You are on page 1of 11

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Appetite 50 (2008) 111 www.elsevier.com/locate/appet

Research Review

How emotions affect eating: A ve-way model


Michael Macht
Department of Psychology, University of Wu rzburg, Marcusstr. 9-11, 97070 Wu rzburg, Germany Received 18 April 2006; received in revised form 13 July 2007; accepted 16 July 2007

Abstract Despite the importance of affective processes in eating behaviour, it remains difcult to predict how emotions affect eating. Emphasizing individual differences, previous research did not pay full attention to the twofold variability of emotion-induced changes of eating (variability across both individuals and emotions). By contrast, the present paper takes into account both individual characteristics and emotion features, and species ve classes of emotion-induced changes of eating: (1) emotional control of food choice, (2) emotional suppression of food intake, (3) impairment of cognitive eating controls, (4) eating to regulate emotions, and (5) emotioncongruent modulation of eating. These classes are distinguished by antecedent conditions, eating responses and mediating mechanisms. They point to basic functional principles underlying the relations between emotions and biologically based motives: interference, concomitance and regulation. Thus, emotion-induced changes of eating can be a result of interference of eating by emotions, a byproduct of emotions, and a consequence of regulatory processes (i.e., emotions may regulate eating, and eating may regulate emotions). r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Emotion; Eating; Mood; Emotional eating; Restrained eating; Motivation; Food choice

Contents Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Variability of emotion-induced changes of eating . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Variability across individuals . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Variability across emotions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Previous theories . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A ve-way model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Food-induced emotions control food choice . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Intense emotions suppress food intake . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Negative and positive emotions impair cognitive eating controls . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Negative emotions elicit eating to regulate emotions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Emotions modulate eating in congruence with emotion features . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Emotion-induced eating in restrained and emotional eaters. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Emotion-induced eating in normal eaters . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 2 3 4 4 5 5 6 6 7 7 7 8 8

Introduction Emotions serve distinct motivational functions and contribute essentially to control of basic behavioural

E-mail address: macht@psychologie.uni-wuerzburg.de 0195-6663/$ - see front matter r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.002

ARTICLE IN PRESS
2 M. Macht / Appetite 50 (2008) 111

systems in animals and men (e.g. Frederickson, 1998; Frijda, 1986; Izard & Ackerman, 2000; Plutchik, 1984; Tomkins, 1963). Specic emotions such as anger, fear, sadness, and joy, as well as moods that are thought to last longer and be more diffuse (e.g. Ekman, 1992; Frijda, 1993) have been found to affect eating responses along the entire process of ingestion: motivation to eat (Macht & Simons, 2000), affective responses to foods (Ferber & Cabanac, 1987; Willner & Healy, 1994), food choice (e.g. Gibson, 2006; Oliver & Wardle, 1999), chewing (Macht, 1998), eating speed (Krebs, Macht, Weyers, Weijers, & Janke, 1996), amount ingested (Greeno & Wing, 1994 for a review), as well as metabolism and digestion (Blair, Wing, & Wald, 1991; Wing, Blair, Epstein, & McDermott, 1990). Surveys demonstrate that most people experience changes of eating in response to emotional stress (on average 30% an increase and 48% a decrease of appetite or intake, Table 1). Epidemiological data indicate that stress-related eating is associated with increased body weight (Laitinen, Ek, & Sovio, 2002). Not surprisingly, effects of emotions on eating have been studied extensively, but due to their variability it remains difcult to predict how an emotion affects eating in a given group of persons. Emotions can increase food intake in one group of persons, e.g. restrained eaters, but decrease food intake in another group, e.g. non-restrained eaters. Furthermore, different emotions may increase or decrease eating in the same group of individuals. For example, boredom may be associated with increased appetite, but sadness with decreased appetite (e.g. Pudel & Richter, 1980). This twofold variability (across individuals and emotions) has been neglected by previous research. Based on an individual difference model, it was mostly examined whether eating habits predispose people to increase food intake in response to negative emotions (Greeno & Wing, 1994). The present paper introduces an integrative model that takes into account both individual characteristics and emotion features. First, the empirical evidence on varia-

bility of emotion-induced changes of eating is summarized briey. Variability of emotion-induced changes of eating Variability across individuals A series of experiments showed that restrained eaters (as identied by questionnaire; e.g. Herman & Mack, 1975; Stunkard & Messick, 1985) consume more food than nonrestrained eaters in response to fear and negative mood states (for a review, see Greeno & Wing, 1994, also Heatherton, Striepe, & Wittenberg, 1998; Rotenberg & Flood, 1999; Rutledge & Linden, 1998; Tuschen, Florin, & Baucke, 1993). Restrained eating refers to a persistent pattern of eating-related cognitions and behaviours in order to reduce or to maintain body weight (Herman & Mack, 1975). Another experiment demonstrated that emotional eaters (also identied by questionnaire; van Strien, Frijters, Bergers, & Defares, 1986) consume more sweet, high-fat foods in response to emotional stress than non-emotional eaters (Oliver, Wardle & Gibson, 2000). Field studies showed that negative emotional states in everyday life can be associated with a tendency to eat as an emotion regulation strategy (Macht, Haupt, & Ellgring, 2005; Macht & Simons, 2000). Emotional eating is thought to be instigated in order to cope with negative emotions (Bruch, 1973; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1957). Probably, the inuences of both eating habits are reected in effects of emotions on binge eating. Negative emotions have been found to increase the tendency to binge in bulimia nervosa and the binge eating disorder (experiments: Agras & Telch, 1998; Cattanach, Malley, & Rodin, 1988; Chua, Touyz, & Hill, 2004; Gluck, Geliebter, Hung, & Yahav, 2004; Telch & Agras, 1996; eld studies: Alpers & Tuschen-Cafer, 2001; Cooper & Bowskill, 1986; Davis, Freeman, & Garner, 1988; Johnson & Larson, 1982; Kenardy, Arnow, & Agras, 1996). Taken together, studies indicate that restrained eaters increase food intake, emotional eaters consume more sweet, high-fat foods, and binge eaters tend to binge in response to negative emotions. By contrast, normal eaters (i.e. normal-weight persons whose emotional and restrained eating scores fall within the normal range) are usually assumed to reduce food intake in response to negative emotions (Herman & Polivy, 1984; Schachter, Goldman, & Gordon, 1968). The evidence does not support this assumption. Table 2 summarizes experimental studies in normal-eating populations: 43% of the results indicate an increase, 39% a decrease and 26% no change of eating in response to emotions (as shown by Bellisle et al. (1990), in the latter studies possibly one part of the sample reduced and the other part increased food intake). Similarly, surveys and eld studies showed that stress and negative emotions can be associated with both increased and decreased motivation to eat and food intake

Table 1 Changes of eating in response to emotional stress reported in surveys Change of eating 11% more appetite, 70% less appetite 16% more appetite, 38% less appetite 25% more appetite, 32% less appetite 44% eat more, 48% eat less 4% eat more, 55% eat less 49% eat more, 51 do not eat more 55% eat more, 45% do not eat more 38% eat more, 42% eat less N 364 1950 1024 80 475 500 101 212 Reference Krumbacher and Meyer (1963) Pudel and Richter (1980) Pudel (1984) Willenbring et al. (1986) Popper et al. (1989) Spillman (1990) Weinstein et al. (1997) Oliver and Wardle (1999)

ARTICLE IN PRESS
M. Macht / Appetite 50 (2008) 111 Table 2 Effects of emotions on eating: experimental studies in normal subjects Effect Increase Eating measure Food intake Emotion Negative mood Negative mood Fear Fear Fear Arousal Arousal Arousal Boredom Anger Arousal/stress Joy Joy Anger Negative mood Negative mood Fear Fear Fear Fear Fear Arousal Depressive mood Sadness Sadness Arousal Arousal Emotion Negative mood Negative mood Negative mood Negative mood Fear Fear Fear Fear Reference Lowe and Maycock (1988) Willner et al. (1998) Pine (1985) Pines and Gal (1977) Bellisle et al. (1990) Glass (1967) Cantor et al. (1982) Grunberg and Straub (1992) Abramson and Stinson (1977) Macht (1996) Ferber and Cabanac (1987) Macht et al. (2002) Macht et al. (2002) Macht (1996) Willner et al. (1998) Baucom and Aiken (1981) Schachter et al. (1968) Herman and Polivy (1975) Heatherton et al. (1991) Herman et al. (1987) Bellisle et al. (1990) Grunberg and Straub (1992) Willner and Healy (1994) Macht et al. (2002) Macht et al. (2002) Macht (1998) Macht (1998) Reference Frost et al. (1982) Tuschen et al. (1993) Telch and Agras (1996) Ruderman (1985) Reznick and Balch (1977) Heatherton et al. (1991) Schotte et al. (1990) Cools et al. (1992) 3

Hedonic responses Motivation to eat Chocolate craving Food intake

Decrease

Hedonic responses Motivation to eat Chewing Eating measure Food intake

Effect No effect

(Table 1; de Castro, 1986; Hill, Weaver, & Blundell, 1991; Macdiarmid & Hetherington, 1995; McCann, Warnick, & Knopp, 1990; Michaud et al., 1990; Pollard, Steptoe, Canaan, Davies, & Wardle, 1995; Roseneld & Stevenson, 1988; Slochower, 1983; Steptoe, Lipsey, & Wardle, 1998; Stone & Brownell, 1994; Striegel-Moore et al., 1999; Wardle, Steptoe, Oliver, & Lipsey, 2000; Weidner, Kohlmann, Dotzauer, & Burns, 1996). Based on this evidence, it is difcult to predict how normal eaters change eating in response to emotions. Variability across emotions Dimensions used to classify emotions, namely valence (pleasuredispleasure), arousal (e.g. Greenwald, Cook, & Lang, 1989; Russell & Feldman Barrett, 1999) and intensity (Frijda, Ortony, Sonnemans, & Clore, 1992), have been found to make an essential contribution to variability of emotion-induced changes of eating. There is clear evidence that emotions differing in valence and arousal or intensity exert different inuences on eating.

Animal studies point to a role of intensity for the direction of effects of emotional stress on food intake. Rats decrease food intake during intense noise (Alario, Gama , 1964), in response to llo, Beato, & Trancho, 1987; Pare intense electric shock (Strongman, 1965; Weiss, 1968), and during chronic stress (Sampson, Muscat, Philips, & Willner, 1992; Willner, Muscat, & Papp, 1992), but increase food intake or eating speed in response to low or moderate noise and electric shock (Krebs et al., 1996; Kupferman, 1964; Rasbury & Shemberg, 1971; Sterritt, 1962, 1965; Sterritt & Shemberg, 1963; Strongman, Coles, Remington, & Wookey, 1970; Ullman, 1951, 1952). Results of a human study correspond to these data (Mehrabian, 1980). Participants imagined themselves in various emotions and reported how much they would eat during each of these emotions. Compared with low-arousal states (e.g. boredom, depressive mood) high-arousal states (e.g. tension, fear) were reported to inhibit food intake. These results support the view that high-arousal or intense emotions decrease food intake whereas low- to moderate emotions may increase food intake (Robbins & Fray, 1980).

ARTICLE IN PRESS
4 Table 3 Previous models of emotion-induced changes of eating Type of model Psychological models Explained phenomenon Increased food intake in response to emotions dto. Increased food intake in response to negative emotions Binge eating Physiological models Decreased food intake in response to stress and fear Decreased hedonic reactions during chronic mild stress Eating induced by tail-pinch Increased eating speed in a stressful environment Eating bouts in response to arousal and frustration Mediating mechanism Disinhibition of restrained eating Impairment of cognitive capacity Reduction of negative emotions by eating Escape from aversive self-awareness Antagonistic physiological stress responses Reduced brain dopamine Activation of endogenous opiates Disposition to ensure energy supply Scope Restrained eaters Restrained eaters Emotional eaters Reference Herman and Polivy (1984) Boon et al. (2002) Bruch (1973), Slochower (1983), Booth (1994) Heatherton and Baumeister (1991) Schachter et al. (1968) Willner et al. (1992) Morley et al. (1983) Lima (1987), Whishaw et al. (1992) Cantor (1981) M. Macht / Appetite 50 (2008) 111

Restrained eaters, bulimics Animals, humans Animals, humans Rats Animals

Biological models

Reduction of arousal through eating

Animal, humans

Human studies demonstrated that positive and negative emotions could differ markedly in their effects on eating. Negative emotions such as anger, fear, and sadness may increase impulsive eating (i.e. fast and irregular eating directed at any food available), eating to regulate the emotional state and consumption of junk food, but decrease food pleasantness. By contrast, joy and other positive emotions increase food pleasantness and consumption of healthy foods (Lyman, 1982; Macht, 1999; Macht, Roth, & Ellgring, 2002). To summarize, high-arousal or intense emotions suppress eating, and negative emotions can increase or decrease food intake. Little is known about effects of positive emotions on eating and about differences between negative emotions such as anger, sadness and fear in their effects on eating. Clinical studies indicate that anger is an important antecedent of binge eating (Arnow, Kenardy, & Agras, 1992). It has also been suggested that disgust contributes to the maintenance of pathological eating patterns and may complicate attempts to normalize eating (Troop, Treasure, & Serpell, 2002). Previous theories During the past decades, a number of theories have attempted to explain the manifold effects of emotions on eating. Whereas emotion-induced increase of food intake was explained by psychological mechanisms (Bruch, 1969, 1973; Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991; Herman & Polivy, 1984; Slochower, 1983), physiological mechanisms were proposed to explain emotion-induced decrease of intake (Herman & Polivy, 1984; Schachter et al., 1968; Willner, 1997). Biologically oriented theories emphasized the adaptive function of changes of feeding behaviour in stressful situations (Cantor, 1981; Lima, 1987; Whishaw, Dringenberg, & Comery, 1992). As can be seen from

Table 3, previous theories aim at different phenomena, propose different mediating mechanisms and differ in scope. It has been a major problem for research that theories were mostly treated separately. Therefore, a general framework is provided in the following. It is suggested that emotions affect eating in ve basic ways, which encompass phenomena addressed by previous theories. A ve-way model As shown above, a number of factors were identied that make an essential contribution to emotion-induced changes of eating: arousal/intensity, valence and food relatedness of emotions as well as restrained and emotional eating. Taking into account these factors as antecedents, effects of emotions on eating can be classied into ve basic classes: (1) Emotions aroused by food stimuli affect food choice. (2) Emotions high in arousal or intensity suppress eating due to incompatible emotional responses. (3) Emotions moderate in arousal or intensity affect eating depending on motivations to eat: (a) In restrained eating, negative and positive emotions enhance food intake due to impairment of cognitive control. (b) In emotional eating, negative emotions elicit the tendency to be regulated by eating and, as a consequence, enhance intake of sweet and high-fat foods. (c) In normal eating, emotions affect eating in congruence with their cognitive and motivational features. The ow diagram in Fig. 1 gives a visual presentation.

ARTICLE IN PRESS
M. Macht / Appetite 50 (2008) 111 5

Emotion

Food-induced emotion?

yes

Control of food choice

Preference Craving Avoidance

no

yes
Intense emotion? Suppression Decreased food intake

no
Restrained eating Eating habits related to emotions? Impairment of cognitive eating controls Increased food intake

yes
Eating to regulate emotions

no

Emotional eating

Increased intake of sweet, high-fat foods

Emotion features relevant to eating?

yes

Congruent modulation

Change of eating congruent with emotion features

no

No change of eating

Fig. 1. A ow diagram to predict basic classes of emotion-induced changes of eating.

Emotions of low arousal or intensity are not expected to affect eating. In the following, each class is described in detail. Food-induced emotions control food choice Food-induced emotions are powerful determinants of food choice (Galef, 1996; Martins & Pliner, 2005; Rozin & Schulkin, 1990). Tasting energy-dense food such as sugar and fat evokes positive affective responses that promote ingestion, whereas tasting bitter compounds, which are correlated with toxins, evokes negative affective responses that promote rejection (Rosenstein & Oster, 1988; Steiner, 1979). The biological basis of these affective reactions is demonstrated in homology across primate species (Steiner, Hawilo, Glaser, & Berridge, 2001). Notably, food-induced emotional responses encompass full-blown emotions, in particular, neophobia and disgust (Raudenbush & Frank, 1999; Rozin & Fallon, 1987). Appetitive food-related stimuli can elicit a strong desire to eat or craving that can be conceived as an additional variant of food-induced emotions. Such craving co-occurs with a number of autonomic responses (Nederkoorn,

Smulders, Havermans, & Jansen, 2004; Nederkoorn, Smulders, & Jansen, 2000) which vary across individuals and can contribute to binge eating (Jansen, 1998). A number of studies demonstrated heightened cue reactivity in restrained eaters, binge eaters and bulimics as compared to controls (Carter, Bulik, McIntosh, & Joyce, 2001; Federoff, Polivy, & Herman, 2003; Karhunen, Lappalainen, Tammela, Turpeinen, & Uusitupa, 1997; Staiger, Dawe, & McCarthy, 2000), although lack of differences between groups has also been reported (e.g. Klaijner, Herman, Polivy, & Chhabra, 1980; Overduin, Jansen, & Eilkes, 1997). It is important to recognize that in some persons by contingent pairing of emotions and eating, emotions may become food-related cues and thus elicit eating (Booth, 1994). Intense emotions suppress food intake Animal studies demonstrated that intense or chronic stress sharply decreases food intake (for reviews see Greeno & Wing, 1994; Robbins & Fray, 1980, Section Variability across emotions). Consequently, it was proposed that a decreased food intake is the natural response to stress

ARTICLE IN PRESS
6 M. Macht / Appetite 50 (2008) 111

(Herman & Polivy, 1984; Schachter et al., 1968). In fact, intense emotions are linked with both behaviours and physiological responses that interfere with eating. For example, intense sadness is associated with behavioural deactivation and withdrawal from the environment. Intense fear motivates ight and avoidance, and is also associated with autonomic responses inhibiting motivation to eat. Stress-induced physiological changes may interfere with digestion by delaying glucose absorption and gastrointestinal transit (Blair et al., 1991; Wing et al., 1990). Negative and positive emotions impair cognitive eating controls Restraint theory suggested that emotional stress disinhibits dietary restraint and therefore increases food intake (Herman & Polivy, 1984). In this view, negative emotions were thought to undermine restrained eaters ability to continue dieting, because they impose a more urgent concern (i.e., how to cope with the stressor) than even dieting and lead to eating as if the diet boundary had been knocked down (p. 152). The diet boundary was conceived as the dieters self-imposed quota for consumption on a given occasion (p. 149). Although this appeared to be a plausible explanation, the common experimental design comparing individuals with high or low scores on restraint scales was not appropriate to test putative mediating mechanisms (Greeno & Wing, 1994). An alternative explanation was suggested by Boon, Stroebe, Schut, and Jansen (1998). According to their limited capacity hypothesis, restrained eaters food intake is increased, if their cognitive capacity to maintain restricted food intake is limited by distraction (be it emotional or not). Processing of emotional stimuli requires attention, and since cognitive capacity is limited, cognitive control over eating may be impaired. The overeating response was attributed to ironic processes, i.e., the more people exert cognitive control, the higher their vulnerability to overeating will be. A number of experimental studies supported this assumption (Boon, Stroebe, Schut, & IJntema, 2002; Boon et al., 1998; Lattimore & Caswell, 2004; Lattimore & Maxwell, 2004; Vreugdenberg, Bryan, & Kemps, 2003; Wallis & Hetherington, 2004; Ward & Mann, 2000). Negative emotions elicit eating to regulate emotions Emotional eating theory suggested that certain individuals eat in order to cope with stress and are therefore at risk of becoming obese (Bruch, 1969, 1973; Kaplan, 1957; Slochower, 1983). As the German colloquialism Kummerspeck (literally: fat of sorrow) shows, it is a popular view that emotional eating causes obesity. Although this view proved to be over-simplistic (Greeno & Wing, 1994), evidence did emerge for a role of emotional eating in a subgroup of obese persons. Obese binge eaters experience an increased tendency to binge in negative mood (Agras &

Telch, 1998; Gluck et al., 2004) and can be treated successfully by improving their skills to regulate emotions (Telch, Agras, & Linehan, 2001; Wiser & Telch, 1999). Moreover, studies indicate that healthy, normal-weight persons regulate negative emotions by eating (Macht, 1999; Macht et al., 2005; Macht & Simons, 2000). Emotional eating theory originated from psychodynamic thinking, but can be related to a number of more recent approaches. The core assumption of the theory (i.e., that negative emotions induce eating and are, as a result, reduced) has been viewed from a learning perspective, with negative emotions as eliciting stimuli, eating as operant behaviour, and its consequence, the eating-induced reduction of negative emotions, as negative reinforcement (Booth, 1994). Regulation was also taken into account by biological approaches which conceptualized arousalinduced eating as adjunctive behaviour or displacement activity that reduces arousal (Cantor, 1981). Finally, eating has been viewed as a strategy to improve negative mood (Thayer, 1989, 2001), to mask stress (masking hypothesis; Polivy & Herman, 1999), or to escape from aversive self-awareness (escape theory; Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991). In spite of its popularity and plausibility, the concept of emotional eating does however face some unresolved problems. Above all, the underlying mechanisms are far from clear. Usually, nutrient-dependent physiological changes are proposed to mediate mood-elevating effects of eating. Carbohydrate-rich meals may lead to a postprandial improvement of mood (for reviews see Benton & Donohoe, 1999; Christensen, 1993, 1997; Gibson & Green, 2002) and also to reduced stress responses in emotionally labile subjects (Markus, Panhuysen, & Tuiten, 1998). These effects were explained by meal-induced increase of brain serotonin availability (Wurtman, 1982). However, relatively small proportions of protein in a meal may deteriorate the increase of tryptophan levels at the bloodbrain barrier and, thus, an increase of brain serotonin. Since most carbohydrate-rich meals in everyday life contain some protein, the ecological validity of the serotonin hypothesis can be questioned (Benton, 2002). Another physiological mechanism is suggested by animal data demonstrating that ingestion of high fat and carbohydrate comfort foods (Wansink, Cheney, & Chan, 2003) reduces activity of the hypothalamo-pituitary adrenal axis and dampens stress responses (Dallman et al., 2003). The main problem with both the serotonin and the endocrine hypothesis is that emotional effects of eating can occur only with delay. Such delayed effects might be useful for coping with chronic stress, but not for responding to negative emotions, as they arise from unpredictable stimuli such as daily hassles. In order to cope with these emotions, it would be most efcient to reduce their intensity immediately. Indeed, sweet solutions have been shown to rapidly calm stress responses in human newborns (Smith, Fillion, & Blass, 1990). Similarly, in adults, experimentally

ARTICLE IN PRESS
M. Macht / Appetite 50 (2008) 111 7

induced negative mood is improved immediately and selectively after eating palatable food, and this effect is most pronounced in persons scoring high on emotional eating (Macht & Mu ller, 2007). This suggests that immediate positive affective reactions elicited by palatable foods diminish the impact of stress. Additionally, it may be important here that eating distracts from the experience of negative emotions (Spitzer & Rodin, 1983). One might speculate that immediate effects of eating based on hedonistic mechanisms and distraction, play a major role in the regulation of everyday emotions and constitute a common phenomenon that applies to the general population. By contrast, in binge eating, immediate hedonic effects may be backed up or substituted by delayed and long-lasting physiological effects of chronic high fat and carbohydrate consumption. Emotions modulate eating in congruence with emotion features During negative mood, negative verbal information is retrieved more readily than positive information, and conversely, positive information is retrieved more readily during positive mood (for a review, see Berkowitz, 2000, pp. 7883). This congruency effect was also shown for recognition of palatable and unpalatable food avours (Pliner & Steverango, 1994) as well as for emotion-induced changes of eating. Sadness has been found to decrease and joy to increase food pleasantness and motivation to eat (Macht et al., 2002; Willner & Healy, 1994). These changes can be explained by emotion features. Sadness is associated with a slowing of cognitive processes and motor activity (Izard & Ackerman, 2000), with an absence of interest (Frijda, 1986, p. 22), and with a lowering of attention to the outside (Barr-Zisowitz, 2000). By contrast, joy is associated with an increased capability to perceive and process stimuli and an increased readiness to engage in activities (Frederickson, 1998; Frijda, 1986; Izard & Ackerman, 2000). Such emotion-congruent changes of eating can be conceived as a by-product of emotional activation, and should mostly occur, if emotions are elicited during eating. Discussion The present paper proposes ve basic classes of emotioninduced changes of eating that can be predicted by antecedent conditions and point to basic functional principles relating emotions and eating. Depending on emotion features and eating habits emotion-induced changes of eating can be a result of interference (suppression of food intake; impairment of cognitive eating controls), a by-product of emotions (emotion-congruent modulation of eating), and a consequence of regulatory processes (emotional control of food choice; eating to regulate emotions). Future research may examine whether these principles can be applied to relations between emotions and other biologically motivated behaviours

such as thirst, sex and aggression. To improve understanding of the manifold effects of emotions on eating research has to disentangle mediating mechanisms and to identify relations between classes of emotion-induced changes of eating. In particular, two problems remain to be resolved. Emotion-induced eating in restrained and emotional eaters Negative emotions increase food intake in restrained eaters as well as intake of sweet, high-fat food in emotional eaters (Section Variability across individual). To explain these ndings, it was suggested that emotional eaters eat to reduce negative emotions. By contrast, restrained eaters were thought to increase food intake, because emotions impair cognitive control over their restricted eating pattern (Sections Negative and positive emotions impair cognitive eating controls and Negative emotions elicit eating to regulate emotions). Although restrained and emotional eating appear to reect different neurobiological processes (Volkow et al., 2003), it remains unclear whether increased consumption in response to emotions is in fact mediated by different mechanisms in restrained and emotional eaters. The present model suggests two ways to test empirically the assumption of different mechanisms. First, emotional and restrained eaters should differ in their eating responses to various emotions and stressors. Provided that cognitive control of eating can be impaired by a variety of inuences, restrained eaters should increase food intake in response to both negative and positive emotions and also to cognitive demand. By contrast, emotional eaters should increase eating only in response to negative emotions. Second, emotioninduced eating should exert different effects on emotional state in emotional and restrained eaters. Consistent with the assumption that emotional eaters increase food intake to reduce negative emotions, they can be expected to report reduced negative emotions due to eating. By contrast, restrained eaters should experience no change of mood due to emotion-induced eating or may even report deterioration of mood (because of fear to gain weight). There is some evidence supporting these predictions. Restrained eaters have indeed been shown to increase food intake not only in response to negative emotions, but also to positive emotions (Cools, Schotte, & McNnally, 1992; Tuschen et al., 1993) and cognitive demand (e.g. Wallis & Hetherington, 2004); it remains, however, to be examined whether emotional eaters intake can also be increased by positive emotions and non-emotional stressors. Also, improvement of experimentally induced negative mood by eating has been found to be correlated with emotional, but not with restrained eating (Macht & Mu ller, 2007). In spite of this evidence, however, more studies comparing emotion-induced changes of eating in restrained and emotional eaters are needed.

ARTICLE IN PRESS
8 M. Macht / Appetite 50 (2008) 111

Emotion-induced eating in normal eaters In normal eaters, negative emotions decrease or increase food intake. The ve-way model explains decreased intake, depending on emotion intensity, by emotion-congruent modulation (Section Intense emotions suppress food intake) or emotional suppression of eating (Section Emotions modulate eating in congruence with emotion features), but it remains unclear how to explain increased intake in normal eaters. To understand this phenomenon, it should be recognized that emotional and restrained eating are regarded as continuous variables and that persons are classied as emotional or restrained eaters, if they score high on these continua. For example, emotional eaters may be dened as lying above the 80th percentile of scores in the most appropriate norm group (van Strien, 2002; personal communication). These persons can be expected to eat in order to regulate negative emotions frequently and consistently across situations. Presumably, a high proportion of their meals and snacks is based on the motivation to eat in order to cope with negative emotions. Persons with low scores of the emotional eating scale (e.g. below the 50th percentile) can be expected to experience this motivation, if at all, less frequently and less consistently. However, normal eaters with moderate emotional eating scores (e.g. between the 60th and 80th percentile) may occasionally also eat to cope with negative emotions. In these persons, a transient tendency to regulate emotions by eating may enhance consumption during emotions, although they are not classied as emotional eaters. It has indeed been demonstrated that normal eaters increase motivation to regulate emotions by eating in response to negative emotions induced in the laboratory (Bekker, van de Meerendonk, & Mollerus, 2004) and experienced in everyday life (Macht et al., 2005; Macht & Simons, 2000). Similarly, normal eaters may occasionally restrict eating, although they do not score very high on restraint scales, and thus enhance their susceptibility to emotion-induced eating. It can therefore be assumed that normal eaters increase intake due to transient shifts in motivations to eat. In other words, impairment of cognitive eating controls and emotion regulation eating (Sections Negative and positive emotions impair cognitive eating controls and Negative emotions elicit eating to regulate emotions) may occasionally also occur in normal eaters. Future research is needed to test the hypothesis that transient shifts in motivations to eat enhance susceptibility in normal eaters to increase intake in response to emotions. A suitable starting point for an experimental approach could be a study by Willner et al. (1998), who reinforced responses with chocolate under a progressively increasing work requirement and showed that craving for and intake of chocolate were increased by depressive mood. It remains to be examined whether the linking of operant responding for food and depressive mood can transiently increase motivation to eat in order to regulate mood in persons not classied as typical emotional eaters.

References
Abramson, E. E., & Stinson, S. G. (1977). Boredom and eating in obese and non-obese individuals. Addictive Behaviors, 2, 181185. Agras, W. S., & Telch, C. F. (1998). The effects of caloric deprivation and negative affect on binge eating in obese binge-eating disordered women. Behavior Therapy, 29, 491503. Alario, P., Gamallo, A., Beato, M. J., & Trancho, G. (1987). Body weight gain, food intake and adrenal development in chronic noise stressed rats. Physiology & Behavior, 40, 2932. Alpers, G. W., & Tuschen-Cafer, B. (2001). Negative feelings and the desire to eat in bulimia nervosa. Eating Behaviors, 2, 339352. Arnow, B., Kenardy, J., & Agras, W. S. (1992). Binge eating among the obese: A descriptive study. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 15, 155170. Barr-Zisowitz, C. (2000). SadnessIs there such a thing? In M. L. J. M. Haviland (Ed.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 607622). New York: Guilford Press. Baucom, D. H., & Aiken, P. A. (1981). Effect of depressed mood on eating: Among obese and nonobese dieting and nondieting persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 577585. Bekker, M. H. J., van de Meerendonk, C., & Mollerus, J. (2004). Effects of negative mood induction and impulsivity on self-perceived emotional eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 36, 461469. Bellisle, F., Louis-Sylvestre, J., Linet, N., Rocaboy, B., Dalle, B., Cheneau, F., et al. (1990). Anxiety and food intake in man. Psychosomatic Medicine, 52(4), 452457. Benton, D. (2002). Carbohydrate ingestion, blood glucose and mood. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review, 26, 293308. Benton, E., & Donohoe, R. T. (1999). The effects of nutrients on mood. Public Health Nutrition, 2, 403409. Berkowitz, L. (2000). Causes and consequences of feelings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Blair, E. H., Wing, R. R., & Wald, A. (1991). The effect of laboratory stressors on glycemic control and gastrointestinal transit time. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53, 133143. Boon, B., Stroebe, W., Schut, H., & IJntema, R. (2002). Ironic processes in the eating behaviour of restrained eaters. British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, 110. Boon, B., Stroebe, W., Schut, H., & Jansen, A. (1998). Food for thought: Cognitive regulation of food intake. British Journal of Health Psychology, 3, 2740. Booth, D. A. (1994). Psychology of nutrition. London: Taylor & Francis. Bruch, H. (1969). Hunger and instinct. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 149, 91114. Bruch, H. (1973). Eating disorders. Obesity and anorexia nervosa. Riverside: Behavior Science Book Service. Cantor, M. B. (1981). Bad habits: Models of induced ingestion in satiated rats and people. In S. Miller (Ed.), Behavior and nutrition. Franklin Institute Press. Cantor, M. B., Smith, S. E., & Bryan, B. R. (1982). Induced bad habits: Adjunctive ingestion and grooming in human subjects. Appetite, 3, 112. Carter, F. A., Bulik, C. M., McIntosh, V. V., & Joyce, P. R. (2001). Changes in cue reactivity following treatment for bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 336344. Cattanach, L., Malley, R., & Rodin, J. (1988). Psychologic and physiologic reactivity to stressors in eating disordered individuals. Psychosomatic Medicine, 50, 591599. Christensen, L. (1993). Effects of eating behavior on mood: A review of the literature. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14, 171183. Christensen, L. (1997). The effect of carbohydrates on affect. Nutrition, 13, 503514. Chua, J. L., Touyz, S., & Hill, A. J. (2004). Negative mood-induced overeating in obese binge eaters: An experimental study. International Journal of Obesity, 28, 606610.

ARTICLE IN PRESS
M. Macht / Appetite 50 (2008) 111 Cools, J., Schotte, D. E., & McNnally, R. J. (1992). Emotional arousal and overeating in restrained eaters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 348351. Cooper, P. J., & Bowskill, R. (1986). Dysphoric mood and overeating. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 25, 155156. Dallman, M. F., Pecoraro, N., Akana, S. F., la Fleur, S. E., Gomez, F., Houshyar, H., et al. (2003). Chronic stress and obesity: A new view of comfort food. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 1169611701. Davis, R., Freeman, R. J., & Garner, D. M. (1988). A naturalistic investigation of eating behavior in bulimia nervosa. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 273279. De Castro, J. M. (1986). Macronutrient relationships with meal patterns and mood in the spontaneous feeding behavior of humans. Physiology & Behavior, 39, 561569. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 6, 169200. Federoff, I., Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2003). The specicity of restrained vs. unrestrained eaters responses to food cues: General desire to eat, or craving for the cued food? Appetite, 41, 713. Ferber, C., & Cabanac, M. (1987). Inuence of noise on gustatory affective ratings and preference for sweet and salt. Appetite, 8, 229235. Frederickson, B. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300319. Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Frijda, N. (1993). Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions. In M. L. J. M. Haviland (Ed.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 381403). London, New York: Guilford Press. Frijda, N., Ortony, A., Sonnemans, J., & Clore, G. L. (1992). The complexity of intensityIssues concerning the structure of emotion intensity. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Emotion (pp. 6089). Newbury Park: Sage Publications. Frost, R. O., Goolkasian, G. A., Ely, R. J., & Blanchard, F. A. (1982). Depression, restraint and eating behavior. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 20, 113121. Galef, J. R. (1996). Food selection: Problems in understanding how we choose food to eat. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 20(1), 6773. Gibson, E. L. (2006). Emotional inuences on food choice: Sensory, physiological and psychological pathways. Physiology & Behavior, 89, 5361. Gibson, E. L., & Green, M. W. (2002). Nutritional inuences on cognitive function: Mechanisms of susceptibility. Nutrition Research Reviews, 15, 169206. Glass, L. B. (1967). The generality of oral-consummatory behavior of alcoholics under stress. Dissertation Abstracts International, 28, 5205. Gluck, M. E., Geliebter, A., Hung, J., & Yahav, E. (2004). Cortisol, hunger, and desire to binge eat following a cold stress test in obese women with binge eating disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 876881. Greeno, G. G., & Wing, R. R. (1994). Stress-induced eating. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 444464. Greenwald, M. K., Cook, E. W., & Lang, P. J. (1989). Affective judgement and psychophysiological response: Dimensional covariation in the evaluation of pictorial stimuli. Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, 5164. Grunberg, N. E., & Straub, R. O. (1992). The role of gender and taste class in the effects of stress on eating. Health Psychology, 11(2), 97100. Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 86108. Heatherton, T. F., Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1991). Effects of physical threat and ego threat on eating behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 138143. Heatherton, T. F., Striepe, M., & Wittenberg, L. (1998). Emotional distress and disinhibited eating: The role of self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(3), 301313. 9 Herman, C. P., & Mack, D. (1975). Restraint and unrestraint eating. Journal of Personality, 43, 647660. Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1975). Anxiety, restraint and eating behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84, 662672. Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1984). A boundary model for the regulation of eating. Eating and Its Disorders, 141156. Herman, C. P., Polivy, J., Lank, C. N., & Heatherton, T. F. (1987). Anxiety, hunger, and eating behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 264269. Hill, A. J., Weaver, C. F. L., & Blundell, J. E. (1991). Food craving, dietary restraint and mood. Appetite, 17, 187197. Izard, C. E., & Ackerman, B. P. (2000). Organizational and motivational functions of discrete emotions. In M. L. J. M. Haviland (Ed.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 253264). New York: Guilford Press. Jansen, A. (1998). A learning model of binge eating: Cue reactivity and cue exposure. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 257272. Johnson, C., & Larson, R. (1982). Bulimia: An analysis of mood and behavior. Psychosomatic Medicine, 44(4), 341351. Kaplan, H. I., & Kaplan, H. S. (1957). The psychosomatic concept of obesity. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 125, 181201. Karhunen, L. J., Lappalainen, R. I., Tammela, L., Turpeinen, A. K., & Uusitupa, M. I. J. (1997). Subjective and physiological cephalic phase responses to food in obese binge-eating women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 21, 321328. Kenardy, J., Arnow, B., & Agras, W. S. (1996). The aversiveness of specic emotional states associated with binge-eating in obese subjects. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 30(6), 839844. Klaijner, F., Herman, P. C., Polivy, J., & Chhabra, R. (1980). Human obesity, dieting, and anticipatory salivation to food. Physiology & Behavior, 27, 195198. Krebs, H., Macht, M., Weyers, P., Weijers, H.-G., & Janke, W. (1996). Effects of stressful noise on eating and non-eating behavior in rats. Appetite, 26, 193202. Krumbacher, K., & Meyer, J. E. (1963). Das Appetitverhalten des gesunden unter emotionalem StreX. Psychosomatische Medizin, 9, 8994. Kupferman, I. (1964). Eating behavior induced by sounds. Nature, 201, 324. Laitinen, J., Ek, E., & Sovio, U. (2002). Stress-related eating and drinking behavior and body mass index and predictors of this behavior. Preventive Medicine, 34, 2939. Lattimore, P., & Caswell, N. (2004). Differential effects of active and passive stress on food intake in restrained and unrestrained eaters. Appetite, 42, 167173. Lattimore, P., & Maxwell, L. (2004). Cognitive load, stress, and disinhibited eating. Eating Behaviors, 5, 315324. Lima, S. L. (1987). Vigilance while feeding and its relation to the risk of predation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 124, 303316. Lowe, M. R., & Maycock, B. (1988). Restraint, disinhibition, hunger and negative affect eating. Addictive Behaviors, 13, 369377. Lyman, B. (1982). The nutritional values and food group characteristics of foods preferred during various emotions. Journal of Psychology, 112, 121127. Macdiarmid, J. I., & Hetherington, M. M. (1995). Mood modulation by food: An exploration of affect and cravings in chocolate addicts. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 129138. Macht, M. (1996). Effects of high- and low-energy meals on hunger, physiological processes and reactions to emotional stress. Appetite, 26, 7188. Macht, M. (1998). Effects of noise-induced arousal on chewing of sweet food and the subjective motivation to eat. Nutritional Neuroscience, 1, 213222. Macht, M. (1999). Characteristics of eating in anger, fear, sadness, and joy. Appetite, 33, 129139. Macht, M., Haupt, C., & Ellgring, H. (2005). The perceived function of eating is changed during examination stress: A eld study. Eating Behaviors, 6, 109112.

ARTICLE IN PRESS
10 M. Macht / Appetite 50 (2008) 111 Rasbury, W., & Shemberg, K. (1971). The effects of aversive levels of white noise on consummatory behavior. Psychonomic Science, 22, 166167. Raudenbush, B., & Frank, R. A. (1999). Assessing food neophobia: The role of stimulus familiarity. Appetite, 32, 261271. Reznick, H., & Balch, P. (1977). The effects of anxiety and response cost manipulations on the eating behavior of obese and normal weight subjects. Addictive Behaviors, 2, 219225. Robbins, T. W., & Fray, P. J. (1980). Stress-induced eating: Fact, ction or misunderstanding? Appetite, 1, 103133. Roseneld, S. N., & Stevenson, J. S. (1988). Perception of daily stress and oral coping behaviors in normal, overweight, and recovering alcoholic women. Research in Nursing & Health, 11, 165174. Rosenstein, D., & Oster, H. (1988). Differential facial responses to four basic tastes in newborns. Child Development, 59, 15551568. Rotenberg, K. J., & Flood, D. (1999). Loneliness, dysphoria, dietary restraint, and eating behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25(1), 5564. Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94(1), 2341. Rozin, P., & Schulkin, J. (1990). Food selection. In E. M. Stricker (Ed.), Neurobiology of food and uid intake (pp. 297328). New York: Plenum Press. Ruderman, A. J. (1985). Dysphoric mood and overeating: A test of restraint theorys disinhibition hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 7885. Russell, J. A., & Feldman Barrett, L. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 805819. Rutledge, T., & Linden, W. (1998). To eat or not to eat: Affective and physiological mechanisms in the stress-eating relationship. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 21, 221240. Sampson, D., Muscat, R., Philips, G., & Willner, P. (1992). Decreased reactivity to sweetness following chronic exposure to mild unpredictable stress or acute administration of pimozide. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 16(4), 519524. Schachter, S., Goldman, R., & Gordon, A. (1968). Effects of fear, food deprivation, and obesity on eating. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 9197. Schotte, D. E., Cools, J., & McNally, R. J. (1990). Film-induced negative affect triggers overeating in restrained eaters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 317320. Slochower, J. (1983). Excessive eating: The role of emotions and environment. New York: Human Sciences Press. Smith, B. A., Fillion, T. J., & Blass, E. M. (1990). Orally mediated sources of calming in 1- to 3-day-old human infants. Developmental Psychology, 26(5), 731737. Spillman, D. (1990). Survey of food and vitamin intake responses reported by university students experiencing stress. Psychological Reports, 66, 499502. Spitzer, L., & Rodin, J. (1983). Arousal-induced eating: Conventional wisdom or empirical ndings? In J. Capiocco, & R. Petty (Eds.), Social psychophysiology (pp. 565591). New York: Guilford Press. Staiger, P., Dawe, S., & McCarthy, R. (2000). Responsivity to food cues in bulimic women and controls. Appetite, 35, 2733. Steiner, J. E. (1979). Human facial expressions in response to taste and smell stimulation. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 13, 257295. Steiner, J. E., Glaser, D., Hawilo, M. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2001). Comparative expression of hedonic impact: Affective reactions to tastes and other primates. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 5374. Steptoe, A., Lipsey, Z., & Wardle, J. (1998). Stress, hassles and variations in alcohol consumption, food choice and physical exercise. British Journal of Health Psychology, 3, 5163. Sterritt, G. M. (1962). Inhibition and faciliation of eating by electric shock. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 226229. Macht, M., & Mu ller, J. (2007). Immediate effects of chocolate on experimentally induced mood states. Appetite, doi:10.1016/j. appet.2007.05.004. Macht, M., Roth, S., & Ellgring, H. (2002). Chocolate eating in healthy men during experimentally induced sadness and joy. Appetite, 39, 147158. Macht, M., & Simons, G. (2000). Emotions and eating in everyday life. Appetite, 35, 6571. Markus, C. R., Panhuysen, G., & Tuiten, A. (1998). Does carbohydraterich, protein-poor food prevent a deterioration of mood and cognitive performance of stress-prone subjects when subjected to a stressful task. Appetite, 31, 4965. Martins, Y., & Pliner, P. (2005). Human food choices: An examination of the factors underlying acceptance/rejection of novel and familiar animal and nonanimal foods. Appetite, 45, 214224. McCann, B. S., Warnick, G. R., & Knopp, R. H. (1990). Changes in plasma lipids and dietary intake accompanying shifts in perceived workload and stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 52, 97108. Mehrabian, A. (1980). Basic dimensions for a general psychological theory. Cambridge: Oelgeschlager, Gunn & Hain. Michaud, C., Kahn, J. P., Musse, N., Burlet, C., Nicolas, J. P., & Mejean, L. (1990). Relationships between a critical life event and eating behaviour in high-school students. Stress Medicine, 6, 5764. Morley, J. E., Levine, A. S., & Rowland, N. E. (1983). Stress-induced eating. Life Sciences, 32, 21692182. Nederkoorn, C., Smulders, F., Havermans, R., & Jansen, A. (2004). Exposure to binge food in bulimia nervosa: Finger pulse amplitude as a potential measure of urge to eat and predictor of food intake. Appetite, 42, 125130. Nederkoorn, C., Smulders, F. T. Y., & Jansen, A. (2000). Cephalic phase responses, craving and food intake in normal subjects. Appetite, 35, 4555. Oliver, G., & Wardle, J. (1999). Perceived effects of stress on food choice. Physiology & Behavior, 66, 511515. Oliver, G., Wardle, J., & Gibson, E. L. (2000). Stress and food choice: A laboratory study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 853865. Overduin, J., Jansen, A., & Eilkes, H. (1997). Cue reactivity to food- and body-related stimuli in restrained and unrestrained eaters. Addictive Behaviors, 22, 395404. , W. P. (1964). The effect of chronic environmental stress on stomach Pare ulceration, adrenal function, and consummatory behavior in the rat. Journal of Psychology, 57, 143151. Pine, C. J. (1985). Anxiety and eating behavior in obese and nonobese American Indians and white Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 774780. Pines, C. J., & Gal, R. (1977). The effect of food on test anxiety. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 49, 774780. Pliner, P., & Steverango, C. (1994). Effect of induced mood on memory for avors. Appetite, 22, 135148. Plutchik, R. (1984). Emotions: A general psychoevolutionary theory. In K. R. Scherer (Ed.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 197219). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1999). Distress and eating: Why do dieters overeat? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 153164. Pollard, T. M., Steptoe, A., Canaan, L., Davies, G. J., & Wardle, J. (1995). Effects of academic examination stress on eating behavior and blood lipid levels. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 2(4), 299320. Popper, R., Smits, G., Meiselman, H. L., & Hirsch, E. (1989). Eating in combat: A survey of US Marines. Military Medicine, 154, 619623. Pudel, V. (1984). Psychosoziale Bewertung der Erna hrung in Familien mit Kindern. Eine Repra sentativerhebung in der Bundesrepublik Deutschland. In Deutsche Gesellschaft fu hrungsbericht hrung (Ed.), Erna r Erna (pp. 103144). Frankfurt am Main: Henrich. Pudel, V., & Richter, M. (1980). Psychosoziale bewertung der erna hrung. Eine repra sentativ-erhebung in der bundesrepublik deutschland. In D. G. f. Erna hrungsbericht. Frankfurt: Deutsche hrung (Ed.), Erna Gesellschaft fu hrung. r Erna

ARTICLE IN PRESS
M. Macht / Appetite 50 (2008) 111 Sterritt, G. M. (1965). Inhibition and facilitation of eating by electric shock: III. A further study of the role of strain and of shock level. Psychonomic Science, 2, 319320. Sterritt, G. M., & Shemberg, K. (1963). Inhibition and facilitation of eating by electric shock-II. Shock level, shock schedule and strain of rats. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 7, 215223. Stone, A. A., & Brownell, K. (1994). The stress-eating paradox: Multiple daily measurements in adult males and females. Psychology and Health, 9, 425436. Striegel-Moore, R. H., Morrison, J. A., Schreiber, G., Schumann, B. C., Crawford, P. B., & Obarzanek, E. (1999). Emotioninduced eating and sucrose intake in children: The NHLBI growth and health study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 389398. Strongman, K. T. (1965). The effect of anxiety on food intake in rats. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 255260. Strongman, K. T., Coles, M. G. H., Remington, R. E., & Wookey, P. E. (1970). The effect of shock duration and intensity on the ingestion of food of varying palatability. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 521525. Stunkard, A. J., & Messick, S. (1985). The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition, and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 29, 7183. Telch, C. F., & Agras, W. S. (1996). Do emotional states inuence binge eating in the obese. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 20(3), 271279. Telch, C. F., Agras, W. S., & Linehan, M. M. (2001). Dialectical behavior therapy for binge eating disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69(6), 10611065. Thayer, R. E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and arousal. New York: Oxford University Press. Thayer, R. E. (2001). Calm energyHow people regulate mood with food and exercise. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Tomkins, S. S. (1963). Affect, imagery, consciousness, 2: The negative affects. New York: Tavistock/Routledge. Troop, N. A., Treasure, J. L., & Serpell, L. (2002). A further exploration of disgust in eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 10, 218226. Tuschen, B., Florin, I., & Baucke, R. (1993). BeeinuXt die Stimmung den Appetit? Zeitschrift fu r Klinische Psychologie, 22(3), 315321. Ullman, A. D. (1951). The experimental production and analysis of a compulsive eating syndrome. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 44, 575581. Ullman, A. D. (1952). Three factors involved in producing compulsive eating in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 45, 490496. van Strien, T. (2002). Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) Manual. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company Ltd. van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E. R., Bergers, G. P. A., & Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional and external eating behavior. International Journal of Eating Behavior, 5, 295315. 11 Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Maynard, L., Jayne, M., Fowler, J. S., Zhu, W., et al. (2003). Brain dopamine is associated with eating behaviors in humans. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 136142. Vreugdenberg, L., Bryan, J., & Kemps, E. (2003). The effect of selfinitiated weight-loss dieting on working memory: The role of preoccupying cognitions. Appetite, 41, 291300. Wallis, D. J., & Hetherington, M. M. (2004). Stress and eating: The effects of ego-threat and cognitive demand on food intake in restrained and emotional eaters. Appetite, 43, 3946. Wansink, B., Cheney, M. M., & Chan, N. (2003). Exploring comfort food preferences across age and gender. Physiology & Behavior, 79, 739747. Ward, A., & Mann, T. (2000). Don t mind if I do: Disinhibited eating under cognitive load. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 753763. Wardle, J., Steptoe, A., Oliver, G., & Lipsey, Z. (2000). Stress, dietary restraint and food intake. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 48, 195202. Weidner, G., Kohlmann, C. W., Dotzauer, E., & Burns, L. R. (1996). The effects of academic stress on health behaviors in young adults. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 9, 123133. Weinstein, S. E., Shide, D. J., & Rolls, B. J. (1997). Changes in food intake in response to stress in men and women: Psychological factors. Appetite, 28, 718. Weiss, J. M. (1968). Effects of coping responses on stress. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 65, 251260. Whishaw, I. Q., Dringenberg, H. C., & Comery, T. A. (1992). Rats (Rattus norvegicus) modulat eating speed and vigilance to optimize food consumption: Effects of cover, circadian rhythm, food deprivation, and individual differences. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 411419. Willenbring, M. L., Levine, A. S., & Morley, J. E. (1986). Stress induced eating and food preference in human: A pilot study. International Journal of Eating Behavior, 5(5), 855864. Willner, P. (1997). Validity, reliability and utility of the chronic mild stress model of depression: A 10-year review and evaluation. Psychopharmacology, 134, 319329. Willner, P., Benton, D., Brown, E., Survijt, C., Davies, G., Morgan, J., et al. (1998). Depression increases craving for sweet rewards in animal and human models of depression and craving. Psychopharmacology, 136, 272283. Willner, P., & Healy, S. (1994). Decreased hedonic responsiveness during a brief depressive mood swing. Journal of Affective Disorders, 32, 1320. Willner, P., Muscat, R., & Papp, M. (1992). Chronic mild stress-induced Anhedonia: A realistic model of depression. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 16, 525534. Wing, R. R., Blair, E. H., Epstein, L. H., & McDermott, M. D. (1990). Psychological stress and glucose metabolism in obese and normalweight subjects: A possible mechanism for differences in stress-induced eating. Health Psychology, 9, 693700. Wiser, S., & Telch, C. F. (1999). Dialectical behavior therapy for bingeeating disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(6), 755768. Wurtman, R. J. (1982). Nutrients that modify brain function. Scientic American, 246, 5059.

You might also like