You are on page 1of 27

Sociology of Religion 2010, 71:3 280-306 doi:10.

1093/socrel/srq045 Advance Access Publication 4 June 2010

America as a Christian Nation? Understanding Religious Boundaries of National Identity in the United States*
Jeremy Brooke Straughn Scott L. Feld
Purdue University
Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

Though predominantly Christian since the time of its founding, the United States has become more religiously diverse in recent decades. Yet since the mid-1990s, the proportion of Americans who see their country as a Christian nation has reportedly increased. Though initially paradoxical, these trends are less mysterious if the idea of a Christian America (CA) is understood, not as a description of religious demography, but as a discursive practice that seeks to align the symbolic boundaries of national belonging with the boundaries of the dominant faith community. Using data from the 1996 and 2004 General Social Survey, it is shown that the growing prevalence of CA was restricted to Americans of Christian faith, thereby widening an existing religious divide over the meaning of American identity.
Key words: national identity; Christianity; symbolic boundaries; social change; USA.

INTRODUCTION In the rst decade of the twenty-rst century, Christians remain the largest religious group in the United States, as they have been since the countrys founding. Hence it is not surprising that most Americans tend to think of their country as a Christian nation. According to a 2006 survey by the Pew Research Center, two-thirds of American adults say they consider the United States a Christian nation (Pew Research Center 2006:5), while nearly a third of

*Direct correspondence to Jeremy Brooke Straughn, Department of Sociology, Stone Hall, 700 W. State St., West Lafayette, IN 47907-2059, USA. E-mail: jstraugh@purdue.edu. This paper has evolved to its present form over several years. Earlier versions of the paper were written in close collaboration between the authors. Later revisions, including the nal version, were prepared by the rst author in consultation with the second. The authors wish to thank Jim Davidson, Fenggang Yang, Dan Olson, Angie Andriot, Lisa Fein, Jun Lu, Dan Mroczek, and the ve anonymous SOR reviewers for their helpful critiques of earlier drafts. We owe them our gratitude, but hold them blameless for any remaining errors. # The Author 2010. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Association for the Sociology of Religion. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org.

280

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

281

respondents in another study strongly agree that, In the twenty-rst century, the United States is still basically a Christian society (Wuthnow 2005:200). In demographic and cultural respects, of course, America is no longer as Christian as it used to be. Over the last few decades, the proportion of Americans professing Christian faith, especially of the Protestant confession, has experienced a steady decline. Although Christians still account for about 78 percent of U.S. adults as a whole, the percentage of Protestants has fallen from over 60 percent in the early 1990s to around 50 percent as of 2006 (see also Kosmin and Keysar 2009; Pew Forum 2008; Smith and Kim 2005). Meanwhile, the ranks of the religiously unafliated have grown from fewer than 10 percent to between 14 and 16 percent over the same period (Hout and Fischer 2002; Pew Forum 2008). Yet paradoxically, the growth of religious diversity has not produced a concomitant decline in the view of America as a Christian country. On the contrary, such views have generally grown more prevalent among U.S. adults since the turn of the century. As of the mid-1990s, only 60 percent of Pew respondents said they regarded their country as a Christian nation. By 2002, agreement with this view had risen to 67 percent and reached a peak of 71 percent in 2005 before settling to 67 percent the following year (Pew Research Center 2006:5). Why is America increasingly perceived as a Christian nation, even though Christians have not been growing as a proportion of the American population? If the notion of a Christian America is not just a reection of demographic realities, what could account for its varying appeal over time? In this article, we suggest that such questions become more tractable when Christian America discourse is viewed through the lens of recent work on symbolic boundary con r 2002; struction (Edgell et al. 2006; Kunovich 2006; Lamont and Molna Wimmer 2008; Zimmer 2003). Rather than merely describing the demographic status quo, statements like America is a Christian nation represent a discursive practice that seeks to align the boundaries of authentic national belonging with adherence to the dominant religious faith. As we go on to argue, practices of symbolic boundary alignment are always a double-edged sword, simultaneously designating certain groups as prototypical of the larger community while relegating others to the symbolic margins. For example, by conditioning recognition as an authentic American on adherence to Christian faith, the idea of a Christian America tacitly reinforces the moral prestige of the religious majority, even as it presents Americans of other faiths, or with no formal religion, with invisible barriers to symbolic inclusion. Consequently, we anticipate that such ideas will appeal primarily to members of the religious ingroup and are likely to be contested by outgroups at risk of marginalization. In other words, we expect Christian America discourse to be divisive along religious lines, rather than a source of consensus and solidarity transcending religious boundaries. To the extent that Christian America discourse appeals mainly to U.S. Christians, it follows that most of its aggregate variation over time should be

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

282

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

due to uctuations within the Christian community. Moreover, because Christians are no more numerous today, relative to non-Christians, than in previous decades, the growing prevalence of Christian America beliefs most likely reects an increase in the importance of Christianity to American identity among Christian Americans themselves. Precisely why this should have occurred when it did is a question we take up in the course of our theoretical and empirical analysis. Whatever its causes, however, the recent surge in these beliefs likely signals a growing divergence in attitudes between American Christians and their non-Christian compatriots. To the extent that such polarization can present an obstacle to political consensus on major issues (DiMaggio et al. 1996), a widening religious divide over the meaning of American identity could become a source of future social conict. The rest of this article is structured as follows. In the next section, we outline the contours of a boundary-oriented approach for explicating the link between religious identity and conceptions of national belonging. In the section thereafter, we develop a series of hypotheses which link the intensity of Christian America beliefs to characteristics and attitudes of individuals, as well as identifying a number of mechanisms to account for their changing patterns over time. In the analytic sections of the paper, nally, we put our hypotheses to the test using data from the 1996 and 2004 General Social Surveys (GSS).

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

THEORIZING RELIGIOUS BOUNDARIES OF AMERICAN IDENTITY Our theoretical approach is informed by recent research on the role of symbolic boundaries in the construction of social identities (Edgell et al. 2006; Kunovich 2006; Smith et al. 1998; Wimmer 2008; Zimmer 2003; for a general r 2002). As Lamont and Molna r (2002) discussion see Lamont and Molna dene them, Symbolic boundaries are conceptual distinctions made by social actors to categorize objects, people, practices, and even time and space. They are tools by which individuals and groups struggle over and come to agree upon r 2002:168). A key func[or contest] denitions of reality (Lamont and Molna tion of symbolic boundaries is to separate people into groups and generate r 2002:168). feelings of similarity and group membership (Lamont and Molna As Edgell et al. (2006:231) have shown, Symbolic boundaries are effective . . . in promoting a sense of solidarity and identity by virtue of imagining an other who does not share the core characteristics imagined to be held by those who are legitimate participants in the moral order; the imagined community must have outsiders as well as insiders. In constructing symbolic boundaries, social actors select from a variety of r 2002; Zimmer culturally available symbolic resources (Lamont and Molna 2003), or boundary criteria. Thus, religious identity often overlaps with national identity because [religion] provides one means of distinguishing

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

283

ingroups from outgroups (Kunovich 2006:438). Yet dening one type of social identity (national belonging) in terms of another (religious afliation) is not simply a matter of boundary overlap. Rather, it involves a practice we call symbolic boundary alignment, or the nesting of one symbolic boundary within another. For example, the statement that America is a Christian nation not only posits an intersection between religious and national boundaries; it also implies that the boundary between Christians and non-Christians helps regulate the threshold between more and less prototypical Americans (on prototypicality, see Mummendey and Wenzel 1999). Because symbolic boundaries are always moral boundaries (Edgell et al. 2006; Lamont 2000), aligning national with religious boundaries is likely to have very different consequences for religious ingroups and outgroups within the national community. Among members of the ingroup, the notion of a Christian America may, at times, serve as a rhetorical weapon in the arsenal of religious activists seeking to mobilize supporters around social or political issues of the day (Regnerus and Smith 1998; Schwadel 2005). At other times, what sounds like the battle cry of cultural warriors may only be intended to strengthen religious identication and solidarity among coreligionists. For example, Smith et al. (2000) argue that much evangelical discourse about reclaiming America is simply talk to construct and maintain collective identity. This evangelical rhetoric is functioning not so much to actually get the troops ready to re-Christianize America as to express and reinforce a distinctive identity for its adherents (Smith et al. 2000:56). Thus, we do not assume that the appeal of Christian America beliefs will always rise and fall in tandem with religious politicization (Schwadel 2005; Servin-Gonzales and Torres-Reyna 1999) and deprivatization (Regnerus and Smith 1998; Wilcox and Goldberg 2002). The idea of a Christian America does not, by itself, imply that Christianity should be imposed on non-Christians, or that Christians should enjoy special rights and privileges as a matter of public policy (see also Wuthnow 2005:200). Nor does it necessarily entail the belief that religion should play a more prominent role in politics. In principle, a Christian who sees her/his faith as central to American identity could still regard her/his religious beliefs and practices as a personal matter. Yet, whatever its symbolic function for self-described insiders, any talk of a Christian America is likely to have a dispiriting impact on those it implicitly marginalizes. Asked to comment on a conference dedicated to Reclaiming America for Christ, one Jewish American responded that, When the language becomes exclusively Christian, Jewish groups become at best ambivalent, at worst hostile (Smith et al. 2000:22). Such fears are not always misplaced. Even if their immediate effects are largely imagined, symbolic boundaries can also have material consequences, serving as an essential medium through which people acquire status and monopolize resources r 2002:168). By attributing contrasting degrees of social (Lamont and Molna prestige to insiders and outsiders, symbolic boundaries can confer differential access to material benets and other advantages. As a result of these benets,

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

284

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

group members limit membership to protect their rewards (Kunovich 2006:440). Indeed, if taken to extremes, symbolic boundaries can have even direr implications, as when restrictive denitions of nationhood serve as a pretext for depriving marginalized citizens of their civil rights or denying citizenship to outsiders on the basis of race, religion, or national origin (Somers 2006; Zolberg 2006).

HYPOTHESES Drawing on our theory of symbolic boundary alignment, our next goal is to develop specic hypotheses for predicting the salience of Christianity to national identity in the American context. Two sets of hypotheses are proposed. First, we attempt to specify the groups in society for whom the appeal of Christian America beliefs (or CA) should be strongest, as well as some of the factors that should further enhance its appeal among potential supporters. Second, with these hypotheses in mind we consider some of the mechanisms that could help account for changes in CA over time. Predictors of CA Most Americans are strongly attached to their country, exhibiting high levels of pride in citizenship and national symbols (Smith and Kim 2006). However, any restrictive denition of national identity is likely to be controversial, even divisive. Because the alignment of national identity with internal symbolic boundaries will have different consequences for ingroups and outgroups, we believe that afnities with CA will differ signicantly between dominant and non-dominant religious groups. In general, we concur with Kunovich (2006) that religious majorities are more likely to state that the dominant religion is very important for national identity, while religious minorities are likely to downplay the dominant religion as a relevant factor for national identity because their own religious background excludes them from membership in the nation (Kunovich 2006:440). Hence, we expect that the salience of Christianity to American identity will be more pronounced within the Christian majority than the non-Christian minority (viz. believers of other faiths and the religiously nonafliated). Hypothesis 1: CA will be higher among Christian Americans than among non-Christians Among U.S. Christians, we expect to nd signicant variation between different denominations and confessions (Steensland et al. 2000:292). One way that denominational heritage could motivate religious understandings of national identity is by encouraging adherents to think of themselves as prototypical representatives of their faith tradition. For example, Smith and colleagues (1998) argue that evangelical Protestants are especially prone to regard themselves as prototypical Christians. Thus, when evangelicals use the word

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

285

Christian, most likely they are instinctively meaning evangelical Christian (Smith et al. 1998:124, original italics; cf., Smith et al. 2000:200; Wuthnow 2005:200). CA could also have disproportionate appeal for Christians in traditionally marginalized social categories, perhaps offering a means of compensating for their historical exclusion. Such a dynamic might be at work in black Protestant denominations, in which racial and religious identities are tightly interwoven (Pattillo-McCoy 1998; Steensland et al. 2000). We therefore expect that CA beliefs will have greater appeal to (white) evangelicals and black Protestants than to mainline Protestants, Roman Catholics, and other Christian groups. Hypothesis 2: Among U.S. Christians, CA will be higher among evangelicals and black Protestants than other Christians Independently of denomination or confession, we further expect that the salience of religion to national identity will increase with the importance of either religious or national identity. As Hoge and De Zulueta (1985) have argued, the importance of any one component [of identity] relative to others should predict whether the component in question is likely to inuence others (Hoge and De Zulueta 1985:22). In previous research, religiosity has been measured by the frequency of attendance at religious services (Kunovich 2006), while closeness to ones country is a good indicator of national identication (Huddy and Khatib 2007). We therefore predict that: Hypothesis 3: Among U.S. Christians, CA should increase with (a) frequency of religious attendance and (b) feelings of closeness to America According to recent studies, religious identities often become more salient in a religiously diverse environment (Kunovich 2006; Smith et al. 1998; Wilcox and Goldbert 2002). For example, Christian Smith and colleagues argue that solidarity among evangelical Christians grows stronger with feelings of embattlement in the face of religious pluralism (Smith, et al 1998, 2000), while Wilcox and Goldberg (2002:371) suggest that highly committed Christians may see growing diversity as threatening their religious hegemony and as undermining the nations status as a Christian nation. Since immigration is a major factor in the growth of religious diversity (Smith and Kim 2005), feelings of embattlement and outgroup hostility should be manifested in preferences for reducing the current rate of immigration (Bail 2008; Esses et al. 2001).1 Hence:
Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

An alternate approach is to calculate the degree of heterogeneity in a particular region of residence. However, such measures would be inappropriate in the present case, as their effects tend to be confounded with those of religious participation (Voas et al. 2002). In contrast, measures of outgroup attitudes should help capture the ways in which religious outgroups have been depicted in public and private discourse, and hence more directly reect the impact of cultural diversity on boundary salience at the individual level.

286

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

Hypothesis 4: Among U.S. Christians, CA will increase with preferences for restricting the rate of immigration to the United States Accounting for change over time From these propositions, we can derive a number of mechanisms that could affect changes in the overall prevalence of CA beliefs over time. Our main focus is on two types of mechanisms: (1) gradual shifts in the religious composition of society (compositional effects) and (2) major events that affect the salience of religious and national boundaries ( period effects). As we will see, there are good reasons to doubt that demographic changes since the 1990s could have directly produced a concomitant increase in CA in the population. We therefore expect that any aggregate rise in CA will have occurred in response to major eventsa reaction, moreover, that should be especially pronounced among members of the Christian majority. Compositional effects In light of Hypothesis 1, it is indeed paradoxical that belief in America as a Christian nation would have grown more prevalent during a period when the proportion of Christians in the population was on the decline. If CA is typically higher among U.S. Christians than among non-Christians, then a decrease in the percentage of Christians should cause CA to become less prevalent in the population, since its adherents have grown proportionately less numerous. By the same token, Hypothesis 2 implies that the overall prevalence of CA should vary with the ratio of Christians in certain denominations (especially evangelicals or black Protestants), other things being equal. According to the most recent wave of the American Religious Identication Survey, there was some growth in the share of Christians belonging to evangelical denominations or non-denominational congregations between 1990 and 2008, mostly at the expense of mainline Protestant churches (Kosmin and Keysar 2009:5). However, the rate of growth in these groups does not appear to have outpaced that of non-Christian religions or of Americans who do not identify with any religious tradition. Whereas the U.S. population is 30 percent larger today than in 1990, there are 50 percent more non-Christians and 138 percent more nones (Kosmin and Keysar 2009:4; on the latter category, see also Hout and Fischer 2002). On balance, then, we do not believe that changes in the religious makeup of American society would be sufcient to account for the growing prevalence of CA beliefs since the 1990s. Indeed, it seems more likely the net effect of compositional change has been to diminish the rate of growth in CA in recent years. Hypothesis 5: Changes in the religious composition of American society have favored the attenuation of CA in the population Period effects Independently of demographic trends, religious conceptions of American identity could have experienced a net surge if major events of the day have brought religious and national issues to the forefront of public attention. For example, Huntington (2004:340) asserted that, in the wake of

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

287

religiously motivated attacks on their homeland, Americans increasingly dene themselves in religious and cultural terms: September 11 dramatically symbolized . . . the beginning of a new era in which people dene themselves primarily in terms of culture and religion . . . . For Americans the religious component of their identity takes on new relevance in this environment. Recent studies conrm that beliefs about the role of religion in public life often uctuate in response to historic events. For example, Regnerus and Smith (1998) argue that religious deprivatization tends to occur in cycles, becoming especially pronounced in times of signicant crisis. Throughout U.S. history, disturbing shifts in the moral order . . . [have] prompted a revisiting of traditional or commonsense strategies of action by the organizational sources of political and religious meaning systems . . .. During these unsettled times, religious convictions play an increasingly prominent role in the public sphere, before receding again into taken-for-granted ways of life during more settled periods (Regnerus and Smith 1998:1351; cf., Swidler 1986). Such phenomena are commonly known as period effects, or societal reactions to sudden changes of political climate during a particular interval (Alwin and McCammon 2003; Weil 1987). In the case of pure period effects, major events will produce a more or less uniform increase (or decrease) in the prevalence of particular beliefs in society as a whole. Unfortunately, it is often difcult to determine whether any particular set of events is directly responsible for pure period effects, since we cannot rule out other developments that may have occurred during the same interval. Certain kinds of events, however, have more distinctive consequences, affecting different segments of the population in contrasting ways. An instructive example is what Regnerus and Smith (1998) call selective deprivatization (see also Casanova 1994). In the wake of the 1960s, they argue, conservative evangelicals came to supplant socially liberal Protestants as the most vocal advocates for the public role of religion, thereby reversing a longer trend of privatization among religious conservatives (Regnerus and Smith 1998; cf., Smith et al. 2000). By extension, we will speak of selective intensication whenever historical developments cause particular attitudes to rise only in certain groups, while declining or remaining stable in others. In the case of CA, we doubt that the impact of recent history will have been uniform. Although events like the attacks of 9 of 11 or the invasion of Iraq could have enhanced the salience of religious and national issues for Americans in general, Hypothesis 1 leads us to expect that the relevance of Christianity to American identity should intensify primarily within the Christian majority. Hypotheses 3 and 4 provide additional support for this expectation. If recent events have caused CA to intensify among U.S. Christians, they may have done so, in part, by elevating the level of religiosity, patriotism, and xenophobia in the population. Religious minorities, on the other hand, are unlikely to respond by recognizing the dominant religion as a hallmark of the true American, since this would reinforce their symbolic marginality within the imagined community (cf., Kunovich 2006).

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

288

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

Hypothesis 6: Unsettling events since the mid-1990s have enhanced CA among U.S. Christians Finally, we expect that period events which trigger selective intensication on the part of Christian Americans will also have heterogeneous effects on different categories of Christians. In general, we expect CA beliefs to intensify the most among those who were most inclined to embrace them in the past. Specically, we predict that any increase in CA should have occurred disproportionately among evangelicals and black Protestants (H2), Christians with frequent religious attendance (H3a), Christians with high levels of patriotism (H3b), Christians with more negative attitudes toward national outgroups (H4). We refer to such patterns as (selective) intensication at the top. Hypothesis 6a: Selective intensication will occur faster among those categories of Christians where CA was previously higher

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

DATASET AND VARIABLES Sample Our analysis draws on two waves of GSS data collected by the National Opinion Research Center and obtained electronically from the InterUniversity Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR 04295). The GSS has conducted face-to-face interviews with a representative sample of American adults every year or two since 1972 (Davis et al. 2007). In 1996 and 2004, items from the 1995 and 2003 national identity modules of the International Social Survey Project were included in the GSS questionnaire for about 2,500 cases.2 Dependent Variable In 1996 and 2004, the GSS questionnaires included a battery of items prefaced by the following question: Some people say the following things are important for being truly American. Others say they are not important. How important do you think each of the following is? Responses for each boundary criterion were coded as very important (1), fairly important (2), not very important (3), or not important at all (4). We measure the salience of Christianity to American identity (CA) using the variable AMCHRSTN (To be a Christian), which we reverse coded to rise with level of importance. AMCHRSTN closely resembles items commonly used to measure what we call boundary alignment, or the salience of particular boundary criteria for symbolic
2 Descriptive statistics for the dependent and independent variables discussed below can be found in Appendix A in the Supplementary Material accompanying this article at Sociology of Religion online (http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org).

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

289

belonging in a specied ingroup (Bail 2008; Citrin et al. 2001; Edgell et al. 2006; Jones and Smith 2001; Kunovich 2006). We generally expect this variable to behave in similar ways to other measures of CA beliefs (e.g., Pew Research Center 2006; Smith et al. 2000; Wuthnow 2005). An advantage of the AMCHRSTN item over alternative items (e.g., Is America a Christian nation) is the specication that being a Christian makes one more truly American, thereby highlighting the normative implications of symbolic boundary construction.

Independent Variables Religious afliation In order to capture distinctions among clusters of Protestant denominations, as well as between these and other religious categories, our analysis employs an aggregation method developed by Steensland et al. (2000), which combines information in the RELIG, DENOM, and OTHER variables into a single polytomous measure of religious tradition. The resulting variable (RELTRAD) codes self-reported religious afliation as evangelical Protestant, mainline Protestant, black Protestant, Roman Catholic, Jewish, other, or nonafliated. However, about 40 percent of respondents in the other category are self-identied Christians (36 percent Protestant, 4 percent Orthodox). Because we are often interested in contrasting Christians and non-Christians, we have moved respondents who are coded as other on RELTRAD, but as Protestant or Orthodox on the RELIG variable, into a separate category of Other Christian. The remaining others (comprising adherents of non-Christian traditions) are combined with Jewish respondents and labeled as Other Faith. Our modied RELTRAD variable thus codes religious afliation as: evangelical Protestant (1), mainline Protestant (2), black Protestant (3), Roman Catholic (4), Other Christian (5), Jewish/Other Faith (6), or nonafliated (7). Other independent variables Additional predictors of CA for Christians include religious commitment, national attachment, and immigration preferences. In our multivariate models, we also include controls for birth cohort, education, sex, region of residence, and race.3 To adjust for non-response bias, household sampling design, and sampling differences between survey years, all of our results were generated using a weighting variable (WT7204) provided by the National Opinion Research Center.
A description of these variables is available in Appendix B in the Supplementary Material accompanying this article at Sociology of Religion online (http://socrel .oxfordjournals.org). Other candidates for inclusion as independent variables (e.g., income, marital status, family background, citizenship) showed very weak or insignicant relationships with the dependent variable and would have made no meaningful contribution to our analysis. Although party afliation is related to CA for one of our two survey years, its effects appear to be spurious, arising from its associations with certain included predictors (e.g., religious afliation, religious attendance).
3

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

290

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

WHO THINKS BEING A CHRISTIAN MAKES ONE MORE TRULY AMERICAN? Descriptive Analysis We begin our empirical investigation by examining the distribution of CA beliefs at the outset of our period of study. As a provisional test of our rst two hypotheses, we cross-tabulate the bivariate relationship between CA and religious preference using our 1996 GSS sample (table 1). In the sample as a whole, we nd that 53 percent of respondents regarded being a Christian as either fairly (15 percent) or very important (38 percent) for being truly American. As expected, however, this majority consensus masks a sharp divergence of opinion between Christians and non-Christians. Among Christians, nearly 62 percent believed that being Christian is either very important (44 percent) or fairly important (18 percent) for being truly American, whereas nearly 66 percent of non-Christians felt that Christian faith was not at all important, and almost one in ve (19 percent) thought it was not very important. CA was also higher among evangelical Protestants than most other Christian groups, although it was even more pronounced among black Protestants. Fifty-eight percent of evangelicals and 69 percent of black Protestants regarded being a Christian as very important for being truly American, while 36 percent of mainline Protestants, and 30 percent of

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

TABLE 1 The Salience of Christianity to American Identity, by Religious Afliation (N 1,283)a How important is being a Christian for being truly American? (percent) N All respondents Christian Evangelical Prot. Mainline Protestant Black Protestant Roman Catholic Other Christian Non-Christian Jewish/other faith Nonafliated 1,283 1,049 346 247 111 318 26 224 84 148 Not at all 25.2 16.0 6.6 21.1 5.4 24.5 34.6 66.2 66.7 66.2 Not very 21.4 21.9 19.1 22.3 13.5 28.0 19.2 19.2 20.2 18.9 Fairly 15.4 17.6 16.8 20.6 12.6 17.3 26.9 5.1 2.4 6.8 Very 38.0 44.4 57.5 36.0 68.5 30.2 19.2 9.4 10.7 8.1 Total 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0

a 1996 GSS (weighted results). Column and row totals reect rounding error and sample weighting.

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

291

Roman Catholics held the same view. The remaining Christians (chiey quasi-Protestant denominations and sects) were more ambivalent. Only 19 percent thought of their faith as essential to American identity, while just over a third felt the opposite way.

Multivariate Analysis Analytic strategy To ensure that these religious differences are not due to other sources of variation among religious groups, we use a multivariate procedure to estimate the effect of religious afliation on CA while controlling for our other independent variables. Because AMCHRSTN is highly skewed (with very important as the modal category), we have dichotomized our dependent variable to highlight the threshold between very important and all other responses. We use binary logistic regression (Long 1997) to estimate the net effects of the independent variables on the log-odds that being a Christian is regarded as very important for being truly American. For ease of interpretation, our discussion focuses on the exponentiated regression coefcients (exp[b]). Results The rst model in table 2 tests whether religious afliation (Christians vs. non-Christians) signicantly inuences CA in the 1996 sample. As expected (H1), Christians are considerably more likely to regard being a Christian as very important for being truly American. If fact, the odds of such a response are more than three and a half times as great for this group than for non-Christians, controlling for other differences between them. The test statistic (DX2) indicates that the model ts the data signicantly better than a comparison model without religious afliation. Models 2 4 test whether CA varies signicantly by denomination, religious attendance, national attachment, and outgroup attitudes among Christian respondents in the sample (H2 H4). As expected (H2), the appeal of CA is quite a bit stronger among evangelicals and especially black Protestants compared to mainline Protestants (Model 2), though it is substantially weaker among smaller Christian sects and denominations. Independently of denominational differences, Christians who attend religious services more frequently (H3a), display very strong national attachment (H3b), or favor a large reduction in immigration (H4) are signicantly more likely to see their faith as essential to American identity (Model 3). When the control variables are added (Model 4), most of these effects remain statistically signicant and similar in size.4
4 The exceptions are black Protestants and other Christians. Although both coefcient estimates are in the expected direction, neither attains statistical signicance in Model 4. We suspect that these denominational differences do exist in the population, but that our sample contains too few cases in these categories to rule out the possibility of random error.

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

TABLE 2 Logistic Regression of CA Beliefs on Religious Tradition and Other Predictors, 1996a All respondents (N 1,005) Model 1 Model 2 exp(b) 3.694*** 1.733*** 1.431* 1.022 1.780** 0.665*** 0.551** 0.302*** 1.086 1.909*** 0.916 1.417 2 0.091 2 1.409 2 0.701 Christian respondents (N 829) Model 3 exp(b) 2.499*** 4.123*** 0.913 0.244 0.749 1.335 2 0.122 2 1.552 0.513 0.597 0.072 0.611 2 1.909 Model 4 exp(b) 2.116*** 3.796*** 0.894 0.212* 1.671*** 1.817*** 1.074 1.842*** 0.871 0.654 0.196 2 1.023 0.466 0.425 2 0.036 0.475 2 0.329 2 0.654 2 1.279 0.109 0.502 0.874 0.606 2 1.023

292
SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

b
Christian (Mainline) Evangelical Black Protestant Roman Catholic Other Christian Religious attendance National attachment Immigration Reduce some Reduce a lot Education Cohort ( , 1940) 1940 59 ! 1960 Female South Race (white) Black Other race Constant 2 2LL X 2 DX 2 df
a

sd(b) 0.296 0.085 0.163 0.194 0.185 0.087 0.203 0.209 0.153 0.159

sd(b) 0.193 0.290 0.201 0.734

sd(b) 0.203 0.304 0.209 0.751 0.084 0.160 0.193 0.183

sd(b) 0.218 0.474 0.227 0.760 0.090 0.171 0.205 0.194 0.090 0.214 0.224 0.162 0.172

exp(b) 2.398*** 1.923 1.217 0.359 1.594*** 1.529* 0.965 1.607* 0.720*** 0.520** 0.278*** 1.115 1.652**

1.307 0.550 0.358 0.022 0.577 2 0.409 2 0.595 2 1.197 0.083 0.647 1.061 1.307 2 2.091

0.252 2.891*** 0.296 3.694*** 0.373 0.124*** 1053.581 23.711*** 1

0.151 0.496*** 1075.422 68.509*** 4

0.243 0.148*** 1009.378 66.045*** 4

0.413 2.396* 0.356 1.833 0.319 0.360*** 944.366 65.012*** 7

For Model 1, DX 2 tests whether the addition of religious afliation improves model t. Model 2 is compared to a null model (intercept only). Models 3 and 4 are compared with Models 2 and 3, respectively. ***p , .001, **p , .01, *p , .05, p , .10 (2-tailed t-test).

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

293

THE GROWING SALIENCE OF CHRISTIAN FAITH TO AMERICAN IDENTITY Descriptive Results Having conrmed that most of our hypothesized predictors signicantly inuence CA in the expected direction, we turn to the question of whether there has been any signicant increase in CA during our period of study and, if so, whether the patterns of change are consistent with the kinds of mechanisms our theory envisions. We begin by cross-tabulating our dichotomous measure of CA by survey year. As a preliminary test of compositional change (H5) and selective intensication (H6, H6a), we examine the distribution of respondents by religious afliation, as well as the rates of change in CA by religious afliation, over time (table 3). The results in the rst line conrm that the salience of Christianity to American identity has signicantly increased. Between 1996 and 2004, the percentage of very important responses rose by more than 11 percentage points, from about 38 percent to almost 50 percent. As we suspected, the overall rise in CA was accompanied by a slight decrease in the proportion of Christians (by about 3 percentage points) between survey years. Although Roman Catholics and Other Christians grew by about 2 percentage points each, their gains were offset by a 5-point loss on the part of mainline Protestants, from 19 percent to just 14 percent. Nor was their any proportionate growth in the two bastions of belief in a CA. Whereas evangelicals were equally represented at both time points, black Protestants suffered a 2-point loss. Among non-Christians, Americans with no religious afliation expanded their relative numbers by 3 percentage points, from 12 percent to 15 percent, while adherents of non-Christian faiths held steady (for a more detailed analysis of these trends, see Kosmin and Keysar 2009; Pew Forum 2008). Given the gradual diminution of Christians (and the lack of proportionate growth in denominational bastions), it is unlikely compositional change was responsible for the observed increase in CA between survey years (H5). A more likely explanation, we hypothesized, is that the rise in CA represents a type of period effect, or societal reaction to unsettling events. Specically, we predicted that major events which focus attention on matters of religion and nationality should lead adherents of the dominant religion to accentuate the importance of their faith as a component of their national identity (H6). As we see in the last set of results, the rise in CA was indeed concentrated in the Christian majority. Whereas the percentage of very important responses increased by an average of 15 points in the Christian category, there was no signicant change for the non-Christian groups. In substantive terms, the fact that CA intensied where it was already the most pronounced means that the existing divide between Christians and non-Christians grew even wider during our period of study. The breakdown by subgroup conrms this impression. While there was no signicant change

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

294

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

TABLE 3 Compositional Change and Selective Intensication, 1996 2004 (N 2,486)a All Respondents CA (percent very important) Religious afliation (percent) Christian Non-Christian Evangelical Protestant Mainline Protestant Black Protestant Roman Catholic Other Christian Jewish/other faith Nonafliated CA by religious afliation (percent) Christian Non-Christian Evangelical Protestant Mainline Protestant Black Protestant Roman Catholic Other Christian Jewish/other faith Nonafliated Sample size (N)
a

All years 42.9 79.9 20.1 100.0 26.4 16.7 7.8 25.8 3.1 7.1 13.0 100.0

1996 37.5 81.1 18.9 100.0 26.7 18.8 8.7 24.9 2.0 7.3 11.6 100.0

2004 48.9 78.5 21.5 100.0 26.2 14.3 6.8 26.9 4.3 6.8 14.7 100.0

D1996 2004 11.4 2 2.6 2.6 0.0 2 0.5 2 4.5 2 1.9 2.0 2.3 2 0.5 3.1 0.0

LR X 2 D 33.404*** 2.664

26.367***
Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

51.2 44.4 59.3 10.1 9.4 10.7 62.7 57.5 68.7 41.6 36.0 50.3 73.0 68.5 79.5 39.4 30.2 49.0 47.9 19.2 63.8 9.5 10.6 8.2 10.0 8.1 11.9 2,486 1,311 1,175

14.9 1.3 11.2 14.3 11.0 18.8 44.6 2 2.4 3.8

42.313*** 0.204 8.039** 8.087** 2.883 23.120*** 14.107*** 0.259 1.211

Weighted results. Note: Column and row totals reect rounding error and sample weighting. ***p , .001, **p , .01, *p , .05, p , .10.

among religious nones and adherents of other faiths, there was a signicant increase in CA for nearly all categories of Christians, though it is greater in some categories than in others. Before drawing rm conclusions from these results, however, we must rst determine whether they remain signicant when other variables are taken into account. Multivariate Analysis For a more rigorous assessment of composition effects versus selective intensication, we turn again to multivariate logistic regression. For this purpose, we have combined the 1996 and 2004 surveys into a single sample

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

295

and added a variable for GSS year to estimate the amount and direction of change in CA between surveys (table 4). The rst model estimates the effect of survey year on our dependent variable. (We include sex, region, and race in this model because their distribution is the same in both surveys.) The exponentiated coefcient for survey year (1.712) in this model conrms that that the odds of a very important response were signicantly higher, by about 70 percent, in 2004 than in 1996. Compositional change The next model adds main effects for the remaining independent variables to see if distributional changes in these predictors as a whole help account for the rise in CA between survey years. As noted above, the modest decline in the ratio of Christians to non-Christians would be expected to favor a slight decrease in the prevalence of CA over time (H5). In addition, two of our control variables that had strong negative effects in table 2 seem likely to have changed in ways that would exert downward pressure on the growth of CA. In particular, the appeal of CA was considerably weaker among better-educated respondents (Regnerus and Smith 1998; Schwadel 2005) and respondents belonging to more recent birth cohorts (Edgell et al. 2006; Kunovich 2006). Over time, the rising level of educational attainment and gradual replacement of older by younger cohorts should have tended to suppress the rate of increase in CA.5 On the other hand, we conrmed (in table 2) that religious attendance, national attachment, and restrictive immigration preferences all had signicant positive effects on CA at the beginning of our study. If these have been on the rise since then (H6), then they might help explain some of the increase in CA over time, perhaps even compensating for suppressing inuence of demographic trends. On inspecting the results in Model 2, however, it appears that the impact of any positive trends has been overshadowed by countervailing winds of compositional change. Here, the estimated effect of survey year (2.224) is even more positive than in the previous model, once all the independent variables are included. This suggests that the rise in CA might have been even sharper if there had been no changes in the predictor variables.6 Selective intensication Having conrmed that the observed rise in CA was indeed signicant, even though formidable demographic trends should have

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

5 In the case of birth cohort, an alternative possibility is that the observed age differences simply reect naturally occurring variations over the life span (also known as lifecycle effects). If this is the case, then the trajectory of CA would be unaffected by cohort replacement (cf., Alwin and McCammon 2003). 6 Upon further analysis (not shown), we conrmed that religious attendance and national attachment had increased signicantly between surveys (and still had positive effects on CA at the later time point), but that the rise in educational attachment and the growing proportion of younger to older cohorts were far more dramatic. CA was also signicantly higher among Christians favoring large reductions in immigration; to our surprise, however, immigration preferences had actually grown more liberal over time. We address some of the possible causes and implications of this trend in the conclusion.

296

TABLE 4

Logistic Regression of Christian American Beliefs (CA) on Survey Year, All Respondents (N 1,992)a Model 1 Model 2 exp(b) 1.712*** 1.315** 2.103*** 2.508*** 0.406*** 0.362*** Model 3 exp(b) 2.224*** 5.722*** 2.021*** 1.538*** 1.076 1.614*** 0.693*** 0.523*** 0.335*** 1.201 1.769*** 2.923*** 0.581* 0.061***

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

b
Year: 2004 Christian Attendance Attachment Immigration Reduce some Reduce a lot Education Cohort ( , 1940) 1940 59 ! 1960 Female South Race (white) Black Other Christian Year Constant 2 2LL X 2 DX 2 df Nagelkerke R 2 0.537 0.274 0.743 0.920 2 0.900 2 1.016

sd(b) 0.095 0.095 0.100 0.155 0.218 0.093 2555.824 184.895*** 5 0.118

b
0.799 1.744 0.704 0.430 0.073 0.479 2 0.367 2 0.648 2 1.095 0.184 0.471 1.073 2 0.543 2 2.791

sd(b) 0.116 0.207 0.062 0.115 0.134 0.137 0.060 0.161 0.162 0.110 0.116 0.184 0.249 0.279 2032.524 523.300*** 8 0.399

b
2 0.110 1.160 0.704 0.431 0.075 0.487 2 0.368 2 0.651 2 1.107 0.180 0.590 1.071 2 0.547 0.987 2 2.245

sd(b) 0.388 0.295 0.062 0.115 0.134 0.137 0.060 0.161 0.162 0.110 0.116 0.184 0.249 0.404 0.337 2026.714 5.810* 1 0.402

exp(b) 0.896 3.189*** 2.023*** 1.539*** 1.078 1.628*** 0.692*** 0.521*** 0.331*** 1.198 1.803*** 2.919*** 0.578* 2.683* 0.106***

a For model 1, DX 2 is the difference in 2 2LL X 2 compared to a null model (intercept only). Models 2 and 3 are compared with Models 1 and 2, respectively. ***p , .001, **p , .01, *p , .05, p , .10 (2-tailed t-test).

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

297

favored a net decline, we now wish to see whether selective intensication on the part of the religious majority was sufciently powerful to overcome the attenuating effects of compositional change. In the nal model, we add a multiplicative term for the interaction between religious afliation (Christian/ non-Christian) and survey year. If selective intensication explains the overall increase in CA over time, then the coefcient for the interaction term should be signicant and positive, while the main effect of survey year should be nonsignicant. On inspecting the results in the last column, we see that both expectations are conrmed. Net of compositional change, the increase in odds of a very important response was 2.7 times greater for Christians than for non-Christians. Because the coefcient for survey year is not signicant, we can infer that there was no discernable net change in CA among non-Christians, corroborating our earlier inference that the existing gap between Christians and non-Christians has grown even larger over time.

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

PATTERNS OF CHANGE WITHIN THE CHRISTIAN COMMUNITY As our ndings up to this point make clear, the idea of a Christian America is an issue about which Christians and non-Christians remain deeply divided, and all the more so in times of international and religious conict. But how do these unsettling times affect previous differences within the Christian community itself? Does the rising belief in a Christian America promote greater consensus around religious conceptions of national identity, or does it only widen existing divisions between various segments of the Christian community? To answer these questions, we conclude our empirical analysis by examining the patterns of change in CA among different groups of Christians. Although we know that CA was higher among Christians, on average, at the end of our period than at the beginning, Hypothesis 6a predicted that the rate of intensication should be signicantly greater in those categories of Christians where CA was already stronger. Thus, we expected that CA would increase more among evangelicals and black Protestants than among mainline Protestants and other Christian groups, as well as among Christians who attend service frequently, display strong national attachment, and advocate greater restrictions on immigration. As before, we use logistic regression to estimate the expected change in odds that Christianity is regarded as very important for being truly American, holding other factors constant. To see whether the change in odds differed signicantly between categories of Christians, we add multiplicative terms representing the interaction between survey year and the other independent variables of interest. (For ease of interpretation, the independent variables with hypothesized interaction effects are coded as dichotomous.) To ensure that positive coefcients for each interaction term represent positive changes in the dependent variable over time, categories of each independent variable with the

298

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

smallest observed increase in CA are used as reference categories. The results are presented in table 5. Since we know that the only signicant rise in CA took place among Christian Americans, it is not surprising that the coefcient for survey year in Model 1 is signicantly positive. Overall, the observed odds of a very important response among Christians doubled in size between 1996 and 2004. Because it is still signicant, and even more positive, when we add the other independent variables (Model 2), we can safely conclude that this result is not explained by compositional change. More striking is that the main effect of survey year is no longer signicant when the interaction terms are included (Model 3). Thus, although there may have been a modest (28 percent) increase in odds for Christians as a whole, most of the net change in CA evidently occurred in those categories where the interactions with survey year are signicant. Consistent with our prediction, CA experienced fairly substantial growth in the category of frequent churchgoers. While the net odds of a very important response did not change signicantly for less-than-weekly attendance, they increased by a factor of 2.7 if attendance is at least a weekly occurrence, almost tripling the previous gap in odds between frequent and less frequent attendees. In other cases, however, the patterns are more suggestive of upward convergence, or intensication from below. Among mainline Protestants, for example, the net odds of a very important response were only 42 percent as great as for evangelicals (and black Protestants) in 1996, but by 2004 the gap had all but disappeared. For the Other Christian category, the rise in CA is exceptionally dramatic. At the end of our period, the estimated odds were more than ve times as great as at the beginning, and about 70 percent as great as for evangelicals (compared with an odds ratio of 13 percent in 1996). Although these exceptions are inconsistent with our prediction of intensication at the top, we believe they are most likely related to compositional changes (H5) for which analysis was unable to control.7 In the case of mainline Protestants, we suspect that the much of relative increase in CA in this shrinking category was due to attrition by the weakly committed into the category of religious nones (cf., Hout and Fischer 2002). Although limitations of our data do not permit us to conrm this directly, it would mean that the remaining contingent of mainliners is increasingly composed of strong believers in a CA. If this is what occurred, then the nding would be a byproduct of compositional change, rather than genuine intensication from below. Unlike that for mainline Protestants, the surge in CA among Other Christians coincided with a twofold increase in the relative size of the category.
An alternative possibility is that CA had already reached some practical upper limit in its usual denominational bastions, so that any further increase among Christians could only come from below. However, such a ceiling effect would not account for the exceptional rise in CA in the category comprising the most frequent churchgoers.
7

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

TABLE 5

Logistic Regression: Interactions with Survey Year, Christian Respondents (N 1,614)a Model 1 Model 2 exp(b) 2.041*** 1.208 2.076*** 2.249*** 0.527** Model 3 exp(b) 2.460*** 0.567*** 0.947 0.614*** 0.435** 3.424*** 0.662*** 0.718** 0.750 0.480*** 0.307*** 1.240 1.721*** 2.447** 0.662

b
Survey year Religion (Evangelical) Mainline Black Protestant Catholic Other Attendance: frequent Attachment: weak Immigration: liberal Education Cohort ( , 1940) 1940 59 ! 1960 Female South Race (white) Black Other Interactions: Religion (Evangelical) Mainline Black Potestant Catholic Other Attendance: frequent Attachment: weak Immigration: liberal Constant 2 2LLX 2 DX2 df Nagelkerke R 2 0.714 0.189 0.731 0.810 2 0.640

sd(b) 0.104 0.104 0.110 0.172 0.239

b
0.900 2 0.568 2 0.054 2 0.488 2 0.832 1.231 2 0.412 2 0.331 2 0.288 2 0.734 2 1.180 0.215 0.543 0.895 2 0.412

sd(b) 0.121 0.161 0.336 0.144 0.313 0.132 0.119 0.126 0.061 0.169 0.172 0.115 0.125 0.282 0.261

b
0.243 2 0.868 2 0.212 2 0.607 2 2.008 0.789 2 0.424 2 0.397 2 0.300 2 0.744 2 1.210 0.222 0.520 0.925 2 0.349

sd(b) 0.288 0.215 0.385 0.199 0.748 0.179 0.168 0.164 0.062 0.171 0.174 0.116 0.126 0.286 0.264

exp(b) 1.275 0.420*** 0.809 0.545** 0.134** 2.201*** 0.654* 0.672* 0.741*** 0.475*** 0.298** 1.248 1.682*** 2.521** 0.706

2 0.741

0.101 2110.320 145.118*** 5 0.114

0.476***

0.804

0.212 1847.794 262.526*** 10 0.295

2.234***

0.674 0.273 0.267 1.682 0.994 0.021 0.137 1.144

0.321 0.475 0.281 0.843 0.272 0.236 0.252 0.248 1825.681 22.113** 7 0.310

1.962* 1.313 1.307 5.376* 2.703*** 1.021 1.147 3.139***

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

Respondents with a non-Christian afliation or with no religious afliation are omitted. For Model 1, DX 2 is the difference in 2 2LLX 2 compared to a null model (intercept only). Models 2 and 3 are compared to models 1 and 2, respectively. ***p , .001, **p , .01, *p , .05, p , .10 (2-tailed t-test).
Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

299

300

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

As it turns out, roughly 40 percent of this growth was made up by newcomers of foreign origin. In light of our theory, we would expect recent immigrants of Christian faith to be especially attracted by the idea that being a Christian makes one more truly American. Although limitations of sample size prevent us from testing this inference, it would mean that the disproportionate rise in CA among Other Christians represents a special case of compositional change, rather than selective intensication. That is, even though the percentage of Christians among recent immigrants is slightly lower than in the population (Pew Forum 2008:47), a disproportionate inux of foreign-born Christians into the Other Christian category may have increased the proportion of members with strong CA beliefs.8 If these conjectures are borne out in future research, then our ndings indicate that the growing prevalence of CA among Christians resulted primarily from selective intensication among the most frequent churchgoers, regardless of denominational afliation and other characteristics. In other words, just as highly committed Christians are more likely than others to embrace the idea of a Christian America, they are also the most prone to nd comfort in this belief during unsettling times. It also means that, within any given Christian denomination, the most frequent churchgoers have drawn even further ahead of less active Christians in promoting religious conceptions of American identity. From the formers perspective, being a pious Christian may be increasingly viewed as more American than mere nominal membership in a Christian denomination.

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

CONCLUSION Conceptions of national belonging inevitably fashion borders and boundaries between people like us and others who do not fully belong (Edgell r 2002; Smith et al. 1998; et al. 2006; Kunovich 2006; Lamont and Molna Zimmer 2003). Just as some men and women, Caucasians and African Americans, or Christians and Jews will be thought more or less prototypical of their sex, race, or religion than others, the symbolic boundaries of national belonging recognize shades and gradations of authenticity among those they designate as insiders with respect to the national community. Thus, while the acquisition of U.S. citizenship has traditionally sufced to make an Italian, Indonesian, or Ivorian immigrant into an American by nationality, the question of who is more truly American also depends on how understandings of
8 A large proportion of Roman Catholics are also immigrants (Pew Forum 2008:19), but the percentage was about the same in both of our samples, perhaps explaining why there was no net increase in CA for this group over time. It may also explain why the odds ratios for Roman Catholics are similar to those for mainline Protestants, especially if the latter are becoming more favorable toward CA due to selective attrition.

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

301

national belonging are aligned with other dimensions of social membership, including the boundaries of religious faith. Because symbolic boundaries can have practical consequencesaffecting r social prestige or even political clout (Kunovich 2006; Lamont and Molna 2002)it is perhaps only natural that the dominant groups in society would seek to align the boundaries of national belonging with membership in their particular communities. Thus, because America has always been a predominantly Christian country, many American Christians no doubt feel justied in viewing their religion as characteristic of the true American. Among individual Christians, the degree of alignment between religion and national identity also tends to reect the salience of both religious and national boundaries. Within the religious majority, belief in a Christian America (CA) grows signicantly stronger with intensity of religious commitment and national attachment. Substantial variation also exists between different segments of the Christian community itself. For example, CA appears to be especially prevalent in evangelical denominations, perhaps because they tend to position their own doctrines and practices as more orthodoxand more Americanthan those of other Christian traditions (Smith et al. 1998, 2000). In other instances, religious denitions of national identity may function primarily as a strategy for combating social and symbolic exclusion. Such a strategy might explain the special appeal of CA within the black Protestant community. Whatever their meaning for religious insiders, however, any religious understanding of American identity is likely to meet with skepticism, if not resentment, among those they symbolically marginalize. Understandably, non-Christian believers, as well as those with no religious afliation, overwhelmingly reject the notion that being truly American requires adherence to the dominant faith. Such opposition to a Christian America need not be construed as denying the objective importance of Christianity in American life. More likely, it represents a desire to downplay the symbolic signicance of religion for dening American identity, in favor of religious pluralism and tolerance for diversityvalues that also have deep roots in U.S. history. Given the diminishing ratio of Christians to non-Christians in the United States (Kosmin and Keysar 2009; Smith and Kim 2005), one might have expected the overall prevalence of CA beliefs to decline over time. Yet as we saw, Americans were even more likely to espouse these beliefs after the turn of the century that they had been just a decade before. As of 1996, 38 percent of our respondents regarded being a Christian as very important for being truly American; by 2004, the gure had risen to nearly 50 percent (see also Pew Research Center 2006). This trend was all the more surprising given the weaker salience of religious boundaries among better educated Americans, as well as among those born in more recent decades. As Americans became better educated, and as older generations gave way to younger ones, the appeal of a Christian American should have been on the decline.

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

302

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

Although demography alone would not have predicted a surge in CA, our reasoning was basically correct. When we took these trends into account, our models predicted an even greater increase in the absence of compositional change. At the same time, the fact that CA experienced a signicant rally at all makes it clear that demography in this instance is not destiny, at least in the near term. For example, it may be, as some have argued, that certain cataclysmic events since the turn of the century have enhanced the importance of religion to the self-understandings of many Americans (Huntington 2004). Such period effects also have precedent in the literature on religious politicization and deprivatization (Regnerus and Smith 1998; Schwadel 2005). Yet, if the traumatic events of 9/11 or the war in Iraq have accentuated the connection between religion and national identity, we suspected that any resulting growth in CA would come primarily from American Christians. This hunch was well supported by the data. Among Christians, the proportion who saw Christianity as very important to American identity increased substantially between 1996 and 2004, assisted in part by a surge in religious commitment and national attachment. Yet, among non-Christians there was no signicant change in either direction, even when other predictors were taken into account. Within the religious majority, moreover, we found the largest proportionate increase in CA among Christians with at least weekly attendance at religious services. This was in line with our prediction that intensication will be especially pronounced, in unsettled times, where the appeal of CA had been strongest in the past. It also conrmed that events which provoke a growing religious divide can accentuate the moral boundary between more and less devout members of the dominant faith. Given the limitations of our data, we cannot be certain that any particular set of events was responsible for the rise in CA during our period of study. All we can state with condence is that the increase was not a direct byproduct of changes in the religious composition of society. At the same time, there are indications that the indirect impact of growing religious diversity has been more complex. On the one hand, the expansion of cultural tolerance and religious diversity in the last few decades may explain why younger and better-educated Christians tend to downplay the relevance of religion as a component of American identity. It may also account for the fact that outgroup hostility (as measured by immigration preferences) was on the decline during our period of study. On the other hand, Christians who did favor stronger restrictions on the rate of immigration were also more prone to erect religious barriers against non-Christian minorities. This suggests that the appeal of Christian America discourse may be related, not only to unsetting international events, but also to the salience of cultural issues in domestic policy debates, such as those involving immigration. Moreover, we speculated that recent immigrants who are Christians may be disproportionately attracted by the idea that their faith makes them more truly American. If so, then it is possible that the overall

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

303

prevalence of CA in the population will decline more slowly than the percentage of Christians among newly minted Americans. More generally, our ndings suggest that, when a controversial belief becomes more pronounced in the population, it may be symptomatic of a growing divergence of opinion around perennial cleavages. If such polarization raises the potential for future social conict, as some have argued (Bolzendahl and Brooks 2005; DiMaggio et al. 1996), then the extent of disagreement over American identity may be as at least as consequential as the prevalence of any particular denition. In practice, of course, the likelihood of conict also depends on differences in attitudes line up with other politically relevant characteristics and commitments (DiMaggio et al. 1996). In the present case, it is likely that belief in a Christian America often varies as much within traditional coalitions as between them. An older African American Protestant, for example, would likely agree on many social and political issues with a young, secular college graduate who happens to be white. (Both would likely have voted for the Democratic nominee in the most recent presidential election.) Yet, as our ndings suggest they would probably have very different views about the relevance of religion to matters of national identity. (We would expect a similar difference between, say, an economic conservative who is Jewish and a socially conservative evangelical, even if both typically vote Republican.) At the same time, there are reasons to believe that religious boundaries of national identity can have major political consequences, even rivaling those of race and national origin. In the 2008 presidential election, for example, the racial identity of the Democratic candidate seemed less of an obstacle to electoral success than certain (mistaken) beliefs about his religious convictions. Given his relatively narrow margin of victory, it seems doubtful that Barack Obama would have become the nations rst black President had he indeed been a Muslim instead of a Christian. It is also worth noting how questions about the candidates religion called attention to issues of race, and vice versa. Thus, rumors that Obama was privately a Muslim helped focus the media spotlight on the Northwest African side of his parental heritage. Meanwhile, Obamas long-standing membership in a West Chicago congregation not only substantiated his Christian convictions; it also brought to light some of the cultural differences between black Protestants and Christians of other races and denominations. Such examples underscore our contention that the salience of any one component of identity often depends on that of other dimensions with which it is aligned. They also serve as a reminder that religion is only one of the many symbolic resources available for constructing the boundaries of American identity. If U.S. Christians tend to dene America as a Christian nation, it would be of interest in future research to see whether other historically privileged categories exhibit similar proclivitiesfor instance, whether white Americans are more inclined than others to think of Caucasians as the

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

304

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

quintessential Americans, or whether Americans who speak only English are more prone insist that being a true American requires uency in English. If so, then a decline in the salience of religious boundaries over time would not, by itself, indicate that American identity has grown more pluralistic. It may instead signal that other axes of symbolic exclusion, such as language, ethnicity, or even patriotism, are on the ascendant.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL A supplementary section is located with the electronic version of this article at Sociology of Religion online (http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org).

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

REFERENCES
Alwin, Duane F., and Ryan J. McCammon 2003. Generations, Cohorts, and Social Change. In Handbook of the Life Course, edited by J. T. Mortimer, and M. J. Shanahan, 23 49. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. Bail, Christopher A. 2008. The Conguration of Symbolic Boundaries against Immigrants in Europe. American Sociological Review 73:37 59. Bolzendahl, Catherine, and Clem Brooks. 2005. Polarization, Secularization, or Differences as Usual? The Denominational Cleavage in US Social Attitudes Since the 1970s. The Sociological Quarterly 46:47 78. . 1994. Public Religions in the Modern World. Chicago: University of Chicago Casanova, Jose Press. Citrin, Jack, Cara Wong, and Brian Duff. 2001. The Meaning of American National Identity. In Social Identity, Intergroup Conict, and Conict Resolution, edited by R. Ashmore, L. Jussim, and D. Wilder, 71 100. New York: Oxford University Press. Davis, James A., Tom W. Smith, and Peter V. Marsden 2007. General Social Surveys, 1972 2006 [ICPSR04697-v2]. Chicago, IL: National Opinion Research Center. DiMaggio, Paul, John H. Evans, and Bethany Bryson. 1996. Have Americans Social Attitudes Become More Polarized? American Journal of Sociology 102:690 755. Edgell, Penny, Joseph Gerteis, and Douglas Hartmann. 2006. Atheists As Other: Moral Boundaries and Cultural Membership in American Society. American Sociological Review 71:211 34. Esses, Victoria M., John F. Dovidio, and Lynne E. Jackson 2001. The Immigration Dilemma: The Role of Perceived Group Competition, Ethnic Prejudice, and National Identity. The Journal of Social Issues 57:389 412. Hoge, D. R., and E. de Zulueta. 1985. Salience as a Condition for Various Social Consequences of Religious Commitment. Journal for the Scientic Study of Religion 24:21 38. Hout, Michael, and Claude S. Fischer 2002. Why More Americans Have No Religious Preference: Politics and Generations. American Sociological Review 67:165 90. Huddy, Leonie, and Nadia Khatib. 2007. American Patriotism, National Identity, and Political Involvement. American Journal of Political Science 51:63 77. Huntington, Samuel P. 2004. Who Are We? The Challenges to Americas National Identity. New York: Simon & Schuster.

CHRISTIAN AMERICA

305

Jones, Frank L., and Philip Smith. 2001. Diversity and Commonality in National Identities: An Exploratory Analysis of Cross-National Patterns. Journal of Sociology 37:45 63. Kosmin, Barry A., and Ariela Keysar. 2009. American Religious Identication Survey (ARIS 2008), Summary Report. www.americanreligionsurvey.aris.org (April 16, 2009). Kunovich, Robert M. 2006. An Exploration of the Salience of Christianity for National Identity in Europe. Sociological Perspectives 49:435 60. ` le. 2000. The Dignity of Working Men: Morality and the Boundaries of Race, Lamont, Miche Class, and Immigration. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ` le, and Vira g Molna r. 2002. The Study of Boundaries Across the Social Lamont, Miche Sciences. Annual Review of Sociology 28:167 95. Long, J. Scott. 1997. Regression Models for Categorical and Limited Dependent Variables. Thousand Oaks: Sage. lie, and Michael Wenzel. 1999. Social Discrimination and Tolerance in Mummendey, Ame Intergroup Relations: Reactions to Intergroup Difference. Personality and Social Psychology Review 3:158 74. Pattillo-McCoy, Mary. 1998. Church Culture as a Strategy of Action in the Black Community. American Sociological Review 63:767 84. Pew Forum. 2008. U.S. Religious Landscape Survey. Washington, DC: Pew Forum on Religion & Public Life. www.pewforum.org (February 6, 2008). Pew Research Center. 2006. Many Americans Uneasy with Mix of Religion and Politics. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center for the People and the Press. www. pewresearch.org (March 16, 2007). Regnerus, Mark D., and Christian Smith. 1998. Selective Deprivatization among American Religious Traditions: The Reversal of the Great Reversal. Social Forces 76:1347 72. Schwadel, Philip. 2005. Education and Churchgoing Protestants Views of Highly Politicized Christianity. Review of Religious Research 47:150 61. Servin-Gonzales, Mariana, and Oscar Torres-Reyna. 1999. Trends: Religion and Politics. Public Opinion Quarterly 63:592 621. Smith, Tom W., and Seokho Kim. 2005. The Vanishing Protestant Majority. Journal for the Scientic Study of Religion 44:211 23. . 2006. National Pride in Comparative Perspective: 1995/96 and 2003/04. International Journal of Public Opinion Research 18:127 36. Smith, Christian, Michael Emerson, Sally Gallagher, Paul Kennedy, and David Sikkink. 1998. American Evangelicals: Embattled and Thriving. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. . 2000. Christian America? What Evangelicals Really Want. Berkeley: University of California Press. Somers, Margaret R. 2006. Citizenship, Statelessness and Market Fundamentalism: Arendtian Right to Have Rights. In Migration, Citizenship, Ethnos, edited by Y. M. Bodemann, and G. Yurdakul, 35 62. New York/Hampshire: PalgraveMacmillan. Steensland, Brian, Jerry Z. Park, Mark D. Regnerus, Lynn D. Robinson, W. Bradford WilcoxRobert D. Woodberry 2000. The Measure of American Religion: Toward Improving the State of the Art. Social Forces 79:291 318. Swidler, Ann. 1986. Culture in Action: Symbols and Strategies. American Sociological Review 51:273 86.

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

306

SOCIOLOGY OF RELIGION

Voas, David, Alasdair Crockett, and Daniel V.A. Olson 2002. Religious Pluralism and Participation: Why Previous Research is Wrong. American Sociological Review 67:212 30. Weil, Frederick D. 1987. Cohorts, Regimes, and the Legitimation of Democracy: West Germany Since 1945. American Sociological Review 52:308 24. Wilcox, Clyde, and Rachel Goldberg. 2002. Public Opinion on Church-State Issues in a Changing Environment. Journal for the Scientic Study of Religion 41:369 76. Wimmer, Andreas. 2008. The Making and Unmaking of Ethnic Boundaries: A Multilevel Process Theory. American Journal of Sociology 113:970 1022. Wuthnow, Robert. 2005. America and the Challenges of Religious Diversity. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Zimmer, Oliver. 2003. Boundary Mechanisms and Symbolic Resources: Towards a Process-Oriented Approach to National Identity. Nations and Nationalism 9:173 93. Zolberg, Aristide R. 2006. A Nation by Design: Immigration Policy in the Fashioning of America. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Downloaded from http://socrel.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on December 3, 2012

You might also like