You are on page 1of 17

RELATIONSHIPS OF TASK PERFORMANCE AND CONTEXTUAL PERFORMANCE WITH TURNOVER, JOB SATISFACTION, AND AFFECTIVE COMMITMENT

James R. Van Scotter


University of Memphis, Memphis, TN, USA

The effects of task performance and contextual performance on turnover, job satisfaction, and affective organizational commitment were examined for two samples of Air Force mechanics. Supervisor ratings of task performance and contextual performance were obtained in 1992 (N = 419) for one sample and in 1993 for the second sample (N = 991). In both samples, task performance and contextual performance predicted turnover and job satisfaction in 1996. Task performance predicted reenlistment eligibility and promotion eligibility in the 1992 sample, but only reenlistment eligibility in the 1993 sample. Contextual performance only predicted promotion eligibility in the 1992 sample, but predicted both outcomes in the 1993 sample. Results support the distinction between task performance and contextual performance.

AFFECTIVE COMMITMENT It is becoming apparent that job performance is not a one-dimensional construct. As interest grows in the type of helpful, cooperative, and innovative job performance behavior that Borman and Motowidlo (1993) labeled as ``contextual performance,'' it becomes more important to understand its influence on organizational and individual outcomes. It would be especially valuable to learn whether or not contextual performance predicts different kinds of outcomes than other dimensions of job performance. It has been widely assumed that contextual performance and related elements of performance such as organizational citizenship behavior (OCB: Bateman & Organ, 1983; Smith, Organ, & Near, 1983), prosocial organizaDirect all correspondence to: James R. Van Scotter, Fogelman College of Business and Economics, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN 38152, USA; E-mail: jvanscot@memphis.edu Human Resource Management Review, Volume 10, Number 1, 2000, pages 7995 All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. Copyright # 2000 by Elsevier Science Inc. ISSN : 1053 4822

80

HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT REVIEW

VOLUME 10, NUMBER 1, 2000

tional behavior (Brief & Motowidlo, 1986), and organizational spontaneity (George & Brief, 1992) contribute to organizational effectiveness. Unfortunately, this belief is based more on logical arguments than empirical evidence (Borman & Motowidlo, 1993; Organ, 1997). Studies have focused on the antecedents of contextual performance more than its outcomes. Research showing its effects on supervisory ratings of employees' overall effectiveness (e.g., Borman, White, & Dorsey, 1995; Motowidlo & Van Scotter, 1994; Van Scotter & Motowidlo, 1996) supports the value of contextual performance in work settings. If contextual performance influences decisions about employees' overall effectiveness, it also seems likely to influence supervisors' decisions about whether or not an employee should be considered for a promotion or rewarded in other ways. If contextual performance leads to positive outcomes, it should also be associated with higher levels of job satisfaction (Steers & Porter, 1983), organizational commitment (Mowday, Porter, & Steers, 1982), and lower levels of turnover. Thus, research examining the impact of contextual performance on a wider range of criteria may be of practical and theoretical value (Borman & Motowidlo, 1997; Podsakoff & MacKenzie, 1997). As an initial effort, this study investigates the usefulness of contextual performance and task performance as predictors of turnover, reenlistment eligibility, promotion eligibility, job satisfaction, and organizational commitment over time. Task performance and contextual performance ratings were obtained in 1992 for one sample (N = 419) of Air Force mechanics and in 1993 for another sample (N = 991). Self-report measures and organizational criteria were obtained in 1996. BACKGROUND A growing body of research has investigated various aspects of contextual performance (Borman & Motowidlo, 1993), OCB (Smith et al., 1983), prosocial organizational behavior (Brief & Motowidlo, 1986), and organizational spontaneity (George & Brief, 1992). Although these constructs emerged from different research streams with different traditions and objectives, they have much in common. They all focus on job performance behaviors that are more discretionary and interpersonally oriented than task performance, but are still expected to meet important organizational needs. Contextual performance by Borman and Motowidlo (1993) is the least restrictive of these constructs. It is based on the characteristics of the behaviors, without reference to role expectations, the actor's intentions, or the expected beneficiaries. For convenience, I will refer to all of them, generically, as contextual performance throughout the rest of this article. Recent studies (e.g., Borman et al., 1995; Motowidlo & Van Scotter, 1994; Van Scotter & Motowidlo, 1996) support the distinction between task performance and contextual performance made by Borman and Motowidlo (1993). When employees use technical skills and knowledge to produce goods or

OUTCOMES OF CONTEXTUAL PERFORMANCE

81

services through the organization's core technical processes, or when they accomplish specialized tasks that support these core functions, they are engaging in task performance. When employees voluntarily help coworkers who are getting behind, act in ways that maintain good working relationships, or put in extra effort to complete an assignment on time, they are engaging in contextual performance. It has been suggested that contextual performance benefits organizations in several ways. Contextual performance behaviors involving persistence, effort, compliance, and self-discipline are expected to increase the effectiveness of individual workers and managers (Motowidlo, Borman, & Schmit, 1997; Podsakoff & MacKenzie, 1997). Helpful, considerate, and cooperative behaviors are expected to increase work group effectiveness and improve organizational coordination and control by reducing friction among organizational members and promoting a social and psychological context that facilitates task performance (Borman & Motowidlo, 1993; Smith et al., 1983). Innovative and voluntary behaviors enhance an organization's ability to solve unanticipated problems and adapt to change. In the aggregate, these behaviors are expected to improve organizational efficiency by freeing up resources that would otherwise be needed to handle disciplinary problems, solve communication difficulties, resolve conflicting demands, or provide closer monitoring of employee performance (Motowidlo et al., 1997). It is not difficult to make the case that employees who follow instructions, display initiative, persist on difficult tasks, cooperate with others effectively, or voluntarily act on the organization's behalf contribute more to the organization than employees who do not. While a single act of contextual performance is not likely to earn anyone a pay raise or a promotion, over time and situations, employees' contextual performance should influence supervisors' decisions about their contribution to the organization and potential for advancement. Bateman and Organ (1983) suggested that contextual performance might show an employee's willingness to help the organization. Voluntary behaviors may also be used to demonstrate skills and abilities that are required for advancement, but are not needed in the employee's present job. Personnel Selection There is already considerable evidence that, for at least some jobs, contextual performance may be a valuable addition to the job performance criteria used in personnel selection (Borman et al., 1995; Motowidlo & Van Scotter, 1994; Van Scotter & Motowidlo, 1996). These studies have focused on individual differences (Motowidlo et al., 1997) that predict the extent to which employees engage in these behaviors. Results support the distinction between contextual performance and task performance made by Borman and Motowidlo (1993). The evidence shows that supervisors weigh task performance and contextual performance about equally when judging employees' overall performance (Borman & Motowidlo, 1997).

82 Turnover

HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT REVIEW

VOLUME 10, NUMBER 1, 2000

Employee turnover is an especially important outcome for many organizations, yet few studies have examined the impact of contextual performance on turnover. The nature of contextual performance suggests one very basic link between contextual performance and turnover. Contextual performance is a form of discretionary behavior that demonstrates an employee's willingness to participate in the organization and interact with other members. In contrast, turnover is described as the culmination of a series of withdrawal behaviors. Although other factors certainly influence participation or turnover, higher levels of participation seem likely to be associated with lower levels of turnover and lower levels of participation seem likely to be associated with higher levels of turnover. Organizations also need to ensure that effective performers are rewarded and encouraged to stay. Ineffective performers must be informed that their substandard performance must be improved or they will risk administrative action or involuntary termination (Kerr, 1975; Steers & Porter, 1983). Motowidlo et al. (1997) suggested that employees learn which types of behaviors are rewarded by supervisors and adapt their behavior in ways that are positively reinforced. Thus, an employee's contextual performance can be expected to influence the favorability of the feedback an employee receives concerning opportunities for advancement and continued membership in the organization, in the same way feedback about the value of task performance does. After receiving feedback, employees may decide to modify their performance or begin searching for another job. The costs or benefits of turnover depend on who leaves and who stays (Mobley, 1982). The meta-analysis of 24 studies (total N = 7,717) by McEvoy and Cascio (1987) reported a mean correlation of r = 0.28 between performance and turnover, suggesting that better performers are less likely to leave. Unfortunately, they did not distinguish between task performance and contextual performance. Turnover is considered to be functional when poorer performers leave. Podsakoff and MacKenzie (1997) suggest the type of helping behaviors included in contextual performance might tend to decrease dysfunctional turnover, by helping to maintain a more pleasant and cohesive work environment. This is the type of turnover that occurs when better performers leave, presumably, to obtain better pay, opportunities for advancement, or working conditions. Job Satisfaction Job satisfaction has stimulated a great deal of research interest, partly because it is viewed as important in its own right and partly because of its association with other important outcomes. In a recent meta-analysis, Organ and Ryan (1995) found an average corrected correlation of r = .28 (N = 2,845) between job satisfaction and behaviors much like those that comprise contextual performance. Like Petty, McGee, and Cavender (1984), they did not

OUTCOMES OF CONTEXTUAL PERFORMANCE

83

have enough evidence to determine the direction of the relationship. With a few exceptions, research in this area has been cross-sectional in nature, making it difficult to establish the direction of relationships. Steers and Porter (1983) suggested that performance leads to rewards and rewards lead to satisfaction. In their view, employees are satisfied when they receive outcomes that are valued and when they feel they have been treated fairly. However, they also acknowledged that employees might find some tasks rewarding or enjoyable by themselves. Many of the interpersonal aspects of contextual performance, especially those that involve expressing oneself seem likely to fit in this category. Podsakoff and MacKenzie (1997) suggested contextual performance increases employees' job satisfaction by making the work context more pleasant and supportive. This is consistent with the description by Borman and Motowidlo (1993) of contextual performance as a category of behavior that is valuable to organizations because it helps support and maintain the psychological and social context in which task activities are performed. Both approaches agree that employees with more effective contextual performance are likely to be more satisfied with their jobs. Organizational Commitment Employees with high levels of organizational commitment are more work-oriented than other employees. They get more satisfaction from work and view their jobs as fulfilling more of their personal needs. As a result, they are willing to exert considerable effort on behalf of the organization (Mowday et al., 1982). Research (e.g., Williams & Hazer, 1986) supports the view that job satisfaction is an antecedent of affective organizational commitment. Podsakoff and MacKenzie (1997) suggested that by helping to create a more attractive work environment, contextual performance might increase employee commitment and improve retention. The model by Steers and Porter (1983) also suggests that as organizational commitment leads to more effective contextual performance, the employee would receive proportionally higher levels of rewards, which would lead to increased satisfaction and higher commitment. Summary Employees who are more helpful, cooperative, and team-oriented are more likely to be judged effective employees. They are also more likely to receive positive supervisory feedback concerning their chances of advancing to the next level of the organization and more likely to be encouraged to remain with the organization. As a result, they are expected to be more satisfied with their jobs, more highly committed to the organization, and less likely to leave it. Finding that contextual performance explains variance in eligibility for promotion, eligibility to reenlist, actual turnover, or employees' affective

84

HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT REVIEW

VOLUME 10, NUMBER 1, 2000

reactions to their jobs and organizations beyond what is explained by task performance would provide additional evidence of its value. METHOD Samples Two samples were available for this study. Members of the first sample were enlisted Air Force mechanics (N = 419) who participated in the 1992 study (Motowidlo & Van Scotter, 1994). The second sample was comprised of a similar group of mechanics (N = 991) who participated in the 1993 study (Van Scotter & Motowidlo, 1996). Both groups were predominately white (85%), males (95%), and included Blacks (12%), Hispanics (2%), and persons of Asian descent (1%). When the studies were conducted about 50 percent of the participants had under 5 years service, 32 percent had 510 years of experience and the rest had over 10 years of service. Procedures In the 1992 study, each mechanic's task performance and contextual performance was rated by one of two supervisors. To eliminate same-source bias, each supervisor rated one and only one dimension. Only one of the raters was the formally designated supervisor, but it was required that each rater: (a) had observed the mechanic for at least 90 days, (b) had supervisory authority over the mechanic, and (c) was qualified in all aspects of the job. (For convenience, all raters are referred to as supervisors throughout the remainder of the article.) Supervisors were briefed on the study's purpose and rating procedures in group meetings. Most rated their subordinates at that time. Ratings were completed independently. Supervisors who could not attend a meeting were contacted in person and asked to participate. The same procedures were used in the 1993 study to obtain supervisors' ratings of their subordinates' task performance and two separate dimensions of contextual performance. Performance Feedback Supervisors were required to counsel mechanics who were determined to be ineligible for promotion or ineligible for reenlistment. At this time, they were required to sign a personnel form acknowledging that they were aware of the administrative action. This was separate from the regular performance feedback sessions conducted by their supervisors. Follow-Up Survey Job satisfaction and affective organizational commitment were measured with a follow-up survey. It was administered by maintenance training officials

OUTCOMES OF CONTEXTUAL PERFORMANCE

85

at each participant's location in the Summer of 1996. The response rate was 68.6 percent, yielding 528 useable responses. Predictors of Employee Outcomes Task Performance. For the 1992 sample, task performance was measured by 14 items adapted from critical incident dimensions developed for the Army's Project A (Campbell, 1987), supplemented with information from Air Force task analyses: ``In comparison to others in this AFSC (Air Force Specialty Code), how effective is this person in: (a) inspecting, testing, and detecting problems with equipment; (b) trouble-shooting; (c) performing routine maintenance; (d) repairing; (e) using tools and test equipment; (f) using technical documentation; (g) operating equipment; (h) planning and organizing work; (i) performing administrative duties; (j) working safely; (k) cleaning shop facilities; (l) inventorying tools; (m) cleaning and lubricating equipment components; (n) overall technical performance.'' Supervisors used a fivepoint scale ranging from 1 = much below average to 5 = much above average to rate their subordinates' performance on each item. The mean of the 14 items formed the task performance score ( = 0.95, N = 169). To preserve sample size, this mean was computed for all cases with ratings on at least 10 items. Before conducting the main portion of the 1993 study, factor analysis was used to identify a set of six homogenous items that measured task performance efficiently. This analysis was accomplished using ratings for an independent set of mechanics. The revised measure consisted of items a, c, d, e, g, and n above. The task performance score was computed as the mean of these six ratings. To preserve sample size, this mean was computed for all cases with ratings on at least five items. In the current sample,  = 0.94 (N = 776). The single-rater intraclass correlation for this measure was 0.50 in an independent sample of 192 mechanics that were rated by two supervisors (Van Scotter & Motowidlo, 1996). Contextual Performance. In the 1992 study, contextual performance was measured by 16 items developed to tap the construct described by Borman and Motowidlo (1993): ``While performing his or her job, how likely is it that this person would: (a) comply with instructions even when supervisors are not present; (b) cooperate with others in the team; (c) persist in overcoming obstacles to complete a task; (d) display proper military appearance and bearing; (e) volunteer for additional duty; (f ) follow proper procedures and avoid unauthorized shortcuts; (g) look for a challenging assignment; (h) offer to help others accomplish their work; (i) pay close attention to important details; (j) defend the supervisor's decisions; (k) render proper military courtesy; (l) support and encourage a coworker with a problem; (m) take the initiative to solve a work problem; (n) exercise personal discipline and self-control; (o) tackle a difficult work assignment enthusiastically; (p) voluntarily do more than the job requires to help others

86

HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT REVIEW

VOLUME 10, NUMBER 1, 2000

or contribute to unit effectiveness.'' Supervisors rated participants on a fivepoint scale ranging from 1 = not at all likely to 5 = extremely likely (Motowidlo & Van Scotter, 1994). The mean of the item scores formed the contextual performance score ( = 0.95, N = 298) for cases rated on at least 11 items. Job Dedication. In the 1993 study, two dimensions of contextual performance were rated by separate supervisors. Items measuring job dedication illustrated effort, initiative, persistence, and self-discipline. Factor analyses of 13 items, using a separate sample, identified eight unidimensional items. They ask supervisors to rate mechanics on a five-point scale according to how likely they are to: (a) put in extra hours to get work done on time, (b) pay close attention to important details, (c) work harder than necessary, (d) ask for a challenging work assignment, (e) exercise personal discipline and self-control, (f) take the initiative to solve a work problem, (g) persist in overcoming obstacles to complete a task, and (h) tackle a difficult work assignment enthusiastically. The job dedication score was computed as the mean of these items for cases with responses on at least six items. Six of the items ( b, d, e, f, g, and h above) were taken from the 1992 measure of contextual performance. The single-rater intraclass correlation was 0.69 in an independent sample of 168 mechanics that were rated by two supervisors (Van Scotter & Motowidlo, 1996). In the present study,  = 0.94 (N = 862). The second dimension of contextual performance was interpersonal facilitation. It includes helpful, considerate, and cooperative behaviors that facilitate other workers' performance. Factor analysis of 13 items, using data from an independent sample, identified seven unidimensional items. They ask supervisors to rate mechanics on a five-point scale according to how likely they are to: (a) praise coworkers when they are successful, ( b) support or encourage a coworker with a personal problem, (c) talk to others before taking actions that might affect them, (d) say things to make people feel good about themselves or the work group, (e) encourage others to overcome their differences and get along, (f ) treat others fairly, and (g) help someone without being asked. The interpersonal facilitation score was the mean of these ratings for cases with responses to at least six of the items. The single-rater intraclass correlation was 0.41 in an independent sample of 147 mechanics that were rated by two supervisors (Van Scotter & Motowidlo, 1996). In the present study,  = 0.89 (N = 862). Experience. For the 1992 sample, job experience was measured via a selfreport item. It was a grouped measure that was coded 1 for 140 persons with 12 years of experience in the Air Force, 2 for 58 persons with 3 4 years of experience, 3 for 108 persons with 5 8 years of experience, 4 for 64 persons with 9 12 years of experience, 5 for 32 persons with 13 16 years of experience, and 6 for 14 persons with 1720 years of experience. For the 1993 sample, personnel records indicating the number of months the mechanics had been on active duty were used as the experience measure.

OUTCOMES OF CONTEXTUAL PERFORMANCE

87

Employee Outcomes Turnover. Turnover was operationalized as a dichotomous variable signifying whether or not the subject was a member of the organization at the end of 1995. It was coded 0 for mechanics who had left the Air Force and 1 for those who stayed. Eligibility for Reenlistment. Eligibility for reenlistment was a dichotomous variable obtained from organizational records indicating whether or not an individual was or was not eligible to reenlist. Mechanics who had disciplinary problems, could not adapt to the Air Force, or were determined to be unfit for continued service for other reasons were not allowed to reenlist. This measure was coded 0 for mechanics who were not eligible to reenlist and 1 for those who were eligible. Eligibility for Promotion. Eligibility for promotion was a dichotomous variable obtained from organizational records indicating whether or not an individual could be considered for promotion. Mechanics who had failed to complete a job knowledge test required for promotion, were involved in disciplinary actions, or were deemed to be unfit for a higher grade were ineligible for promotion. This measure was coded 0 for mechanics who were not eligible and 1 for those who were eligible for promotion. In most cases, a mechanic's eligibility for promotion could be restored if his or her performance improved. Job Satisfaction. Overall job satisfaction was measured by asking participants how strongly they agreed or disagreed with five statements: ``Overall, I am happy to be in the Air Force; I am very pleased with the kind of work I do in the Air Force; I work in some very nice places; The people I work with are very pleasant; and, I am dissatisfied with the work I do in the Air Force (Reversed).'' They used a five-point response scale anchored with 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neutral; 4 = agree, and 5 = strongly agree and  = 0.64 (N = 333). Affective Organizational Commitment. Affective organizational commitment was measured with three items based on the instrument of Mowday et al. (1982). The mechanics were asked the strength of their agreement that: ``The Air Force is the finest organization I have ever worked with; I would not recommend an Air Force career to my friends or relatives (Reversed); I take pride in being a part of the Air Force.'' Responses were on the same five-point disagreeagree scale used for job satisfaction. For this sample,  = 0.72 (N = 336). Analysis Separate analyses were performed for subjects in the 1992 sample and subjects in the 1993 sample. Correlation and regression were used to estimate the relationships between performance, promotion eligibility, reenlistment eligibility, turnover, job satisfaction, and organizational commitment.

88

TABLE 1 Intercorrelations among Variables for the 1992 Sample 3 305 310 (1.0) 0.06 0.09* (1.0) 0.20** (1.0) 0.48** 0.01 0.08 0.10 0.64** 0.04 (0.64) (0.72) (1.0) 0.38**
a a

Variable 3.49 3.80 2.61 0.25 0.93 355 128 128 0.93 17.84 10.43

SD 0.69 0.73 1.46 0.43 0.25 0.25 3.38 2.45

(0.95)

0.25** 415 419 419

(0.95)

0.40** 0.18** 0.15* 0.07 0.44** 0.10

0.14** 0.12* 0.11*

0.16**

0.08

0.27**

0.18*

1. Task performance 2. Contextual performance 3. Experience 4. Turnover 5. Promotion eligibility 6. Reenlistment eligibility 7. Job satisfaction 8. Affective commitment

HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT REVIEW

0.08

0.24**

Notes: *p < .05 (one-tailed). **p < .01. a There were no cases with both of these variables. N = 95128 for correlations involving job satisfaction or affective commitment. N = 232 419 for intercorrelations among all other variables. The s are on the diagonal. Reliabilities for single-item measures estimated at 1.0.

VOLUME 10, NUMBER 1, 2000

TABLE 2 Intercorrelations among Variables for the 1993 Sample 3 909 925 (0.94) (1.0) 0.01 0.09** (1.0) 0.21** (1.0) 0.39** 0.01 0.04 0.10* 0.02 (1.0) (0.64) 0.52** (0.72) 0.32**
a a

Variable 3.58 3.52 3.62 991 991 991 861 333 336 71.43 2.03 0.93 0.90 16.92 10.33

SD 0.79 0.80 0.90 49.79 0.40 0.25 0.29 3.42 2.34

(0.94)

0.36** 910

(0.89)

OUTCOMES OF CONTEXTUAL PERFORMANCE

0.48**

0.36**

0.35** 0.13** 0.05 0.18** 0.14** 0.08 0.04 0.28** 0.08**

0.14** 0.11** 0.10**

0.23** 0.14** 0.12**

0.16**

0.14**

0.11*

0.22**

1. Task performance 2. Interpersonal facilitation 3. Job dedication 4. Experience 5. Turnover 6. Promotion eligibility 7. Reenlistment eligibility 8. Job satisfaction 9. Affective commitment

0.02

0.14**

Notes: *p < .05 (one-tailed). **p < .01. a There were no cases with both of these variables. N = 304336 for correlations involving job satisfaction or affective commitment, and N = 725991 for intercorrelations among all other variables. The s are on the diagonal. Reliabilities for single-item measures estimated at 1.0.

89

90

HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT REVIEW

VOLUME 10, NUMBER 1, 2000

To control for differences in mechanics' experience levels in the regression analysis, the experience variable was entered into the regression equation first, before any of the other independent variables. Either task or contextual performance was entered on the second step and the other one was entered on the third step. For the 1993 sample, the two facets of contextual performance, interpersonal facilitation, and job dedication, were entered as a set (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). Next, the correlations were adjusted for attenuation caused by measurement error (cf. Guilford, 1965) and the regression analyses were reaccomplished. This helped ensure that the effects of measurement error did not affect the conclusions. RESULTS The zero-order correlations for the 1992 sample are shown in Table 1. Both dimensions predicted mechanics' eligibility for promotion. Task performance and contextual performance in 1992 each predicted the level of job satisfaction reported by those who were still on active duty in 1996, but only contextual performance significantly predicted their organizational commitment (r = .24, p < .01). Task performance (r = .18, p < .01) and contextual performance (r = .12, p < .05) each predicted turnover. Intercorrelations for the 1993 sample are shown in Table 2. Interpersonal facilitation (r = .10, p < .01) and job dedication (r = .12, p < .01) predicted eligibility for promotion, but task performance did not. All three
TABLE 3 Hierarchical Multiple Regressions Showing Incremental Effects of Task Performance and Contextual Performance in the 1992 Sample Dependent variable Promotion eligibility (N = 229) Reenlistment eligibility (N = 229) Turnover (N = 229) Job satisfaction (N = 86) Affective commitment (N = 87)
Notes:

Step 1 Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp

R 2 0.03** 0.03** 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.18* 0.18* 0.01 0.01

Step 2 CP TP CP TP CP TP CP TP CP TP

R 2 0.01 0.01 0.00 0.03* 0.02 0.05** 0.04* 0.02 0.07* 0.01

Step 3 TP CP TP CP TP CP TP CP TP CP

R 2 0.01 0.01 0.02* 0.00 0.04** 0.01 0.01 0.03* 0.00 0.06*

*p < .05. **p < .01. Exp = experience, CP = contextual performance, and TP = task performance.

OUTCOMES OF CONTEXTUAL PERFORMANCE

91

TABLE 4 Hierarchical Multiple Regressions Showing Incremental Effects of Task Performance and Contextual Performance in the 1993 Sample Dependent variable Promotion eligibility (N = 812) Reenlistment eligibility (N = 709) Turnover (N = 812) Job satisfaction (N = 270) Affective commitment (N = 271)
Notes:

Step 1 Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp Exp

R 2 0.01** 0.01** 0.01* 0.01* 0.00 0.00 0.07* 0.07* 0.00 0.00

Step 2 IF, JD TP IF, JD TP IF, JD TP IF, JD TP IF, JD TP

R 2 0.01** 0.00 0.03** 0.01* 0.03** 0.02** 0.03* 0.00 0.02* 0.00

Step 3 TP IF, JD TP IF, JD TP IF, JD TP IF, JD TP IF, JD

R 2 0.00 0.01** 0.00 0.02** 0.00 0.01* 0.00 0.03* 0.00 0.03*

*p < .05. **p < .01. Exp = experience, IF = interpersonal facilitation, JD = job dedication, and TP = task performance.

performance variables significantly predicted turnover (r = .13) and reenlistment eligibility (r = .16). Table 2 also shows that task performance (r = .11, p < .05), interpersonal facilitation (r = .22, p < .01), and job dedication (r = .14, p < .01) each predicted the level of job satisfaction reported by those who were still on active duty in 1996, but only interpersonal facilitation significantly predicted their organizational commitment (r = .14, p < .01). Hierarchical regression results for the 1992 sample ( Table 3) show task performance predicted reenlistment eligibility, but contextual performance did not. Table 3 shows that contextual performance explained separate variance in self-reported job satisfaction (R2 = .03, p < .05) and affective organizational commitment in 1996 (R2 = .03, p < .05), but task performance did not. Results for the 1993 sample (Table 4) show that contextual performance explained a small but significant amount of additional variance in turnover (R2 = .01, p < .05) after controlling for the effects of task performance, but task performance did not account for variance in turnover over and above what was explained by contextual performance. Thus, the analyses with turnover as the criterion produced results that were opposite those found for the 1992 sample. As with the 1992 sample, contextual performance accounted for unique variance in affective commitment (R2 = .03, p < .01) and job satisfaction (R2 = .03, p < .01) beyond the effects of task performance, but task performance failed to account for a significant amount of variance beyond what was explained by contextual performance.

92

HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT REVIEW

VOLUME 10, NUMBER 1, 2000

TABLE 5 Results of ANOVAs Comparing the Contextual Performance and Task Performance of Leavers and Stayers for the 1992 Sample Variable M SD 0.74 0.68 0.70 0.61 N 234 76 232 73 F for test of means 4.23* 10.61** Contextual performance Stayers 3.85 Leavers 3.65 Task performance Stayers 3.56 Leavers 3.26
Notes: *p < .05. **p < .01.

Table 5 shows that mechanics from the 1992 sample who stayed in the Air Force had significantly higher task performance (F = 10.61, df = 1,303, p < .01) and contextual performance ratings (F = 4.23, df = 1,308, p < .05) than those who left the Air Force. Results for the 1993 sample were consistent with those reported above. Mechanics who stayed with the organization were rated significantly higher in interpersonal facilitation (F = 12.24, df = 1,923, p < .01), job dedication (F = 18.45, df = 1,908, p < .01), and task performance (F = 14.56, df = 1,907, p < .01) than those who left. Two more sets of regression analyses were conducted to gauge the effects of measurement error on the results. The results of hierarchical regression analyses for the 1992 and 1993 samples after adjusting the correlations for attenuation differed very little from the results shown in Tables 3 and 4. None of the results gave any reason to suspect that measurement error affected the study's conclusions. To save space, the results of these regressions are not shown here.
TABLE 6 Results of ANOVAs Comparing the Interpersonal Facilitation, Job Dedication, and Task Performance of Leavers and Stayers for the 1993 Sample Variable Interpersonal facilitation Stayers Leavers Job dedication Stayers Leavers Task performance Stayers Leavers
Note: **p < .01.

M 3.56 3.37 3.69 3.37 3.63 3.38

SD 0.77 0.88 0.87 0.97 0.78 0.81

N 735 190 725 185 727 182

F for test of means 12.24** 18.45** 14.56**

OUTCOMES OF CONTEXTUAL PERFORMANCE

93

DISCUSSION The results leave little doubt that contextual performance is related to important organizational criteria. In the 1993 sample, contextual performance explained a significant amount of incremental variance in promotion eligibility, reenlistment eligibility, and actual turnover, over and above the variance that task performance explained in those outcome variables. Across two samples, and several years, results consistently showed that task performance and contextual performance ratings for employees who remained with the organization were higher than the ratings for those who left (see Tables 5 and 6). Thus, the turnover that occurred between 1992 and 1995 in the first sample, or between 1993 and 1995 in the second sample appears to have been functional for the organization (Mobley, 1982). These results are compatible with the meta-analysis of McEvoy and Cascio (1987) showing a negative correlation between performance and turnover. The relationships between task performance, contextual performance, and job satisfaction and organizational commitment were very consistent across the two samples. Employees whose contextual performance was higher also reported being more satisfied with their jobs and more committed to the organization (Tables 1 and 2). Hierarchical regression analyses (Tables 3 and 4) showed that contextual performance explained additional variance in job satisfaction or organizational commitment, over what was explained by task performance, but task performance did not explain significant incremental variance in either of these dependent variables. The inconsistent results for hierarchical regressions predicting turnover and reenlistment eligibility in the 1992 and 1993 samples (Tables 3 and 4) were a surprise. In retrospect, they may not be too hard to explain. The performance measures used in the 1993 study differed from those in the 1993 study. Correlational results for the 1993 study (Table 2) suggest that refining them was generally useful. Although comparing the incremental variance accounted for by a single variable (task performance) with that accounted for by a set of variables (interpersonal facilitation and job dedication) is theoretically meaningful (Cohen & Cohen, 1983), comparing results for different measures across studies may not be. The time lag between obtaining supervisory ratings and criterion data also differed in the two samples. Given these differences, the amount of consistency in the results for these two samples is impressive. It is hard to conclude that contextual performance is not related to important outcomes. Even if contextual performance has not been formally incorporated in job performance criteria, the results show that supervisors, recognize it, value it, and reward it. Finding that task and contextual performance predict different outcomes supports the distinction Borman and Motowidlo (1993) made between task performance and contextual performance.

94

HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT REVIEW

VOLUME 10, NUMBER 1, 2000

REFERENCES
Bateman, T. S., & Organ, D. W. (1983). Job satisfaction and the good soldier: The relationship between affect and employee ``citizenship.'' Academy of Management Journal, 26, 587 595. Borman, W. C., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1993). Expanding the criterion domain to include elements of contextual performance. In N. Schmitt, & W. Borman (Eds.), Personnel selection in organizations ( pp. 7198). New York: Jossey-Bass. Borman, W. C., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1997). Task and contextual performance: The meaning for personnel selection research. Human Performance, 10, 99 109. Borman, W. C., White, L. A., & Dorsey, D. W. (1995). Effects of ratee task performance and interpersonal factors on supervisor and peer performance ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 168 177. Brief, A. P., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1986). Prosocial organizational behavior. Academy of Management Review, 11, 710 725. Campbell, J. P. (1987). Improving the selection, classification, and utilization of Army enlisted personnel: Annual report, 1985 fiscal year. ARI Technical Report 746. Arlington, VA: Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlational analysis for the behavioral sciences, (2nd edn.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. George, J. M., & Brief, A. P. (1992). Feeling gooddoing good: A conceptual analysis of the mood at work organizational spontaneity relationship. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 310 329. Guilford, J. P. (1965). Fundamental statistics in psychology and education, (4th edn.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Kerr, S. (1975). On the folly of rewarding A, while hoping for B. Academy of Management Journal, 18, 769 783. McEvoy, G. M., & Cascio, W. F. (1987). Do good or poor performers leave? A meta-analysis of the relationship between performance and turnover. Academy of Management Journal, 30, 744 762. Mobley, W. H. (1982). Some unanswered questions in turnover and withdrawal research. Academy of Management Review, 7, 111 116. Motowidlo, S. J., Borman, W. C., & Schmit, M. J. (1997). A theory of individual differences in task and contextual performance. Human Performance, 10, 71 83. Motowidlo, S. J., & Van Scotter, J. R. (1994). Evidence that task performance should be distinguished from contextual performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 475 480. Mowday, R. T., Porter, L. W., & Steers, R. M. (1982). Employeeorganization linkages: The psychology of commitment, absenteeism, and turnover. New York: Academic Press. Organ, D. W. (1997). Organizational citizenship behavior: It's construct clean-up time. Human Performance, 10, 85 97. Organ, D. W., & Ryan, K. (1995). A meta-analytic review of attitudinal and dispositional predictors of organizational citizenship behavior. Personnel Psychology, 48, 775 800. Petty, M. M., McGee, G. W., & Cavender, J. W. (1984). A meta-analysis of the relationships between individual job satisfaction and individual performance. Academy of Management Review, 9, 712 721. Podsakoff, P. M., & MacKenzie, S. B. (1997). Impact of organizational citizenship behavior on organizational performance: A review and suggestions for future research. Human Performance, 10, 133 151.

OUTCOMES OF CONTEXTUAL PERFORMANCE

95

Smith, C., Organ, D. W., & Near, J. P. (1983). Organizational citizenship behavior: Its nature and antecedents. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 453 463. Steers, R. M., & Porter, L. W. (1983). Motivation and work behavior, (3rd edn.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Van Scotter, J. R., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1996). Interpersonal facilitation and job dedication as separate facets of contextual performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 525 531. Williams, L. J., & Hazer, J. T. (1986). Antecedents and consequences of satisfaction and commitment in turnover models: A reanalysis using latent variable structural equation methods. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 219 231.

You might also like