clement, Drnyei and Noels (1994). The hypothesis was tested amongst Hungarian EFL learners who were somewhat isolated from the target language culture (Clement et al, 1994:423). Building on social psychological constructs, 301 students from Budapest answered a questionnaire assessing their attitude, anxiety and motivation towards learning English and their perception of classroom atmosphere and cohesion (see Appendix B). To operationalize the

complexity of classroom environment, the researchers drew upon group dynamics. One concept central to this is group cohesion, or the strength of the relationship linking the members to one another and to the group itself (Forsyth, 1990:10 in Clement et al, 1994:424) . Questionnaires were given to students, which included an assessment of their teachers, and vice-versa. Results

confirmed earlier research towards integrative motivation and linguistic self-confidence, but the 3

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

classroom-specific component was novel and provided support for a pedagogical extension to research (Drnyei and Csizr, 1988:205). Further questionnaire survey research has looked at motivational strategies within the classroom, by questioning both the beliefs and practices of 200 Hungarian teachers of English (Drnyei and Csizr, 1998). The main purpose was to draw up a set of motivational macrostrategies to which teachers could pay special attention when trying to implement a motivation-conscious teaching approach (Drnyei, 1998:131). This was done by focusing on the importance and frequency of each strategy. A pilot study, with 20 respondents, was first carried out, with modifications made to the questions, until 51 motivational strategy items were grouped into clusters and internal consistency of these scales was verified by means of reliability analysis (ibid:213). By standardising the

importance and frequency scores a relative frequency index was created, therefore determining underuse or overuse. The final rank order (see Appendix C), including ten multi-item scales and eight individual item strategies revealed which ten variables would form the basis of ten commandments for motivating language learners (Appendix D). Quantitative methods still abound but have not remained unchanged. Examples of these are

correlational studies (e.g. Drnyei and Kormos, 2000), factor analytical studies (e.g. Chen, Warden and Huo-Tsan Chang, 2005) and experimental studies (e.g. Inbar et al, 2001). More significantly, according to Drnyei (2001/2011), the relatively recent structural equation modelling (SEM) (e.g. Wen-Ta Tseng and Schmitt, 2008) has been used to interpret large, multivariate datasets. It is appropriate for testing grand theories, that is, comprehensive models made up of a number of complex, interrelated variables (Drnyei and Ushioda, 2011:227). QUALITATIVE APPROACH Whereas quantitative research is relatively straightforward to define, qualitative research has no theory or paradigm that is distinctly its own. Nor does qualitative research have a distinct set of methods or practices that are entirely its own. A wide range of seemingly unconnected methods are used to get a better fix on the subject matter in hand (Denzin and Lincoln, 1994:2 and in Drnyei, 2007:35). Furthermore: [It] is not simply a question of methodology. It is defined by the nature of its theoretical focus and empirical purpose. The value of the approach lies in its potential to cast a different light on the phenomena under investigation and to raise a different set of issues *for SLA+ the issue is not whether qualitative research methods may yield to support or undermine traditional and current theoretical approaches in the quantitative paradigm. The purpose is rather to analyse and 4

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

explore aspects of motivation that are not easily accommodated within the dominant research paradigm. In essence, a qualitative approach is underpinned by a fundamentally different concept of motivation itself. (Ushioda, in Drnyei and Schmidt, 2001:96) I will begin a look at more qualitative studies by raising a concern made by Oxford and Shearin (1994) that they: [had rarely] seen an L2 teacher administer a motivation survey or discuss students goals still less often have we found teachers tracking the changes in students motivations over several years. (Oxford and Shearin, 1994:16) One longitudinal study (Nikolov, 1999) which addressed this concern, explored the attitudes and motivations of young Hungarian learners aged between 6 and 14, using the teachers direct involvement. The study was triggered by classroom needs juxtaposed with her own lack of

theoretical knowledge (ibid:39). A total of 84 children were involved in three separate eight year periods, with 45 included for the full term. A short questionnaire consisting of six open-ended questions were asked in their native tongue, based upon reasons for learning English, its importance compared with other subjects, likes and dislikes about learning and suggestions for the teacher. This questionnaire was administered each year, followed by a discussion about the responses. Answers were tabulated by theme with comparison across three different age ranges. Motivation was found to be situation-specific, for example, attitudes towards the learning context, the teacher, tasks and materials. These were deemed to be stronger than integrative or instrumental motives. Knowledge overtook the role of extrinsic factors like rewards and approval (Drnyei and Ushioda, 2011:210). The way in which motivational processes happen in time was first highlighted, amongst others, by Williams and Burden (1997). They argued for a continuum consisting of initiating and then sustaining motivation: It is important to emphasise here that motivation is more simply than arousing interest. It also involves sustaining interest and investing time and energy into putting the necessary effort to achieve certain goals. We make this point because so often, from a teachers point of view, motivation is seen as simply sparking an initial interest, for example, presenting an interesting language activity. However, motivating learners entails far more than this (Williams and Burden, 1997:121 in Drnyei and Ushioda, 2011:61). During this process-oriented period, Williams and Burden (for example, 1999) also drew upon psychological attribution theory for their own research. Attributional processes form one of the most important influences on the formation of peoples expectancies and their investigation was the 5

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

dominant model in research on student motivation in the 1980s (Drnyei and Ushioda, 2011:15). The small-scale study focused on an investigation into the development of British schoolchildrens attributions for their perceived success or failure in learning French. Semi-structured interviews

were conducted across four school year groups, in a relaxed manner, expressing confidentiality (Williams and Burden, 1999:196). Using a grounded theory approach (Strauss and Corbin, 1990), the researchers allowed the data to suggest natural categories. The results: indicated that most of these learners tended to judge their success by external factors such as teacher approval, marks, or grades, and that the range of attributions increased with age. Many of the attributions mentioned, however, were superficial in nature. It appears that the teacher plays a significant role in the development of students attributions. Implications [were] drawn with regard to language teaching and to the nature of the learning environment (Williams and Burden, 1999:193).

Effort became increasingly identified as the main reason for success while, in contrast, reasons cited for failure included distraction by others, difficulty of work, and poor teaching, as well as lack of concentration (see Appendix E). I now wish to turn to one of the main early advocates for qualitative research. Ushioda (2001) provided a key longitudinal work which examined the temporal dimension of motivation. The study, actually conducted in two separate stages in 1991 and 1993, used a manageable sample of 20 Irish university undergraduates studying French. It sought to provide insights into the learners own working conceptions of their motivation and their perspectives on motivational evolution over time (ibid:93). For institutionalised language learning especially, Ushioda (1996) states: the common experience would seem to be motivation flux rather than stability yet the potential for developing a dynamic theory of L2 motivation would seem to extend beyond the phenomenon of motivational loss or growth alone. In this respect a more introspective type of research approach is needed to explore qualitative developments in motivational experience over time, as well as to identify the contextual factors perceived to be in dynamic interplay with motivation (Ushioda, 1996:240). Rather than priming respondents with motivational concepts, that might unduly influence responses, a loosely structured interview technique was employed to draw out introspective data. Despite the dangers of the elicited data varying considerably (ibid:98), the first stage resulted in the emergence of eight descriptive dimensions (ibid:102-4; see Appendix F). These dimensions were arrived at, not by factor analytic reduction, as quantitative data might provide, but rather to put 6

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

some kind of collective shape on the introspective data (ibid:102). Subjects were prompted to consider other factors which they had previously not mentioned (ibid:104) before a motivational profile for each subject was generated. Having collated these, four motivational dimensions, in particular, were emphasized. A positive learning history and enjoyment of the French language scored over future goal-orientation and a desired level of L2 competence, pointing to intrinsic factors rating higher than extrinsic goals. The second stage followed a semi-structured interview format, with 14 of the original 20 subjects, taking four aspects of motivation as a dynamic phenomenon: 1. Motivational evolution over time (5 questions) 2. Motivational perspectives on L2 development over time (2 questions) 3. Factors negatively affecting L2 motivation (1 question) 4. Motivational strategies (1 question) (Ushioda, 2001: 99-100; 109) For each aspect, subjects responses to the open-ended questions posed were summarized in note form. These were compared and common underlying patterns found. Qualitative developments, not possible under more rigid, statistically-based procedures, were found to include individual or personal reasons. Examples would be positive or negative experiences from visiting France, success in exams leading to greater perception of achievement and the dominance of exam-oriented motivation at the expense of intrinsic motivation (ibid:110). Interestingly, demotivating factors were found to be, almost exclusively related to negative aspects of the institutionalized learning context (ibid:114).

MIXED METHODS Having looked very briefly at some predominantly quantitative and qualitative approaches, I now wish to turn to a mixed method approach, which combines elements of both. The purpose is threefold: There are two main and somewhat conflicting purposes (a) to achieve a fuller understanding of a target phenomenon and (b) to verify one set of findings against the other the traditional goal of triangulation, namely to validate ones conclusions by presenting converging results obtained through different methods. [but] we can add a very practical third purpose to reach audiences that would not be sympathetic to one of the approaches if applied alone (Drnyei, 2007, partly quoting Sandelowski, 2003). 7

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

One procedure involves a questionnaire survey with a follow-up interview. An example of this was Lambs (2004) research into the motivation of Indonesian junior high schoolchildren, aged 11-12 years. The investigation combined a survey of 219 students with semi-structured interviews with a focal group of 12 of these. Interviews were also conducted with eight teachers at the school. The questionnaire contained eight items to elicit background information, while the remainder of items sought to uncover attitudes and motivations towards learning English. A simple single-item, three-

point Likert scale was used to rank order their responses (see Appendix G). There were also three open-ended questions. From the resultant discovered disposition and behaviour, a selection of a broadly representative sample formed the focal group, which were subjected to in-depth interviews exploring feelings, activities involving English usage outside the classroom and attitude towards English-speaking peoples and cultures (Lamb, 2004:7 - Appendix H). Very high levels of motivation were found, including integrative and instrumental orientations, but these two constructs were broadly indistinguishable, raising questions about the meaning of integrative orientation in this context (Drnyei and Ushioda, 2011:244). As Lamb stated: English is no longer associated just with Anglophone countries. Moreover, we have seen that an integrative and instrumental orientation are difficult to distinguish as separate concepts. Meetings with westerners, using computers, understanding pop songs, studying or travelling abroad, pursuing a desirable career all these aspirations are associated with each other and with English as an integral part of the globalization processes that are transforming their society and will profoundly affect their own lives (Lamb:2004:14-15). Another example is the pioneering study of Egbert (2003), using the concept of flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990); an experiential state characterized by intense focus and involvement that leads to improved performance on a task. Flow can be seen as a heightened level of motivated task engagement, or optimal experience (Drnyei and Ushioda, 2011:94-5), which had not previously been researched in respect of SLA. Egbert found that: The task conditions under which flow occurs can be organised along four dimensions: (1) there is a perceived balance of task challenge and participant skills during the task, (2) the task offers opportunities for intense concentration and the participants attention is focused on the pursuit of clear task goals, (3) the participants find the task intrinsically interesting or authentic, and (4) the participants perceive a sense of control over the task process and outcomes. These underlying dimensions display a balanced mixture of motivational, cognitive and affective constituents. (Drnyei and Ushioda, 2011:95).

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

Egbert wanted to establish the foundation for a research stream addressing flow in language learning and to investigate whether flow exists in the L2 classroom. She focused on one class (13 students) of American teenagers studying Spanish. The researcher observed a number of tasks (see Appendix I) carried out within class. A task was defined as a goal-specific, meaningful and purposeful endeavour that is self-contained (Egbert, 2003:508, sic). First, participants answered questions about their personal and educational backgrounds and past language learning experiences. Past performances in Spanish were added from records. These were coded into a database. Second, a modified perceptions questionnaire, consisting of 14 items reflecting the four flow dimensions, were answered on a 7-point Likert format. Some university students were used to pilot this. Third, observational data was acquired using checklist topics. Fourth, task products, such as transcripts, were collected for the purpose of examining actual task processes. Finally,

retrospective interviews involving stimulated recall were carried out. This crucial element allowed for further qualitative data to be elicited. Various operational techniques were used to garner participation, whilst flow theory was analysed thus: We independently coded mentions of the four flow dimensions in the interview data. We highlighted and categorized instance of control, focus, interest, and challenges or skills. We compiled the results and reconciled differences through discussion. Interview data for students in flow that indicated positive perceptions of any of the four flow dimensions served as confirmatory evidence that flow did occur in the classroom and as support for conclusions about task characteristics. Negative perceptions served to signal potential barriers to flow (Egbert, 2003: 510). The above mixed methods examples involve a survey followed by more exploratory interviews. An alternative approach is to carry out focus group interviews first, in order to narrow the focus. This is useful if developing an instrument (questionnaire), which can be subsequently tested via a pilotstudy (e.g. Tseng et al, 2006). There are several other complex combinations (see Drnyei,

2007:169-173) which can be very beneficial in conducting research, but they require sufficient knowledge about method mixing and significant expertise in design (Drnyei, 2007:174). COMMENTARY The research methods discussed above cannot be said to represent all of the variable methods that have been applied in this field. For example, I have neglected self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 1985, in Drnyei, 2001) with its well-known distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. The latter would encapsulate both instrumental and integrative purposes. I have not discussed goal-setting (e.g. Locke and Latham, 1990) or goal-orientation theory. Nor have I

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

discussed task motivation (e.g. Drnyei and Kormos, 2000) or the conception of the Ideal L2 Self and Ought-to L2 Self (Drnyei in Drnyei and Ushioda, 2009:29). I have, however, given some examples, using methods permeated by ideological perspectives. Much of this focuses on the different approaches to individual diversity. While the quantitative approach aims to analyse individuals in general, using reductionist logic, the qualitative approach looks at specific, context-related, individual motivation. Statistical evidence is impossible to ignore because, as evidenced by mixed methods approaches, categories need to be formed, coding needs to be generated and the validity of information demonstrated. Generalisations based on variables, however, can be meaningless. As the examples above showed, qualitative techniques are far more appropriate in discovering the dynamic factors involved within the individual learner. Individual motivation can, for example, develop over time, increase through cumulative experiences or be adversely affected by peers or, has been found, institutions. We can learn more from a more sociodynamic approach, but this requires even greater radical changes to methodology. IMPLICATIONS FOR OWN RESEARCH Using the discussion so far, I will now draw some implications for how I might conduct research in my own ELT context. I feel persuaded towards carrying out a micro-study focusing on specific sociodynamic factors for motivation. My own personal strengths lie not in the accumulation of statistical data, but in detailed, socio-cultural explanations. I am interested by the principles of attribution theory. I am keen, therefore, to uncover meaningful causal attributions about past achievements. I am also interested by demotivating influences, and which factors, supposedly outside of the student or teachers control, affect positive achievement. As mentioned already, some research (e.g. Ushioda, 2001) has shown that demotivating aspects for students consist of negatives aspects of the institutionalised learning context (Drnyei and Ushioda, 2011:146). Subsequently, many students do not successfully develop autonomous learning, where motivation comes from within (Deci, 1996 and others). I am similarly interested in self-

determination theory and possibly exploring this through a cohort of students who are motivated not simply because of extrinsic pushes and pulls, but through an inner enjoyment of knowledge development. My own ELT context is not geographically or culturally defined. I have taught in two culturally diverse locations, Beijing and Riyadh, on foundation or preparatory year courses for prospective university students. As I am unlikely to return to either setting, I can hypothetically refer to my

context as being L2 learners in similar tertiary educational settings. Any qualitative analysis would 10

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

need, however, a more grounded understanding of location, otherwise I would be in danger of generalising. I could describe my context as the virtual classroom, one in which my learners motivation comes from an intrinsic enjoyment of using Information Communication Technology (ICT). Learners, who

are comfortable with using technology in everyday life, may suffer demotivation in traditional classroom settings when ICT is rarely used. Whilst various forms of technology (e.g. interactive

whiteboards) have been used in the traditional classroom for some time, these are often teacher aids which enable clear presentations of the learning topic. The scope of these and the kinds of technology brought to class by learners themselves (iPads, smartphones etc) bring numerous web 2.0 tool opportunities to engage, motivate and increase autonomous learning. One possibility would be to carry out some wholly online research, drawing upon attribution theory or self-determination theory, by conducting interviews via webinars. Student perceptions of

computer assisted learning or online homework feedback could also be researched, using one class over the duration of a course. Furthermore, an investigation into the motivation of L2 teachers to use web 2.0 technologies could build upon existing research into this area. RELFECTIVE WRITING I will now conclude by reflecting on my own thinking about second language learning/acquisition and how it has developed since I began this course. Despite having taught EFL for three cumulative years, I had no theoretical underpinning of SLA before I started. If I have developed any theoretical knowledge then these have been assumptions gained retrospectively. Having been a relatively

unsuccessful second language learner myself, I am interested how I attribute SLA with my own past failures. In addition, I am interested in how performance is affected by motivation and other affective factors, such as anxiety, and social-cultural barriers, such as those to critical thinking. I have seen at first-hand the frustrations of learners trying to meet the difficult demands of a syllabus forced upon them by profit and status-driven institutions. Often the perceptions and reality are wildly different. I have become interested in how individuals overcome their particular constraints, the sociallymediated process involved and how motivation, in particular, is part of a dynamic complex relationship, rather than something that can be simply fostered. My own motivation for doing the course was examined during one particular seminar. This reflective session was very useful,

especially for comparing responses with others. My motivation developed out of conversations with more experienced teachers that I have met over the last twelve months. In addition, I felt to 11

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

develop my skills (intrinsic) and to improve my job prospects for the future (extrinsic), I needed a further qualification (extrinsic). I also have a genuine interest in understanding the theoretical concepts behind what I do and, in specialising in ICT, an interest in how to keep students engaged by acknowledging and utilising an increasing amount of otherwise technological distractions. With this course, I am quickly building up a theoretical knowledge of the underlying factors for successful/unsuccessful language learning. I have become very interested in the areas of autonomy, self-motivation and learner-learner interaction, such as collaborative dialogues and peer assistance. Each of these areas is broadly connected, as the onus moves away from the teacher, as responsible for all areas of learning, and onto the students, who could and should be encouraged to become autonomous. The use of multimedia technologies inside and outside of the classroom will now be studied as I proceed with the MA course. This existing interest area can be allied with my growing interest in learner motivation and autonomy towards a dissertation proposal, which might look at current trends for stimulating second language acquisition.

Words: 3,257 (not including direct quotes, authors names and years in parenthesis)

12

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning REFERENCES:

Student I/D: 1163612

Chen, J., C.Warden and H-T Chang. 2005. Motivators that do not motivate: The case of Chinese EFL learners and the influence of culture or motivation. TESOL Quarterly 39/4: 609-33 Cheng, H-F. and Z. Drnyei. 2007. The use of motivational strategies in language instruction: The case of EFL teaching in Taiwan. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 1/1: 153-174. Clement, R., Z. Drnyei and K.Noels. 1994. Motivation, Self-Confidence and Group Cohesion in the Foreign Language Classroom. Language Learning 44/3:417-448. Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1990. Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. In Drnyei, Z., and E.Ushioda. 2011. Teaching and researching motivation (2nd ed.) Harlow: Longman. Deci, E. (with R.Flaste). 1996. Why We Do What We Do: Understanding Self-Motivation. New York: Penguin. Denzin, N. and Y.Lincoln. 1994. Introduction: Entering the field of qualitative research. In Denzin, N. and Y.Lincoln (eds.) Handbook of Qualitative Research. London: Sage Drnyei, Z. 1994. Motivation and motivating in the foreign language classroom. Modern Language Journal 78/3: 273-284. Drnyei, Z. 1998. Motivation in second and foreign language learning. Language Teaching. 31:117135. Drnyei, Z. 2001. New Themes and Approaches in Second Language Motivation Research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics. 21:43-59 Drnyei, Z. 2007. Research Methods In Applied Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Drnyei, Z. 2008. Researching L2 Motivation: Towards Combined Quantitative-Qualitative Paradigms. Paper presented at the 11th Warwick Postgraduate Conference, University of Warwick, UK, 18 June. Drnyei, Z. and K. Csizr. 1988. Ten Commandments for motivating language learners: results of an empirical study. Language Teaching Research 2(3): 203-29 Drnyei, Z. and J. Kormos. 2000. The role of individual and social variables in oral task performance Language Teaching Research July 2000 4: 275-300 Drnyei, Z., K. Csizr and N.Nmeth. 2006. Motivation, language attitudes and globalisation: A Hungarian perspective. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters. Drnyei, Z. and R.Schmidt (eds.) 2001. Motivation and Second Language Acquisition. Honolulu : Second Language Teaching & Curriculum Center, University of Hawai'i at Manoa Drnyei, Z. and E.Ushioda (eds). 2009. Motivation, Language Identity and the L2 Self. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. Drnyei, Z., and E.Ushioda. 2011. Teaching and researching motivation (2nd ed.) Harlow: Longman. 13

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

Egbert, J. 2003. A Study of Flow Theory in the Foreign Language Classroom. The Modern Language Journal. 83/4: 499-518 Gardner, R. and P.MacIntyre. 1991. An Instrumental Motivation in Language Study Who says it isnt effective? Studies in Second Language Acquisition 13:57-72. Gardner, R., R.Lalonde and R Moorcroft. 1985. The role of attitudes and motivation in second language learning: Correlational and experimental considerations. Language Learning. 35:207-227. Inbar, O., S.Donitsa-Scmidt and E.Shohamy. 2001. Students Motivation as a Function of Language Learning: the teaching of Arabic in Israel in Drnyei, Z. and R.Schmidt (eds.) 2001. Motivation and Second Language Acquisition. Honolulu Julkunen, K. 1989. Situation- and task-specific motivation in foreign-language learning and teaching. In Dornyei, Z. 1998. Motivation in second and foreign language learning. Language Teaching. 31:117135. Lamb, M. 2004. Integrative motivation in a globalizing world. System 32/1:3-19. Locke, E. and G.Latham. 1990. A Theory of Goal Setting and Task Performance in Drnyei, Z., and E.Ushioda. 2011. Teaching and researching motivation (2nd ed.) Harlow: Longman. Nikolov, M. 1999. Why do you learn English? Because the teacher is short. A study of Hungarian childrens foreign language motivation. Language Teaching Research 3:33-56. Oxford, R and J.Shearin. 1994. Language Learning Motivation: Expanding the Theoretical Framework. The Modern Language Journal. 78/1:12-28. Sandelowski, M. 2003. Tables of tableaux? The Challenges of writing and reading mixed methods studies. In Drnyei, Z. 2007. Research Methods In Applied Linguistics. Oxford: OUP. Strauss, A., and J.Corbin. 1990. Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory procedures and techniques. London: Sage. Tseng, W-T and N.Schmitt. 2008. Toward a model of motivated vocabulary learning: A structural question modelling approach. Language Learning 58/2: 357-400. Ushioda, E. 1996. Developing a dynamic concept of L2 motivation. In Hickey, T. and Williams, J. (eds) Language, Education and Society in a Changing World. Dublin/Clevedon: IRAAL/Multilingual Matters: 239-45. Ushioda, E. 2007. Motivation, autonomy and sociocultural theory. In P. Benson (ed.) Learner Autonomy 8: Teacher and Learner Perspectives (pp5-24). Dublin: Authentik. Ushioda, E. 2008. Motivation and good language learners. In C.Griffiths (ed.) Lessons from Good Language Learners (pp.19-34). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Williams, M and R.Burden. 1999. Students Developing Conceptions of Themselves as Language Learners. The Modern Language Journal. 83/2: 193-201

14

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

Appendix A : Likert items for 8 different attitudinal/motivational characteristics, as used in Gardner and MacIntyres (1991) study.

15

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

Appendix B: Student Questionnaires from a quantitative approach - Clement, Drnyei and Noels (1994):

16

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

Appendix C: Final rank order, top 5 clusters (top 17 motivational strategies only (Drnyei and Csizr, 1998)

Appendix D: (Drnyei and Csizr, 1998)

17

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

Appendix E: Williams and Burden (1999):

18

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning

Student I/D: 1163612

Appendix F: Classification of motivational features from interviews, 1991 - Ushioda (2001)

19

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning Appendix G: Questionnaire data obtained and summarised by Lamb (2004):

Student I/D: 1163612

20

ET990-2 Second Language Acquisition and Classroom Language Learning Appendix H: Interview responses extract, from Lamb (2004):

Student I/D: 1163612

Appendix I: Tasks observed by Egbert (2003):

21

You might also like