You are on page 1of 360

Maintaining Relationships

Through c ommunication

F&tional, Contextual,
an d Cultural Variations
LE/fYS SERIES ON PIXSONAL Ri3-ATlONSHlP.S
Steve Duck. Series Editor

Bennett l Time and Intimacy: A New Science of Personal


Relationships

Canary/Dainton l Maintaining Relationships Through


Communication: Relational, Contextual, and Cultural
Variations

Christopher l To Dance the Dance: A Symbolic Interaction


Exploration of Premarital Sexuality

Goodwin/Cramer l Inappropriate Relationships:


The Unconventional, The Disapproved, and The Forbidden

Hone cutt/Cantrdl l Cognition, Communication


an c? Romantic Relationships

M;IIer/AIberts/Hecht/Trost/KrizeJc l Adolescent
Relationships and Drug Use

Monsour l Men and Women as Friends: Relationships Across


the Life Span in the 2 1 st Century

www wlhnllm rnm


Maintaining Relationships
Through c omrnunication

Relational, Contextual,
an d Cultural Variations

Edited by

Daniel J. Canary
Arizona State University
and
Marianne Dainton
La Salle University

LAWRENCE ERLBAUM ASSOCIATES, PUBLISHERS


2003 Mahwah, New Jersey London
Copyright 0 2003 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any
form, by photostat, microform, retrieval system, or any other means,
without prior written permission of the publisher.

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers


10 Industrial Avenue
Mahwah, NJ 07430

Cover design by Kathryn Houghtaling Lacey

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Maintaining relationships through communication : relational, contextual,


and cultural variations / edited by Daniel J. Canary and Marianne
Dainton.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN O-8058-3989-5 (cloth: alk. paper)
ISBN O-8058-3990-9 (pbk.: alk. paper)
1. Interpersonal relations. 2. Interpersonal communication I. Canary,
Daniel J. II. Dainton, Marianne.
HMllO6 .M34 2002
302.24~21 2002026373
CIP

Books published by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates are printed on


acid-free paper, and their bindings are chosen for strength and durability.

Printed in the United States of America


10 9 8 7 h 5 A ?
Contents
+t++ e

Series Foreword ix
...
x111
Preface
About the Contributors xvii

Overview

1 Definitions and Perspectives on Relational


Maintenance Communication
Kathryn Dindia

Part I: Maintaining Different Types


of Relationships

2 M aintaining Family Relationships 31


Sally Vogl-Bauer

3 Maintaining Romantic Relationships: Summary and 51


Analysis of One Research Program
Laura Stafford
\‘I -is=+ CONTENTS

4 M aintaining Friendships Throughout the Lifespan 79


Marianne Dainton, Elaine Zelley, Emily Langan

5 Maintaining Undesired Relationships 103


Jon A. Hess

Part II: Contextual Variations


in Maintaining Relationships

6 Maintaining Long-Distance Relationships 127


Brooks Aylor

7 Computer-Mediated Communication Effects on


Relationship Formation and Maintenance
141

Michael K. Rabby and Joseph Walther

8 Relationship Maintenance in Organizational Settings 163


Vincent R. Waldron

9 When Partners Falter: Repair After a Transgression


Tara M. Emmers-Sommer
185

Part III: Cultural Variations


in Maintaining Relationships

10 Relationship Maintenance in Same-Sex Couples 209


Stephen Haas

11 Relationship Maintenance in Intercultural Couples: An 23 1


Interdependence Analysis
Stanley 0. Gaines, Jr., and Christopher R. Agnew

12 Maintaining Marriages in Russia: Managing Social 255


Influences and Communication Dynamics
Deborah Ballard-Reisch, Daniel Weigel and Marat
Zaguidoulline
CONTENTS t. VII

13 Maintaining Relationships in Korea and the United 277


States: Features of Korean Culture that Affect
Relational Maintenance Beliefs and Behaviors
Young-Ok Yum and Daniel J. Canary

Part IV: Epilogue

14 Framing the Maintenance of Relationships Through 299


Communication: An Epilogue
Marianne Dainton

Author Index 323


Subject Index 335
This page intentionally left blank
Steve Duck
University of Iowa

his series from Lawrence Erlbaum is intended to review the prog-


ress in the academic work on relationships with respect of a broad array
of issues and to do so in an accessible manner that also illustrates its
practical value. The LEA series includes books intended to pass on the
accumulated scholarship to the next generation of students and to those
who deal with relationship issues in the broader world beyond the acad-
emy. The series thus not only comprises monographs and other aca-
demic resources exemplifying the multi-disciplinary nature of this area,
but also, in the future, textbooks suitable for use in the growing num-
bers of courses on relationshios.
The series has the goal of prloviding a comprehensive and current sur-
vey of theory and research in personal relationship through the careful
analysis of the problems encountered and solved in research, yet it also
considers the systematic application of that work in a practical context.
These resources not only are intended to be comprehensive assessments
of progress on particular “hot” and relevant topics, but also will be sig-
nificant influences on the future directions and development of the
study of personal relationships. Although each volume is focused and
centered: authors all attempt to place the respective topics in the
broader context of other research on relationships and within a range of
wider disciplinary traditions. The series already offers incisive and for-
k
\ -I+-=+ SERIES FORWARD

ward-looking reviews and also demonstrates the broader theoretical im-


plications of relationships for the range of disciplines from which the
research originates. Present and future volumes include original studies,
reviews of relevant theory and research, and new theories oriented to-
ward the understanding of personal relationships both in themselves
and within the context of broader theories of family process, social psy-
chology, and communication.
Reflecting the diverse composition of personal relationship study, read-
ers in numerous disciplines-social psychology, communication, sociology,
family studies, developmental psychology, clinical psychology, personality,
counseling, studies, gerontology, and others-will find valuable
and insightful perspectives in the series.
Apart from the academic scholars who research the dynamics and
processes of relationships, there are many other people whose work
takes them up against the operation of relationships in the real world.
For such people as nurses, the police, teachers, therapists, lawyers, drug
and alcohol counselors, marital counselors, and those who take care of
the elderly, a number of issues routinely arise concerning the ways in
which relationships affect the people whom they serve. Examples are
the role of loneliness in illness and the ways to circumvent it, the com-
plex impact of family and peer relationships upon a
attempts to give up the drug, the role of playground unpopularity on a
learning, the issues involved in dealing with the relational side of
chronic illness, the management of conflict in marriage, the establish-
ment of good rapport between physicians and seriously-ill patients, the
support of the bereaved, and the correction of violent styles of behavior
in dating or marriage. Each of these is a problem that may confront some
of the aforementioned professionals as part of their daily concerns and
each demonstrates the far-reaching influences of relationship processes
on much else in life that is presently theorized independently of rela-
tionship considerations.
The present volume is a good example of the concerns, since it at-
tends to something rarely given, until quite recently, significant research
attention. The study of relational maintenance is one of those topics over-
looked in the literature for several years yet now beginning to be regarded
as central to the field. If we do not understand the many ways in which
people sustain and manage their relationships day-to-day then we miss
much of the way in which relationships intertwine with everyday existence
and experience. The two editors are leaders in this development whose
amplification of the topic has led to its recognition as a key part of any un-
derstanding of relating, at any age. In this single volume they have gathered
together a list of distinguished authors who have many new and exciting
thoughts about the ways to conceptualize relational maintenance and its
impact in the lives of, for example, those who must live long distances
SERIES FORWARD +=a XI

apart for a while, those who must work with uncongenial colleagues, or
those who maintain relationships against a cultural background of
unacceptance or even strong social disapproval. The connections between
successful maintenance of relationships and their long term survival is ever
to the fore in the chapters here and the close causal connections between
failures of maintenance and breakdown of relationship are too obvious and
too important to overemphasize. For theorists, therapists, and the rest of
us, this theme is of immense significance and the present collection of
thinking on the topic represents one of the best collections to date.
This page intentionally left blank
ost sane people know that relationships require work. That is, part-
ners need to spend time and effort to maintain functional, satisfying rela-
tionships. Without such efforts, relationships tend to deteriorate. Of course,
one might rely on external inducements to keep a relationship intact
(Attridge, 1994). For example, one might use structural dependencies, in-
cluding irretrievable investments, to keep a partner locked within the con-
fines of a personal involvement (Johnson, 1999). However, this book is not
about using existing structures to maintain a personal relationship. Instead,
this book focuses on the communicative processes that people engage in to
keep their relationships stable and satisfactory. As Perlman (2001) observed,
our primary assumption is that “maintenance is what we do. In other words,
it is a process rather than, as some suggested, as state” (p. 360).
In our view, the examination of relational maintenance offers a rallying
point for people interested in discovering the behaviors that people utilize
to sustain various relationships. Theoretical models, research programs,
and individual studies have examined how people in a variety of relation-
ships keep those relationships defined in ways that they want them de-
fined. More precisely, students in communication, social psychology,
family studies, sociology, and related fields now possess a variety of articles
and chapters to read on this topic.
This anthology constitutes the third book that specifically focuses on the
topic of relational maintenance. The first, by Canary and Stafford (1994)
framed the area of study as one that emphasizes communication, social psy-
chology, and dialectics. It summarized the burgeoning research to that time,
hoping to provide traction for the construct. (About the same time [ 19931, a
-e= PREFACE

special issue of the Journal of Social and Personal Relationships on relational


maintenance research was edited by Dindia and Canary.) We are pleased to
note that years later the focus on relational maintenance processes has
grown both theoretically and empirically. Recent reviews of alternative ways
to look at relational maintenance processes testify to the ways that scholars
have recently explored the construct (e.g., Canary & Zelley, 2000; Dindia,
2000; Perlman, 2001). The second book, by Harvey and Wenzel(2001, also
published by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), emphasizes psychological pro-
cesses and social psychological models used to examine relational mainte-
nance. As indicated, the present volume focuses on communicative
processes that have been found critical in the maintenance and enhance-
ment of social and personal relationships, thereby complementing the
Harvey and Wenzel project.
Previous research has shown that how people maintain their relationships
through interaction is more than a temporary whim. For instance,
(1994) research clearly shows how conflict management behaviors are linked
to eventual divorce. Importantly, the relevance of maintenance behaviors ex-
tends beyond heterosexual, romantic involvements. Despite the importance
of communication and relational maintenance across relational types, how-
ever, little effort (if any) has been made to synthesize the insights into mainte-
nance processes that might hold true regardless of relational context or change
as the result of variation in context. Accordingly, the primary goal of this vol-
ume is to summarize and integrate the research on maintaining various rela-
tional types that occur in a variety of contexts.
We divide the book into three sections. The first section focuses on varia-
tions in maintaining different types of personal relationships. These include
kin, romantic, friendship, and (even) undesired relationships. The second
section focuses on contextual constraints on relationship maintenance. Such
contexts potentially affect the manner in which people sustain their per-
sonal involvements, and these include long-distance relationships, com-
puter-mediated relationships, associations within an organization, and
relationships that have experienced a transgression. The final section focuses
on cultural variations in relational maintenance. More specifically, this sec-
tion explores how people maintain gay and lesbian relationships, cultur-
ally-mixed relationships, and two chapters on maintaining relationships in
other nations (i.e., Russia, Korea). We do not claim to exhaust the many rela-
tional, contextual, and cultural factors that might affect how people main-
tain their relationships. Instead, we aim to provide the reader a
representation of the kinds of issues that have emerged regarding how com-
munication functions to maintain various kinds of personal relationships.
Each contributor was to address five issues. Our goal here was to have
the scholars address specific issues that readers would recognize from
chapter to chapter, without constraining the authors regarding how they
PREFACE _I

went about writing about the issues. The issues include: (1) assumptions
influencing their research; (2) specific communicative strategies and pro-
cesses identified by research for maintaining this relational type; (3) spe-
cial or unique characteristics of the relationship type or context that is
examined; (4) conclusions maintaining this type or aspect of relationships;
and (5) implications/directions for future research. We are pleased at the
creative manner in which each contributor examined these issues, and we
believe the reader will find the alternative approaches thought provoking.
In lieu of summarizing each chapter, we urge the reader to consider the
implications of the present chapters. Some of the chapters are written by es-
tablished, veteran scholars; some are composed by new scholars. We think
the range of issues they discuss in part reflects on how research in personal
relationships has emerged more generally-new issues concerning relation-
ships in modern society, which are often raised by new scholars, are ex-
plained through systematic, theoretically based research, the foundation of
which was laid by established scholars. Regardless, we hope that the reader
appreciates the various levels of seasoning we wanted to represent.
Many people have earned our respect and gratitude. First, we want to
thank the chapter authors. Each contributor presented material in a timely
and responsible fashion, and they energetically revised to meet our re-
quests for revision. We are grateful to all of the authors that each chapter in
this volume represents a positive writing and editing experience.
Next, we want to thank both Arizona State University and La Salle Uni-
versity for providing us with needed support. In particular, La Salle Uni-
versity offered Marianne a research leave in order to work on this project.
Moreover, our colleagues at Arizona State and at La Salle are wonderful in
their continued enthusiasm for our work.
To the people at LEA-Linda Bathgate, Karin Wittig Bates, and
Marianna Vertullo-we are indebted for providing the publication support
and careful editing needed. We are pleased not only to have this volume
published by LEA, but also that it is part of their personal relationships se-
ries, edited by Steve Duck. It amazes us how (again) Steve has stepped to
the fore in shaping the discipline of personal relationships, and we are
grateful for his efforts on our behalf.
Finally, we thank all of those people in our respective social and personal
networks who have kept us both fascinated and challenged by the prob-
lems of relationship maintenance. Most especially, we want to thank our
very patient partners. Without their efforts at relational maintenance, this
volume would not have been possible.

-Daniel J. Canary
-Marianne Dainton
April 2002
XVI -is= PREFACE

Attridge, M. (1994). B arriers to the dissolution of romantic relationships. In D. J. Ca-


nary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational maintenance (pp.
14 l-1 64). New York: Academic Press.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1994). Communication and relational maintenance. New
York: Academic Press.
Canary, D. J., & Zelley, E. D. (2000). Current research programs in relational mainte-
nance behaviors. In M. E. Roloff (Ed.), C ommunicationyearbook 23 (pp. 304-339).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Dindia, K. (2000). Relational maintenance. In C. Hendrick & S. S. Hendrick (Eds.),
Close relationships: A sourcebook (pp. 287-301).
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationships between marital pro-
cesses and marital outcomes. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Harvey, J. H., & Wenzel, A. (2001). Close romantic relationships: Maintenance and en-
hancement. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Johnson, M. I? (1999). P ersonal, moral, and structural commitment to relationships:
Experiences of choice and constraint. In J. M. Adams &IV H. Jones (Eds.), Hand-
book of interpersonal commitment and relationship stability (pp. 73-87). New York:
Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships (1993). Special Issue on Relational Mainte-
nance, 10, 163-304.
Perlman, D. (2001). Maintaining and enhancing relationships: Concluding commen-
tary. In J. H. Harvey &A. Wenzel (Eds.), Close romantic relationships: Maintenance
and enhancement (pp. 357-377). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Aboutthecontributors
M M
Christopher R. Agnew is currently an Associate Professor in the Department
of Psychological Sciences at Purdue University in West Lafayette, Indiana.
He received his doctoral degree from the University of North Carolina at
Chapel Hill in 1994. A social psychologist, his research interests in the in-
terpersonal realm include couple decision-making, the cognitive represen-
tation of relationships, commitment processes, and social network
interactions and influence. He has published widely, including articles in
the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, Personal Relationships, and the Journal of Social and Per-
sonal Relationships, as well as chapters in Advances in Population:
Psychosocial Perspectives and the Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology.
Brooks Aylor is an Assistant Professor at La Salle University, where he also
serves as Director of the Public Speaking Program. He teaches in the areas
of communication theory, interpersonal, argumentation, organizational
communication, and oral communication. His research interests include in-
terpersonal, political, and instructional communication. He has published
in Western Journal of Communication, Communication Quarterly, Commu-
nication Research Reports, Communication Teacher, and Journal of Commu-
nication Studies. He has also co-authored chapters in The I996 Presidential
Campaign: A Communication Perspective (Praeger, 1998) and The 2000
Presidential Campaign (Praeger, in press). Brooks received his doctorate in
Communication Studies from the University of Arizona and his MA in
Speech and Theater Arts from Arkansas State University.
Deborah Ballard-Reisch is a Professor in the Health Ecology Department at
the University of Nevada. Her research is international in focus, emphasiz-
ing the U.S., Russia, and Zimbabwe. Areas of interest include marital com-
mitment, relationship maintenance, and marital satisfaction; perceptions of
femininity and masculinity; the impact of feminism on culture;
status; health concerns; families coping with cancer; client/pro-
vider communication in health settings, and narrative theory and the social
sciences. In her spare time, she likes to travel with her children Stefan, age
XVIII -ts=+ ABOUT THE CONTRIBUTORS

13, and Alyssa, age 8, take pictures, go to movies and the theatre, eat at good
restaurants, hike, listen to music, and read.
Daniel Canary (PhD, USC, 1983) is a Professor in the Hugh Downs School of
Human Communication, Arizona State University. research focuses
on conflict management, relational maintenance, conversational argument,
and sex differences in communication. A member on several editorial
boards, Dan is also Editor, Western Journal of Communication. Dan enjoys
traveling, golfing, and writing songs.
Marianne Dainton is an Associate Professor of Communication at La Salle
University in Philadelphia. She received her PhD from The Ohio State Uni-
versity in 1994. research focuses on the symbolic exchanges
that facilitate relationship maintenance. Of particular interest are routine
and strategic maintenance efforts, and the maintenance of long-distance re-
lationships. She has published in Communication Monographs, the Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, Family Relations, Western Journal of
Communication, Communication Quarterly, Communication Reports, and
Communication Research Reports. She has also published numerous book
chapters, and is the co-editor of this volume.
Kathryn Dindia (PhD, Speech Communication, University of Washington,
198 1) is a Professor in the Department of Communication at the Univer-
sity of Wisconsin-Milwaukee. research interests include sex dif-
ferences and similarities in communication behavior; self-disclosure,
including, reciprocity of self-disclosure, self-disclosure and relationship de-
velopment, and sex differences in self-disclosure; and relational mainte-
nance strategies. Kathryn has published approximately 30 articles and book
chapters including articles in Psychological Bulletin, Human Communica-
tion Research, Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, Personal Rela-
tionships, and chapters in the Handbook of Personal Relationships and The
Handbook of Communication Skills. She co-edited Sex Differences and
Similarities in Communication, Communication in Personal Relationships,
and a special issue of the Journal of Social and Personal Relationships on Re-
lational Maintenance.
Tara M. Emmers-Sommer (PhD, 1995, Ohio U niversity) is Associate Profes-
sor, Department of Communication, University of Arizona.
research interests include problematic communication
in close relationships. work is published in the Journal
of Communication, Human Communication Research, Communication
Yearbook, Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, Personal Relation-
ships, Communication Quarterly, and Communication Studies.
Emmers-Sommer is also the co-author, along with Dan Canary, of the 1997
Guilford book, Sex and gender differences in personal relationships.
Stanley 0. Gaines, Jr. received his PhD in social psychology from the Univer-
sity of Texas at Austin in 199 1. Gaines currently is a Lecturer in Psychology,
Department of Human Sciences, Brunel University, in London.
book, Culture, Ethnicity, and Personal Relationship Processes, was pub-
ABOUT THE CONTRIBUTORS -+t- XIX

lished by Routledge in 1997. In addition, Gaines has authored or


co-authored more than 50 journal articles and book chapters.
Stephen M. Haas (PhD, Ohio State University) is an Assistant Professor in
the Department of Communication at the University of Cincinnati. His re-
search is in the areas of relational and health communication. Much of his
work has explored communication dimensions of persons living with
chronic illness including relationship maintenance in couples coping with
HIV or AIDS; the communicative management of uncertainty in illness;
and patient self-advocacy in physician-patient interactions. re-
search has been published in journals such as Human Communication Re-
search, Communication Monographs, the Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, and Health Communication.
Jon A. Hess (PhD, University of Minnesota, 1996) is an assistant professor in
the Department of Communication at the University of Missouri-Colum-
bia. His research interests focus on how people can successfully maintain
relationships under difficult circumstances. He has presented papers and
published articles on getting along with people we dislike, ways people reg-
ulate distance, and foundations of civility in personal relationships.
Emily Jane Langan (PhD, Arizona State University, 2001) is an Assistant Pro-
fessor in the Department of Communication at the University of Texas, San
Antonio. Her dissertation investigated attachment theory in young adult
friendships and associations with perceived friendship functions, friend-
ship quality, maintenance strategies, and relational perceptions. re-
search focuses on the development and maintenance of friendships,
compliance gaining and resistance, attachment theory, and nonverbal com-
munication.
Michael K. Rabby currently serves as an Assistant Professor of Communica-
tion at the University of Central Florida. He received his degree
from the Pennsylvania State University, and his PhD from Arizona State
University. His research interests include the impact of technology on a va-
riety of contexts, including organizations and personal relationships.
Laura Stafford is an Associate Professor in the Department of Journalism and
Communication at The Ohio State University. Her research interests lie
broadly in the domain of communication within personal relationships. She
received her PhD in Communication from the University of Texas at Aus-
tin, 1985. In addition to numerous journal articles, Laura has published two
books: with Dan Canary she coedited Communication and Relational
Maintenance (Academic) and she wrote interaction Between Parents and
Children (Sage) with Cherie Bayer.
Sally Vogl-Bauer (PhD 1994, University of Kentucky) is an Associate Profes-
sor in the Department of Communication at the University of Wiscon-
sin-Whitewater. Her research interests in family communication focus
primarily on parent-child relationships and family maintenance patterns.
She has been published in communication and family relations journals.
KY ++ ABOUT THE CONTRIBUTORS

Vincent R. Waldron (PhD, Ohio State University) is Associate Professor of


Communication Studies at Arizona State University West, where he cur-
rently serves as Dean of the Division of Collaborative Programs. He special-
izes in the use of quantitative research methods to study interpersonal
communication in personal and work relationships. Vince has published a
series of articles and book chapters on such topics as the communication of
emotion at work, upward influence processes, and relationship mainte-
nance in supervisory relationships. Among his current projects are a longitu-
dinal study of social support processes in a retirement community, and an
examination of relationship maintenance patterns in a large data processing
organization. His work has been funded by such entities as the State of Ari-
zona, the Del Webb Corporation, and the Templeton Foundation.
Joseph B. Walther (PhD, U niversity of Arizona, 1990) is an associate profes-
sor of communication at Cornell University. His research focuses on the in-
terpersonal dynamics of communication via computers in personal
relationships, work groups, and educational settings. He is the creator of
original theories in this area and the author of numerous empirically based
research articles. Walther also serves on the editorial boards of a number of
international journals in communication, management, and other fields.
Dan Weigel is an Associate Professor and Human Development Specialist for
the University of Nevada Cooperative Extension. His research interests in-
clude marital relationships-especially communication, commitment, and
change-as well as parent-child interaction. His teaching areas include par-
ent education, child development, and family communication. When not
working, he likes golfing, skiing, backpacking, and spending time with fam-
ily and friends.
Young Ok Yum (PhD, 2000, Th e Pennsylvania State University) is an Assis-
tant Professor in the Department of Speech Communication, Theatre, and
Dance at Kansas State University, Manhattan. Her research interests in-
clude cultural and individual variations in relational maintenance communi-
cation; communication patterns and quality in intercultural interactions;
and individual, relational, and environmental factors that contribute to stu-
success in higher education institutions. She enjoys working, talking,
dining, cooking, traveling, watching sitcoms and CNN, repairing, garden-
ing, and fishing.
Marat Zaguidoulline is a graduate student in the Interdisciplinary PhD pro-
gram in social psychology, University of Nevada, Reno. He is a graduate of
the Philology Department at Kazan State University, Russia and received a
Masters degree in English from the University of Bergen, Norway. His re-
search interests include language related social problems in contemporary
society and interpersonal relationships. His hobbies include music (piano,
guitar, samples) and taking photographs.
Elaine D. Zelley is an Assistant Professor of Communication at La Salle Uni-
versity, Philadelphia, PA. She received her PhD from Pennsylvania State
University in 2001. current research explores the relationships be-
tween friendship, competition, conflict, social support and the occurrence
ABOUT THE CONTRIBUTORS +=e- XXI

of eating disorders among women. She teaches in the areas of interpersonal,


group processes, communication theory, and ethics. She also has worked as
a team facilitator and in the fields of public relations and corporate commu-
nication. Elaine has recently been published in Communication Yearbook
and the International Encyclopedia of Marriage and Family Relationships.
She is a fitness enthusiast, loves watching movies, and enjoys spending time
at the beach.
This page intentionally left blank
“%
if
4,
%$\

Definitions a Per5 ectives


P
on Fdational Maintenance
Communication

Kathryn Dindia
University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

o maintain a relationship, partners must communicate with one an-


other. Conversely, as long as people communicate, they have a relation-
ship. The end of the relationship occurs when people stop communicating.
This does not mean they stop communicating for some period of time, for,
as Sigman
S&man (199 1) pointed out, relationships are continuous despite discon-
tinuous periods of physical and interactional copresence. But once two
people stop communicating (and do not anticipate future interaction),
their relationship is over. Thus, to maintain a relationship one must main-
tain communication. Similarly, the quality of a relationship is primarily de-
termined by the quality of the communication in the relationship. Thus, to
maintain the quality of a relationship, one must maintain the quality of the
communication. Communication is central to relationship maintenance.
Communication scholars have much to say about relational mainte-
nance. That is not to say we should study communication and relational
maintenance in a vacuum; concepts such as costs and rewards, attraction,
commitment, and so on, are all relevant to the study of relational mainte-
nance. But communication constitutes the primary way we maintain rela-
2-e=+ DINDIA

tionships. Consequently, this volume is about how we maintain


relationships through communication.
The purpose of this chapter is both to review communication ap-
proaches to relational maintenance and to preview communication per-
spectives on relational maintenance elaborated in this volume. The goal of
this chapter is to, among other things, illustrate the central role that com-
munication plays in the process of relational maintenance and the central
role that communication scholars play in the study of relational mainte-
nance. This chapter begins by reviewing definitions of relational mainte-
nance and is followed with a discussion of perspectives on communication
and relational maintenance. This leads to a discussion of communication
strategies to maintain relationships. In discussing communication strate-
gies, the issue of strategic versus routine relational maintenance will be
elaborated as well as the issue of self versus partner perceptions of rela-
tional maintenance behaviors. Uniphasic versus multiphasic models of re-
lational maintenance strategies are discussed followed by a discussion of
the types of relationships, and the limitations of the types of relationships,
in which relational maintenance communication has been studied. The or-
ganization of the chapter is chronological, beginning with earliest perspec-
tives that dealt explicitly with the role of communication in relational
maintenance moving forward to the most recent perspectives on relational
maintenance communication.

RIELATIONAL MAINTENANCE DEl=lNlED

Relational maintenance processes and strategies compose the heart of rela-


tionship research. Although the processes and strategies of initiating and
terminating relationships are important, people spend more time main-
taining relationships than initiating or terminating them (Duck, 1988).
Thus, although early research on relationships focused on relationship ini-
tiation strategies and the initial stages of relationships, and was followed by
the study of the processes and strategies of terminating relationships, in
the last 30 years the field has come to focus explicitly on the process and
strategies of maintaining relationships.
Thousands of studies pertain to relational stability and satisfaction, and
each of these has something to say about relational maintenance processes
and strategies although the authors may not use the term relational mainte-
nance. (1979,1994) research is an excellent example of research
that has important ramifications for communication and relational mainte-
nance but does not explicitly use the term relutionship maintenance.
However, in the last 30 years, several lines of research have emerged
that explicitly use the term relational maintenance. In an earlier article,
Canary and I (Dindia & Canary, 1993) elaborated four definitions of rela-
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION 3t ”

tional maintenance as found in the literature. The first definition is to keep


a relationship in existence. A relationship that is maintained is a relation-
ship that is not terminated (i.e., a couple is married, not divorced). This is
the most basic definition of relationship maintenance on which all others
are based.
The second definition of relational maintenance is to keep a relationship
in a specified state or condition. This definition of relational maintenance
implies not only that the relationship is maintained but also that the funda-
mental nature of the relationship is maintained, as it currently exists. From
a stage perspective (e.g., Knapp &Vangelisti, 2000), to maintain a relation-
ship is to maintain the stage of the relationship and the characteristics of
the relationship associated with the stage of the relationship. For most peo-
ple, when they talk about maintaining a relationship, they do not mean
maintaining the existence of a relationship, they mean maintaining the
closeness, the trust, the commitment, the liking, and so on; and failure to
sustain these fundamental properties of a relationship constitutes a failure
to maintain the relationship. In close, personal relationships, what is usu-
ally desired to be maintained is not just a relationship but a close, personal
relationship. From a stage perspective, maintaining the stage of a relation-
ship prevents a relationship from de-escalating (or escalating), and conse-
quently from terminating; but it is not the same as continuing a
relationship. People can maintain a relationship at a lower (or higher) level
than they have in the past, thus, the relationship is maintained, but the
level of the relationship is not.
Relational maintenance defined as maintenance of a steady state is a
definition of relational maintenance at odds with dialectical theory. Ac-
cording to a dialectical perspective, to maintain a relationship, the relation-
ship must constantly change and adapt to opposing tensions (Montgomery,
1993). Thus, dialectical perspectives on relational maintenance reject the
term relational maintenance and advocate the term relational sustainment.
However, relational maintenance need not imply that a relationship is
static and unchanging. As stated by Wilmot (198 1) a stable relationship
still has considerable change occurring within it.
The third definition of relational maintenance is to keep a relationship
in a satisfactory condition (i.e., maintaining a satisfactory relationship).
Sometimes relational maintenance is conceptually and operationally de-
fined as maintaining relational satisfaction. Other times, relational main-
tenance is conceptually defined as relational continuity (i.e., whether the
relationship is intact) but the operational definition of relational mainte-
nance is relational satisfaction because predicting relational continuity is
more difficult to do (requires longitudinal data) than predicting rela-
tional satisfaction. However, maintaining relational satisfaction and
maintaining the relationship are not synonymous. One can maintain a dis-
satisfying relationship. Although it makes sense to study how people
4 w DINDIA

maintain satisfaction (and other important characteristics of relation-


ships such as liking and trust), and maintaining satisfaction may lead to
relational continuity, maintaining satisfaction is not the same as relational
continuity. Relationships can be maintained whether or not they are sat-
isfying (e.g., in involuntary or circumstantial relationships, to be dis-
cussed later in this chapter). This is not to say that maintaining
satisfaction and other important qualities of relationships are not theo-
retically important issues in and of themselves as well as predictors of re-
lational continuity. It is only to say that some relationships that are not
satisfying are, none-the-less, maintained (continued).
The fourth use of the term rebztiond maintenance is to keep a relation-
ship in repair. It includes both relationship maintenance and repair in the
definition of relational maintenance. Davis (1973) defined relational
maintenance to include preventative maintenance and corrective mainte-
nance. As stated by Davis, “Integrations that have a tendency to become
loose can be tightened by preventive maintenance before they become
loose, or by corrective maintenance afterward” (p. 2 10). Similarly, Dindia
and Baxter (1987; Baxter & Dindia, 1990) included both maintenance and
repair in their definition of relational maintenance. Dindia and Baxter spe-
cifically tested whether relational maintenance and repair should be distin-
guished from one another. Conceptually, repair implies that something has
gone awry with the relationship that needs correcting. Maintenance does
not. Thus, the degree to which these two processes are similar-different,
and the degree to which similar communication behaviors function to
maintain-repair relationships is at issue. To keep a relationship in repair is
related to the first definition of relationship maintenance (relational conti-
nuity) because to keep a relationship in repair, using both preventative and
corrective maintenance, would, in effect, prevent the relationship from
deescalating further and terminating.
When Canary and I reviewed these definitions in 1993, we did not advo-
cate a particular definition of relational maintenance. We did, however, ar-
gue that because the definition of relationship maintenance varies across
studies of relational maintenance, the definition of relational maintenance
should be explicitly stated in every study of relational maintenance. Al-
though the definitions of relational maintenance overlap to some degree,
important conceptual and operational distinctions exist among the four
definitions. Differentiating the various conceptualizations of relational
maintenance allows for conceptual clarity, which is necessary for theory
development. For example, Interdependence Theory (Thibaut & Kelley,
1959) elaborates two distinct outcomes-satisfaction and depend-
ence-and posits that different equations predict satisfaction and depend-
ence. Thus, satisfaction and dependence are not the same and what causes
satisfaction and dependence are not the same. The same is true for the dif-
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION +=a- 5

ferent definitions of relational maintenance, that is, what predicts relation-


ship continuity, stability, satisfaction, and closeness can and do vary.
In addition, a fundamental difference separates relational maintenance as
relational continuity from other definitions of relational maintenance. A rela-
tionship that is maintained is a relationship that is not terminated. This defini-
tion of relationship maintenance does not imply anything about the
dimensions or qualities of the relationship that are maintained. It does not
specify whether the relationship changes or remains stable during the process
of relational maintenance (an important theoretical issue in dialectical ap-
proaches to maintenance). Relational maintenance means that the relation-
ship is maintained, not that relational satisfaction is maintained, or that a
particular stage of the relationship is maintained, or that any other quality of
the relationship, such as liking, is maintained. Maintaining certain characteris-
tics of relationships (intimacy, liking, etc.) may affect whether a relationship is
maintained or declines and terminates. In addition, the stage in which a rela-
tionship is maintained affects whether a relationship is maintained (if the rela-
tionship is maintained at a particular stage, the relationship is, in effect,
maintained). But whether certain characteristics of a relationship are main-
tained or not is not the same as whether or not the relationship continues.
Thus, researchers should clearly indicate whether they are studying
maintaining the existence of the relationship, maintaining relational satis-
faction, or other important qualities of the relationship, and so on, when
they use the term relational maintenance. Satisfaction, stability, and close-
ness are important relational phenomena that may predict and explain re-
lational continuity and should be studied as such, but they should not be
confounded with the concept of relational continuity. Norton (1983)
made a similar argument regarding conceptual and operational definitions
of marital quality. Norton argued that an overall evaluation of a
relationship was confounded with characteristics of the relationship in tra-
ditional measures of marital quality (Locke & Williamson, 1958; Spanier,
1976). In traditional measures marital quality is measured by a multidi-
mensional scale that includes several dimensions of relationships that are
thought to be important components of marital quality (e.g., communica-
tion, cohesion, consensus). Norton conceptually defined marital quality as
a global evaluation of the relationship and dimensions of quality marriage
(communication, cohesion, consensus) were not included in his opera-
tional definition of marital quality and, therefore, could be studied as pre-
dictors of marital quality. Similarly, the ability to maintain a relationship at
a given level, or the ability to maintain relational satisfaction, may predict
relational continuity and therefore should be kept separate from relational
continuity. Specifically, we need to better understand what must be main-
tained in a relationship (satisfaction, liking, intimacy, etc.) for a relation-
ship to be continued.
0 -es DINDIA

COMMUNICATION APPROACHi TO THE STUDY


Ol= RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE

A number of theoretical perspectives on relationships implicitly or explic-


itly pertain to relational maintenance. Social exchange theories including
Interdependence Theory, Equity Theory, and the Investment Model, have
made explicit theoretical propositions about relational maintenance (c.f.,
Rusbult & Buunk, 1993; Thibaut & Kelley, 1959). Relational Dialectics, an-
other theoretical perspective, has major implications for relational mainte-
nance in terms of the dynamic process of relational maintenance (see
Montgomery, 1993) and the strategies for maintaining relationships (i.e.,
the concept of praxis, Baxter & Montgomery, 1996). (1979,
1994) interactional approach focuses on how communicative patterns pre-
dict relational continuity. This chapter does not review research on social
exchange, dialectical, or approaches to relational maintenance
because these approaches were recently reviewed elsewhere (Canary &
Zelley, 2000; see also Dainton, 2000 for a more recent test of Interdepen-
dence Theory). Instead, scholarly work is reviewed that explicitly uses the
term relational maintenance and has focused on communication behaviors,
strategies, and routines for maintaining relationships.

Maintenance-By-Expression Versus
Maintenance-By-Suppression

Kaplan (1975; 1976) argued that relationship maintenance entails three ba-
sic functions: emotional expression, definition of reality (i.e., definition of
relationship), and preservation of order. Emotional expression is based on
the assumption that human interaction continuously evokes feelings, which
must be released (Kaplan, 1975/l 976). This is particularly true for negative
emotion because it poses a threat to relationship stability. As Kaplan ex-
plained, “no relationship is immune to negative emotion . . . and no relation-
ship escapes the need to deal with these antisocial sentiments” (p. 106).
A second function of maintenance is to define the relationship. Rela-
tionship partners need to understand what happens between them. Both
individuals need to know what they think, feel, and expect of the partner,
and they also need to know what the partner thinks, feels, and expects of
them (Kaplan, 1975/l 965).
The third function of maintenance is to preserve order in the relation-
ship. The essence of a relationship is coordinated activity. Individuals
achieve coordination by restricting the range of possible behaviors and
bringing actions in to alignment with the actions (Kaplan,
1975/1976).
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION ==a

Kaplan (1975/l 976) listed two global and polar-opposite types of main-
tenance behaviors relevant to these three functions of relational mainte-
nance. Maintenance-by-expression occurs when partners verbalize their
feelings, their observations of the relationship, and the regulation of the in-
teraction between them. Maintenance-by-expression has been labeled by
others as metacommunication or relationship talk, openness, and
self-disclosure. It encompasses direct strategies for maintaining relation-
ships. Maintenance-by-suppression occurs when any direct discussion of
mutual feelings, views of the relationship, or efforts to carry on in an orderly
fashion is suppressed. Maintenance-by-suppression included expressing
emotions indirectly through nonverbal and verbal communication (joking
and laughter). Maintenance-by-suppression also includes direct expression
to third parties in the absence of the partner. Indeed, Oliker (1989) showed
that married friendships promote marital stability. They do this, in
part, by diffusing anger or other volatile emotions and managing these emo-
tions so as to sustain married commitment to their marriage. Main-
tenance-by-suppression encompasses indirect strategies.
Kaplan (1975/l 976) argued that expressive maintenance is better able
to sustain relationships of high involvement over time than mainte-
nance-by-suppression. According to Kaplan, “expressive maintenance pro-
vides a way of preserving a strong emotional bond and, in general, promotes
closeness and satisfaction in relationship” (p. 301). Kaplan also argued that
maintenance-by-expression involves some amount of maintenance-by-
suppression (i.e., tact). Similarly, Kaplan indicated that maintenance-by-
expression should be conducted in a constructive manner and he provided
some guidelines for constructive maintenance-by-expression.
No studies have directly tested thesis. However, others have
studied metacommunication (or directness) as a strategy to maintain rela-
tionships. Research indicates that it is not a frequent study to maintain rela-
tionships (Ayres, 1983). Dindia and Baxter (1987) found that
metacommunication was more frequently reported to repair than to main-
tain a relationship. In particular, several studies using Stafford and
(I 99 I) measure of openness (defined as talking about the relationship and
self-disclosure) have found mixed results about the effectiveness of open-
ness in maintaining relationships (see Stafford, this volume).

Maintaining the ~ociaI Forces that HoId


Relationships Together

One common approach to relational maintenance posits a set of external


forces that act either to hold a relationship together (centripetal forces) or
to pull it apart (centrifugal force). Lewin (195 1) labeled these forces driv-
ing forces and restraining forces. Levinger (1965, 1976) called them at-
8 w DINDIA

tractions and barriers. According to this perspective, relationship


maintenance strategies can focus on any of the forces that affect a relation-
ship. For example, Levinger (1979) indicated that a traditional strategy for
maintaining marriage was to keep up the barriers preventing divorce and to
remove all realistic alternatives; whereas, a contemporary approach to rela-
tional maintenance is to revive or raise the mutual feelings of at-
traction (although see Attridge, 1994, for a return to barriers and
maintenance). Similarly, Davis (1973) observed:
like any human construction, intimate relations are subject to deterioration.. ..
As these minor breakdowns accumulate, they will both weaken the centripetal
forces that hold intimates together and strengthen the centrifugal forces that
drive them apart. When the centrifugal forces become stronger than the centrip-
etal, the intimates will break up. (p. 209)

According to Davis, individuals who want to maintain a relationship


must sustain certain social forces that fasten themselves together.
Davis (1973) o b served three global types of preventative and corrective
maintenance designed to sustain the social forces that fasten intimates to-
gether. First, is manipulation of the external environment, both social and
physical, surrounding the relationship. To employ this strategy, intimates
deliberately seek out an environment that will bring them closer together.
Second, intimates can “work-it-out” or “have-it-out.” Intimates can
work it out through explicit communication about the relationship or the
problems in the relationship (i.e., “a serious talk”). Davis (1973) labeled
this “meta-intimate conversation,” but it is similar to the term
metacommunication. Davis also referred to this as a “State of the Union
Address,” which Baxter and Wilmot (1984) labeled a state-of-the-
relationship talk. Davis argued that metacommunication functions to rein-
tegrate the relationship.
Alternatively, intimates can “have it out” by engaging in an argument or
fight. This strategy also includes a number of “intimacy tests” that test the
desire to continue the relationship (e.g., becoming obstinate, in-
solent, hypercritical, sullen) and other destructive behaviors (e.g., ultima-
tums, withdrawal, acting cold to the partner, being unwilling to provide
customary self-disclosure or favors, taking preliminary steps to end the re-
lationship, etc). Ironically, all of these behaviors are intended to reintegrate
the relationship but accomplish the opposite. Thus, negative and antisocial
behaviors are used as strategies to maintain a relationship (albeit probably
not very effectively).
Third, couples can intentionally renew their relationship through peri-
odic ceremonies that Davis (1973) ca11e d reintegration ceremonies. Reinte-
gration ceremonies include formal reintegration ceremonies such as
celebrating anniversaries and less formal reintegration ceremonies such as
when couples recall times during which they were especially close (play
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION +=a- ?

“our song,” etc.). Davis discussed two other kinds of reintegration ceremo-
nies (eating out at an expensive restaurant) and reassurance rituals (verbal
and nonverbal expressions of love, compliments, etc.).
Although not all of relational maintenance strategies are commu-
nication based, many of them are: primarily work-it-out and have-it-out
but also reintegration ceremonies. Davis did not empirically test his obser-
vations. However, his observations led to the work of Dindia and Baxter
(1987) and are later elaborated in this chapter.

Relational Maintenance as Metacommunication

Braiker and Kelley (1979) were interested in understanding the role that
conflict plays in relationship development. Employing a social exchange
approach to relational maintenance, Braiker and Kelley conceptually de-
fined maintenance as communication behaviors engaged in by members of
the couple to reduce costs and maximize rewards in the relationship. Main-
tenance behavior was operationally defined using items primarily measur-
ing communication with partner about the relationship (also
included one item measuring self-disclosure and one item measuring will-
ingness to change behavior). Thus, Braiker and Kelley also focus on
metacommunication as a relational maintenance strategy.
Braiker and Kelley (1979) ask ed married couples to complete question-
naires in which they estimated the degree or extent to which they experi-
enced a particular attitude, feeling, or behavior during each stage of the
history (casual dating, serious dating, engaged, and married).
The results of the study were that the maintenance scale showed a linear de-
velopment over time with gradual increases from casual dating to marriage.
Thus, metacommunication increased linearly from casual dating to marriage.
In addition, Braiker and Kelley (1979) f ound that the maintenance scale
loaded on a general love dimension during the first two stages of the rela-
tionship, but by the fourth stage (marriage) it was more heavily loaded on a
conflict-negativity dimension. The authors concluded that maintenance
strategies change meaning over time, with maintenance behavior serving to
increase interdependence and love in the earlier stages of development and
to resolve conflict in the later ones. Thus, it appears that talking about the
relationship functions to escalate a relationship (increase love and interde-
pendence) in the early stages of relationship development and to maintain
the relationship (resolve conflict) in later stages.

Tyvoiogies
L of Relational Maintenance Strategies

The next set of studies on relational maintenance emphasized rehtional


maintenance strategies, or conscious and intentional behaviors designed to
maintain the relationship. Several typologies of relational maintenance
IO -c=+ DINDIA

strategies have emerged in the literature. Each of these typologies is unique


in their conceptualization of relational maintenance. However, more re-
cent research calls into question the strategic nature of these behaviors, us-
ing the term routine maintenance behaviors to refer to behaviors that are
not consciously and intentionally employed as relational maintenance
strategies but, nonetheless, function to maintain the relationship.

Typology of Relational Maintenance Strategies

Although most of the research on relational maintenance has been con-


cerned with strategies for keeping a relationship from de-escalating and ter-
minating, Ayers (1983) defined relational maintenance as keeping a
relationship in a stable state, thus preventing it from de-escalating or escalat-
ing. Ayres gave male and female college students a relationship scenario and
asked them the likelihood of using 28 strategies (generated by the author
and supplemented by undergraduate students) to maintain the hypothetical
relationship. A factor analysis of the 28 strategies suggested three factors.
The first factor, avoidance strategies, included ignoring things the other per-
son might do to change a relationship and avoiding doing things that might al-
ter the relationship trajectory (up or down). The second factor, balance
strategies, involved keeping the number of favors and emotional support lev-
els constant or balanced (again, preventing the relationship from moving up
or down). The third factor, directness strategies, involved directly telling the
other person that the relationship should remain unchanged.
Overall, the most frequently reported strategy was balance strategies
followed by avoidance strategies then directness strategies regardless of
the relationship condition (trying to maintain when partner wants to esca-
late, de-escalate, or maintain). Thus, regardless of perceived partner in-
tent, individuals primarily maintain their relationship through balance and
avoidance strategies rather than directness strategies. However, perceived
partner intent did affect the use of avoidance, balance, and directness
strategies. Participants who wanted to maintain the level of the relation-
ship but were told that their partner wanted to escalate the relationship re-
ported more avoidance strategies than participants who were told their
partner wanted to maintain or de-escalate the relationship. Participants
who were told that their partner wanted to escalate the relationship were
less likely to report balance strategies than those who were told that their
partner wanted to maintain or de-escalate the relationship. Participants
who were told that their partner wanted to escalate the relationship re-
ported more directness strategies than participants who were told that
their partner wanted to de-escalate the level of the relationship. Shea and
Pearson (1986), in an extension of (1983) research, found three fac-
tors that resembled, but were not identical to, factors. They found
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION +==+- II

that perceived partner intent only affected balance (not avoidance or di-
rectness) strategies. Thus, it is difficult to draw conclusions from this re-
search. However, this research is important because it illustrates the
different types of relational predicaments in which relational maintenance
strategies occur (maintain when partner wants to escalate, maintain when
partner wants to maintain, maintain when partner wants to de-escalate)
and tries to illuminate similarities and differences in strategies to maintain
a relationship across these various relationship conditions.

Bell et al. (Bell, Daly, & Gonzalez, 1987) examined affinity mainte-
nance strategies. The researchers developed a typology of affinity main-
tenance strategies by asking a sample of wives to describe, in writing,
the things they and their husband said and did in their marriage that
they thought maintained liking and solidarity. The re-
sponses were content analyzed and used to develop a typology of 28
strategies. The strategies were:

altruism, concede control, conversational rule-keeping, dynamism, elicit


disclosures, equality, facility enjoyment, faithfulness, honesty, inclusion of other,
influence perceptions of closeness, listening, openness, optimism, physical affec-
tion, physical attractiveness, present interesting self, reliability, reward associa-
tion, self-concept confirmation, self-improvement, self-inclusion, sensitivity,
shared spirituality, supportiveness, third-party relations, and verbal affection.

Conceptual definitions and examples for each strategy are found in Bell
et al. (1987).
Bell et al. (1987) examined reported frequency of affinity maintenance
strategies. Wives reported the most frequently used strategies by both them-
selves and their husbands were faithfulness, honesty, physical attractiveness,
self-concept confirmation, supportiveness, and verbal affection. The least fre-
quently used strategies were altruism, conceding control, conversational
rule-keeping, dynamism, equality, shared spirituality, and similarity.
Bell et al. (1987) ask ed wives what affinity maintenance behaviors they
wanted from their husbands as well as what they thought their husbands
wanted from them. The affinity behaviors wives most desired from hus-
bands included being faithful, honest, physically attractive, sensitive, and
confirming the self-concept. Far less important to wives were strate-
gies of conceding control, dynamism, equality and self-improvement.
Wives believed that their husband most wanted them to be faithful, hon-
est, physically affectionate, sensitive, and physically attractive.
Bell et al. (1987) correlated marital satisfaction with perceptions
of their own and their frequency of strategy use. There were nu-
12 +=+ DINDIA

merous correlations between frequency judgments and marital satisfac-


tion. marital satisfaction was related to the frequency with which
wives reported engaging in 18 of the strategies and the frequency with
which wives felt their husbands engaged in 24 of the strategies.
Bell et al. (1987) investigated predictors of martial satisfaction.
Because the frequency of use of five strategies had accounted
for more than half the variance in marital satisfaction, marital
satisfaction was regressed on these five strategies. The stepwise regression
revealed the following predictors of marital satisfaction in order: (a) sensi-
tivity, (b) shared spirituality, (c) physical affection, (d) self-inclusion, and
(e) honesty. Thus, there is some evidence of the effectiveness of these
strategies for maintaining relationships. One reason that Bell et al.‘s re-
search on affinity maintenance strategies is important is because it reminds
us that liking is an important dimension of relationships and that to main-
tain a voluntary relationship requires maintaining affinity.

Dindia and Typobgg 06 KeIationaI Maintenance


and Repair strategies

In a third line of research Dindia and Baxter studied relational mainte-


nance and repair strategies (Baxter & Dindia, 1990; Dindia & Baxter,
1987). Following (1973) conceptual definition of relational mainte-
nance, which included both preventative and corrective maintenance (re-
pair), a typology of relationship maintenances strategies was generated
using a combination of deductive and inductive methods. strategies
to maintain relationships were supplemented with strategies derived from
interviews with married couples regarding their relational maintenance
strategies. The resulting typology consists of 49 categories that are clus-
tered into 11 superordinate types:

1. changing the external environment (drawn from Davis),


2. communication (from pilot study, not addressed by Davis),
3. metacommunication (derived from Davis),
4. avoid metacommunication (from Davis, keep quiet, let it pass),
5. antisocial strategies (derived from Davis, includes coercive attempts
to change the partner in some way, including fights, ultimatums,
threats, negative behaviors such as being insolent or rude, breaking
contact, being sullen, acting cold, etc.),
6. prosocial strategies (not from Davis but opposite of antisocial strate-
gies, things we do in relationship to maintain equilibrium, being nice,
cheerful, refraining from criticism, giving in, etc.),
7. ceremonies (from Davis, includes celebrations, reassurance rituals
including expressions of affection, compliments, etc.),
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION +=+I- If,

8. spontaneity (not from Davis, includes efforts to be spontaneous),


9. togetherness (spending time together, doing things together, etc.),
10. seeking or allowing autonomy (not from Davis), and
11. seeking outside help (not from Davis, see counselor, pray, etc.).

This typology of relational maintenance and repair strategies was used


to categorize the relational maintenance and repair strategies reported by
50 married couples (N = 100 spouses; Dindia & Baxter, 1987). Strategies
were categorized into the 12 superordinate categories. Respondents most
frequently reported the use of prosocial (primarily being nice, refraining
from criticism, giving in, being warm, providing favors), ceremonial (pri-
marily expressions of affection followed by going out to dinner), commu-
nication (primarily references to quantity of communication not quality of
communication), and togetherness (primarily spending time together and
doing things together) strategies. Changing the external environment,
avoid metacommunication, antisocial strategies, and seeking or allowing
autonomy were seldom reported as relational maintenance or repair strate-
gies. Moreover, the results of the study indicated that not all strategies
used to maintain a relationship were also used to repair a relationship.
Metacommunication occurred more frequently when the goal was repair-
ing the relationship than when the goal was maintaining the relationship.
Introducing spontaneity into the relationship occurred more frequently
when the goal was maintaining the relationship than when the goal was re-
pairing the relationship. In addition, more strategies were reported for
maintenance than repair indicating that repertoires of strate-
gies for maintaining their relationship is larger than their repertoire of
strategies for repairing their relationship.
Dindia and Baxter (1987) tested whether reported relational mainte-
nance strategies were correlated with relational satisfaction, length of mar-
riage, and gender. The only significant result was that the number of
relational maintenance strategies was negatively correlated with the length
of marriage. We concluded that either couples do less to maintain their re-
lationship the longer they have been married; alternatively, through a pro-
cess of trial and error, couples come to rely on a small number of strategies
to maintain their relationship. Relational satisfaction was not related to re-
lational maintenance strategies. This could be due to fact that we only
asked partners to indicate what they did to maintain their relationship (not
what their partner did to maintain the relationship) and research later re-
viewed in this chapter indicates that our perceptions of our rela-
tionship maintenance strategies predicts relational satisfaction (not our
perceptions of our own maintenance strategies).
Because this research included relational maintenance and repair in the
definition of relational maintenance, it illuminates similarities and differ-
ences between strategies designed to maintain (at an advanced level as op-
1-v -I+- DINDIA

posed to deescalate) a relationship versus strategies designed to repair a


relationship (return to a previously advanced level after decline).

Canary and staffordJs Typobgy


o F Relational Maintenance Strategies

Finally, Stafford and Canary (199 1) d erived five relational maintenance


strategies through factor analysis of items derived from previous research
and by asking a sample of married and dating couples what they did to
maintain their relationship. The factors were:

1. positivity (being positive and cheerful),


2. openness (self-disclosure and open discussion about the relationship),
3. assurances (stressing commitment, showing love, and demonstrat-
ing faithfulness),
4. network (spending time with common friends and affiliations), and
5. sharing tasks (sharing with household tasks).

This line of research is not reviewed here because it is reviewed by


Stafford (chap. 3) in this volume. However, this line of research is impor-
tant for several reasons. First, it has become the most frequently used op-
erational definition of relational maintenance strategies in the literature, in
part because it has spawned research by others beyond the original devel-
opers of the instrument. Second, because of all the research using this in-
strument, an impressive body of evidence is developing on the following
issues: the frequency of various relational maintenance strategies, the rela-
tionship between relational maintenance strategies and relational satisfac-
tion, the effect of relationship type on relational maintenance strategies,
the effect of gender on relationship maintenance strategies, the link be-
tween relational maintenance strategies and various relationship charac-
teristics, in particular, control mutuality, trust, and liking but also
commitment and love, the results of which are reviewed in Stafford (chap.
3, this volume).

l=UNDAMENTAL lSSUES REGARDING RWATIONAL


MAINTENANCE STRATiEGIES

To more thoroughly review research on communication and relational


maintenance strategies one can elaborate on three issues that are central to
research on relational maintenance strategies: the various perceptions of
relational maintenance strategies, the degree to which these behaviors are
strategic, and the degree to which they are unique to this relationship goal.
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION ==s- 12

Perceptions 06 Relational Maintenance Strategies

Some of the research on relationship maintenance strategies has examined


an perception of his or her maintenance strategies (e.g. Canary
& Stafford, 1993; Canary, Stafford, Hause, & Wallace, 1993; Dainton &
Stafford, 1993). For example, Canary et al. (1993) asked students who
were involved in a wide variety of relationships what communication strat-
egies they used to maintain their relationships. Other research has exam-
ined an perception of his or her maintenance
strategies (Ayres, 1983; Dainton, Stafford, & Canary, 1994; Guerrero,
Eloy, & Wabnik, 1993; Shea & Pearson, 1986; Stafford & Canary, 1991).
Some researchers have examined both an perception of his or
her own maintenance strategies and the same perception of his
or her maintenance strategies (Bell et al., 1987; Canary &
Stafford, 1992). Research has also examined both perceptions of
their own relationship maintenance strategies (Dindia SKBaxter, 1987).
Determining whose perceptions of relational maintenance strategies are
being examined is important for a number of reasons. For example,
Stafford and Canary (1991) examined perceptions of their
relational maintenance strategies. They found that women per-
ceived their male partners as using more positiveness, assurances, and so-
cial networks than men perceived their female partners as using. Based on
results of this study, it is unknown if men use more relationship mainte-
nance strategies than women or if women are just more perceptive of rela-
tional maintenance strategies (Canary & Stafford, 1992, studied
perceptions of their own maintenance behaviors and found the opposite;
women reported using more openness, networks, and tasks than men re-
ported using). Only by systematically taking into account whose perspec-
tive is being measured, can we determine the answer to this question.
Whose perceptions are being examined is also important in terms of de-
termining whose perceptions of self or partner relational maintenance
strategies predict relational satisfaction (and ultimately relational continu-
ity) . If an individual does not perceive his or her relational main-
tenance strategies, his or her relational maintenance strategies
may not affect the relational satisfaction. It is possible for an
individual to perceive that he or she is using a relational maintenance strat-
egy and for his or her partner not to perceive that he or she is using this rela-
tional maintenance strategy. We need to know whose perceptions of whose
(self or partner) maintenance strategies are related to relational satisfac-
tion (and other relational characteristics).
Spiegelhoff and Dindia (2001) studied perceptions of self and
partner relational maintenance strategies and relational satisfaction. Both
partners in a relationship reported their perceptions of their own and their
lo +i=+ DINDIA

relationship maintenance strategies. This allowed us to examine


agreement in perceptions of their relationship maintenance strat-
egies (e.g., “If I perceive I use assurances does my partner perceive I use as-
surances?“) and perceived similarity in use of relational
maintenance strategies (e.g., “If I perceive I use assurances does my part-
ner perceive s/he uses assurances?“). It also allowed us to examine whose
perspective is more predictive of relational satisfaction, “my perceptions
of my relational maintenance strategies,” “my perceptions of my
own relational maintenance strategies, ” “my perceptions of his or
her relational maintenance strategies,” or “my perceptions of my
relational maintenance strategies.”
Partners in 75 opposite-sex dyads independently completed the Canary
and Stafford Relational Maintenance Strategies Scales (1991) and a mea-
sure of relational satisfaction. The results indicate high reliability in part-
perceptions of relational maintenance strategies and high similarity in
use of relational maintenance strategies. The results also indicate
that, in general, an perceptions of his or her relational
maintenance strategies are most predictive of the relational
satisfaction. Specifically, when controlling for self and partner perceptions
of self and partner relational maintenance strategies, perceptions of
their female assurances and positiveness were related to
relational satisfaction, and perceptions of their male as-
surances were related to relational satisfaction. However,
perceptions of their male assurances were related to
relational satisfaction. Apparently, men who are satisfied with their
relationship engage in assurances, and these assurances, are perceived by
their female partners, and such assurances are highly predictive of both
their own and their female relational satisfaction.

StrategicVersus Routine Relational Maintenance


Dainton and Stafford (1993) d i ff erentiated relational maintenance strate-
gies and routine maintenance behaviors by defining strategic behavior as
conscious and intentional behavior enacted by partners to maintain the re-
lationship, whereas routine maintenance behavior was held to occur at a
lower level of consciousness and is not intentionally used to maintain the
relationship. Duck (1988) argued for the need to look at routine behaviors,
stating, “there are many other instances where the little things of life keep
us together” (p. 99). For example, people probably do not think of asking
their partner how his or her day went or telling their partner how their day
went as strategies to maintain their relationship. However, these acts may
nonetheless function to maintain the relationship.
Dainton and Stafford (1993) extended previous typologies of relational
maintenance strategies by probing for routine maintenance behaviors used
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION -+I-

by married and dating couples. The resulting 12-category taxonomy in-


cluded both strategic and routine maintenance behaviors. The researchers
found that the most frequently reported maintenance behavior was shar-
ing tasks, a category that was infrequently mentioned in prior research.
This discrepancy lead the researchers to note that sharing tasks may be a
routine behavior that functions to maintain relationships that couples per-
form without the explicit purpose of maintaining the relationship.
Dainton (1995) compared use of strategic and routine main-
tenance behaviors. Dainton developed a typology of interaction types
based on a daily interaction log completed by subjects. The typology in-
cluded seven categories of interaction: instrumental, leisure, mealtime,
affection, conversations, and network. Dainton had participants rate
how much thought each interaction required and how typical it was for
the relationship. Dainton then combined these scores to come up with
an index of routineness. Dainton discovered that the majority of inter-
actions couples engage in are routine, rather than strategic. Participants
rated the category of affection as the most routine and the most impor-
tant to the relationship.
Similar to routine maintenance behaviors are the rituals that function to
maintain relationships. Bruess and Pearson (1995) focused on the types
and uses of interpersonal rituals in marriages. They developed a typology of
seven ritual types: couple time rituals, idiosyncratic and symbolic rituals,
daily routines and tasks, intimacy expressions, communication rituals,
spiritual rituals, and patterns, habits, and mannerisms. Bruess and Pearson
studied the functions of these rituals. They found that marital rituals, such
as communication rituals and performing everyday tasks together, served
to bond and maintain the relationship.
In reality, the distinction between strategic and routine relational main-
tenance behaviors may not be dichotomous. Couples may routinely (with-
out thinking about maintaining the relationship or intending to maintain
the relationship) kiss each other and say “I love you,” and this behavior may
function to maintain the relationship. Alternatively, an individual may kiss
his or her partner and say, “I love you” as a conscious and intentional strat-
egy to maintain the relationship. People may initially use some relational
maintenance behaviors as strategic, but such behaviors become routine
over time. Some behaviors may be strategic for some partners or couples
and routine for others, or they might be perceived as strategic by some
partners or couples but not by others. Finally, strategic or routine may not
be characteristic of maintenance behaviors but of specific instances of
maintenance behaviors on particular occasions. For example, some rela-
tional maintenance behaviors may be produced routinely but they may also
be used in a strategic manner on occasion. Regardless, it is clear that cou-
ples engage in both strategic and routine behaviors that function to main-
tain the relationship.
uniphasic Versus Mukiphasic Relational
Dedo~ment Strategies

Dindia (1994) argued that research on relationship development strategies


was phase bound, that is, relationship development strategies were studied
within the boundaries of a particular stage of relationship development. How-
ever, there are typologies of relationship initiation strategies (e.g., Baxter &
Philpott, 1982), relationship escalation strategies (e.g., Tolhuizen, 1989,
1992), relationship maintenance strategies (e.g., Ayres, 1983; Dindia &
Baxter, 1987; Stafford & Canary, 1991), and relationship termination strate-
gies (e.g., Baxter, 1982, 1984). Al so, relationship development strategies are
multiphasic, that is, strategies used to initiate and escalate a relationship are
also used to maintain relationships and these are the opposite of strategies to
de-escalate and terminate a relationship. For example, in reviewing the re-
search one can find that communication and contact is used to initiate, esca-
late, and maintain relationships. The opposite, avoidance, is used to deescalate
and terminate relationships. Rewards, self-presentation of positive attributes,
and affection are used to initiate, escalate, and maintain relationships. Their
opposites, costs, self-presentation of negative attributes, and indifference are
used to terminate relationships. Similarity is used to initiate and maintain rela-
tionships; whereas, dissimilarity is reported to terminate relationships, and so
on. Taking a closer look, it becomes apparent to me that a typology of relation-
ship development (broadly conceived as relationship initiation, escalation,
maintenance, de-escalation, repair, and termination) strategies is applicable to
all stages of relationship development, including relationship maintenance.
Also, a typology of relationship development strategies that is multiphasic has
more generality and parsimony than separate typologies of relationship initia-
tion, escalation, maintenance, and termination strategies and that a
multiphasic typology is important for understanding the similarities and dif-
ferences between relationship maintenance and other stages of relationship
development as well as the similarities and differences between relationship
maintenance strategies and relationship initiation, relationship termination,
and the like, strategies.
There is some empirical evidence to support this view. As stated earlier,
Bell et al. (1987) d eveloped a typology of 28 affinity maintenance strate-
gies by asking wives what they said and did to maintain liking and solidarity
in their marriage. Bell et al. found that 24 of the 28 strategies used to main-
tain liking were used to generate liking in the first place (Bell & Daly,
1984). The only exceptions were assume control, personal autonomy,
comfortable self, and nonverbal immediacy (all strategies to generate but
not maintain liking). As stated by Bell et al. (1987), the exertion of control
and demonstration of independence would seem to be counterproductive
to the goal of relational maintenance; the strategies of comfortable self and
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION t, l”7

nonverbal immediacy may be more suited to interactions between ac-


quaintances than interactions between married couples. In addition, the
typology of affinity maintenance strategy contained six strategies not rep-
resented in the reports of affinity-seeking strategies. Theses strategies
were faithfulness, honesty, physical affection, verbal affection,
self-improvement, and third-party relationships, which derive their mean-
ing and significance from the interdependence and emotional involvement
characteristic of more developed relationships (Bell et al., 1987). Thus,
Bell et al.‘s study provides some empirical support that, to a large degree,
similar strategies are used to generate and maintain liking.
Similarly, Tolhuizen (1989) studied strategies to generate liking across
relationship stages. Participants read a description of one of four levels of
relationship development: new acquaintances, fully developed stable
friendship, deteriorating friendship in which the desire is to save and con-
tinue the relationship, and deteriorating friendship in which the desire is to
terminate the relationship. Participants indicated the likelihood that they
would use Bell and (1984) 25 affinity-seeking strategies.
The results indicated that more than one half (14) of the strategies were
similar across all stages of relationship development (altruism, assume con-
trol, assume equality, concede control, conversational rule-keeping, dyna-
mism, elicit disclosures, inclusion of other, nonverbal immediacy,
optimism, personal autonomy, physical attractiveness, self-confirmation,
and trustworthiness). However, 11 of the strategies showed significant dif-
ferences across relationship stages. Without going into too much detail,
participants reported greater use of a number of affinity-seeking strategies
to maintain a relationship than to initiate, repair, or terminate a relation-
ship. These results suggest either greater effort to maintain a developed re-
lationship than to initiate or terminate a relationship, or a greater
repertoire of strategies to maintain a relationship than to initiate, repair, or
terminate a relationship.
When the goal was initiating the relationship, participants reported be-
ing more likely to use similarity than when the goal was to repair or termi-
nate the relationship and more likely to use present interesting self than
when the goal was to terminate the relationship (Tolhuizen, 1989). Thus,
these strategies may be unique to early stages of relationship development.
Among other findings, openness, was more likely to repair the relationship
than to initiate or terminate a relationship. This is similar to Dindia and
Baxter (1987) w h o f ound that metacommunication was used more to re-
pair than to maintain a relationship. When the goal was termination, par-
ticipants were more likely to use supportiveness than when the goal was
initiation. Tolhuizen speculated that this result might confirm Knapp and
(2000) contention that supportiveness may be evident in the
farewell rhetoric of decaying relationships. Indeed, research by Baxter
(1984) indicates that other-oriented strategies are specifically designed to
20 +e DINDIA

avoid hurting the other party in the break-up. Overall, these studies pro-
vide evidence to support the contention that some strategies are used to
initiate, maintain, and terminate relationships. Similarities and differences
across relationship strategies (strategies to initiate, intensify, maintain, re-
pair, de-escalate, terminate) should be studied more in the future.

MAINTAINING NONVOLUNTARY, NONINTIMATE


RlZLATIONSHIPS

Communication scholars, like others who study relationships, have fo-


cused their study of relational maintenance strategies on voluntary, inti-
mate (i.e., close) relationships (marriage, romantic couples, friends) to the
exclusion (until lately) of involuntary or circumstantial relationships or
nonintimate relationships (e.g., acquaintances, friendly relations, supe-
rior-subordinate relationships, co-worker relationships). This emphasis on
voluntary, intimate relationships, to the exclusion of involuntary or
nonintimate relationships, is not unique to the relational maintenance lit-
erature but is true in general for the study of personal relationships. As
Milardo and Wellman (1992) wrote:

The field [of p ersonal relationships] has become myopic, with most papers fo-
cusing on emotionally supportive close relationships: friends, spouses and lov-
ers.. . . There are more relationships worth studying in heaven and earth than
love, marriage and friendship. People work together, are neighborly.. . . In a world
where people have many hundreds of ties, we often need to extend analysis to
more than a few close relationships. (pp. 339-340)

Although only voluntary and intimate (close) relationships for the most
part have received scholarly attention, people maintain a variety of rela-
tionships, some of which are subjectively significant including relation-
ships with spouses, friends, romantic partners, and family members, and
some of which are less significant, such as relationships with co-workers,
acquaintances, neighbors, etc. (Burleson & Samter, 1994). Although these
relationships are viewed as less significant by participants in the relation-
ships, as well as those who study relationships, these relationships are im-
portant and serve a variety of functions in everyday lives. Milardo
and Wellman (1992) explained:

[Weak ties] are quantitatively important because there are so many of them.
They are the basis for many of the allies or enemies people have when things get
complicated. They form potential outlets for changing lives when people change
jobs, spouses, neighborhoods or political systems. They lend familiarity and a
sense of community to daily routines. (p. 340)

A few recent studies of relational maintenance examined nonintimate


relationships. Ray and Poulson (1994) studied holiday greetings and found
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION ++ 21

that holiday greetings function to maintain nonintimate relationships. Ray


and Poulson concluded that Christmas letters are “significant means by
which persons structure and maintain low intimate, long-term relation-
ships” (p. 27). They stated: “This form of maintenance allows the relation-
ship to remain on a non-intimate, but stable basis. Inadequate as this
maintenance may seem to others, especially in regard to intimate relation-
ships, it still functions effectively to keep the relationship going” (p. 30).
Dindia et al. (1995) a1so studied holiday greetings and found that peo-
ple did not perceive holiday greetings as functioning to maintain relation-
ships (i.e., average score was very near mean of scale “neither agree nor
disagree” that holiday greetings function to maintain relationships). In ad-
dition, holiday greetings were perceived to maintain relationships even less
for nonintimate than intimate relationships. Type of relationship did not
affect the degree to which holiday greetings were perceived to maintain re-
lationships except that holiday greetings sent to co-workers, superiors, and
subordinates were perceived to maintain the relationship less than greet-
ings sent to friends, relatives, and romantic partners. Although holiday
greetings were not perceived as functioning to maintain relationships,
when asked why people send holiday greetings, the most frequent reason
given for sending holiday greetings was to maintain the relationship (ap-
proximately one-fourth of responses). Thus, the authors hypothesized
that holiday greeting cards might be a hygienic factor; the absence of holi-
day greetings, rather than the presence, may negatively impact relational
maintenance.

Maintaining work ~elationshps


Recently there has been a major effort to study relational maintenance
communication in superior-subordinate relationships. Waldron (199 1) ar-
gued that maintaining superior-subordinate relationships is the most im-
portant communication objective pursued by subordinates. Waldron also
argued that relational maintenance in supervisor-subordinate relation-
ships deserves special attention because of the unique nature of this rela-
tionship. First, maintenance communication in supervisor-subordinate
relationships is highly influenced by formal role definitions and subject to
constraints not encountered in unstructured interpersonal relationships.
Second, the unique features of superior-subordinate relationships limit
the use of some maintenance tactics (e.g., avoidance). Third, upwardly di-
rected maintenance communication is important because relationship de-
terioration has potentially substantial career and possibly economic
implications for the subordinate.
relationship maintenance strategies with subordinates are also
important to the career and the organization as a whole. According
22 -is- DINDIA

to the results of research conducted by the Gallup Organization (Buckingham


& Coffman, 1999) involving surveys of more than 1 million employees, how
long an employee stays with an organization and how productive that em-
ployee is, is determined by his or her relationship with the immediate supervi-
sor. As stated by Buckingham and Coffman (1999), employee retention is
“most directly influenced by the immediate manager. What does
this tell us? It tells us that people leave managers, not companies” (p. 33).
Waldron (1991) and L ee and Jablin (1995) studied strategies to main-
tain relationships in superior-subordinate relationships. However, rela-
tional maintenance in work settings involves more than maintaining the
superior-subordinate relationship. Work relationships take on a number of
forms (Waldron, chap. 8, this volume). A given employee may be faced
with maintaining relationships with a supervisor, several subordinates,
members of a work team, formal and informal mentors, and any number of
co-workers, customers and clients, suppliers, and so on. Although research
on the maintenance of work relationships needs to continue and expand
beyond the supervisor-subordinate relationship, some conclusions can be
drawn about strategies used to maintain work relationships versus strate-
gies used to maintain close, personal relationships based on research con-
ducted to date (see Waldron, chap. 8, this volume).

Maintaining Nonvokmtar~ Relationships with Disliked Others


Hess (2000) studied relational maintenance in nonvoluntary relationships
with disliked partners. rationale for studying relational maintenance
in this context was that, “people do maintain relationships, often healthy
and close relationships, with partners they dislike, and they do so on a daily
basis” (p. 459). Hess defined nonvoluntary relationships as relationships in
which the actor believes he or she has no viable choice but to maintain it.
Nonvoluntary relations include a variety of types of relationships according
to Hess, including family relationships (e.g., siblings, stepparents, or
in-laws), work relationships (e.g., bosses, students, clients, or tenured col-
leagues), and social relationships (e.g., roommates, church members,
camp counselors, or members of a sports team).
Hess had participants respond to questionnaires about their mainte-
nance of voluntary and nonvoluntary relationships with liked and disliked
partners. Hess was interested in distancing behaviors and asked
open-ended and closed-ended questions to learn what type of distancing
behaviors people use to maintain relationships. The closed-ended ques-
tions were specifically about distancing behaviors. The open-ended ques-
tions (e.g., “what did you do to maintain this relationship, what did you do
differently when interacting with this person than you did when interact-
ing with people you like . . . “) were coded specifically for distancing behav-
iors. The results showed that people use distancing behaviors (expressing
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION ++- 21,

detachment, avoiding involvement and showing antagonism) to maintain-


ing involuntary relationships with disliked partners. It makes sense that
people use distancing behaviors to maintain nonvoluntary relationships
with disliked others. If you cannot end the relationship, you can at least
distance yourself from the relationship and your partner as a strategy to
maintain the relationship.
This line of research should be continued and expanded; of particular
interest, whether traditional relational maintenance strategies (e.g., posi-
tiveness, assurances, openness, sharing tasks) are strategies to maintain
nonvoluntary relationships with disliked partners. If we are stuck with
these relationships due to marriage, our job, or whatever, it seems that in
addition to distancing behaviors, we might employ positive relational
maintenance strategies to make the relationship we cannot terminate more
satisfying, or at least tolerable. People generally try to get along with their
in-laws, colleagues, and others as a strategy to maintain these relationships.
Similarly, it seems reasonable that some of (1991) and Lee and
(1995) strategies may be used to get along with disliked superiors
(as well as disliked others in other non-voluntary relationships).

RELATIONAL CONTINUITY CONSTRUCTION UNITS

This chapter is concluded with a brief elaboration of (199 1) theo-


retical perspective on relational continuity and the behaviors that function
to construct relational continuity because this perspective has major impli-
cations for relationships in the 2 1st Century, including long-distance rela-
tionships and on-line relationships.
Sigman (1991) noted a fundamental anomaly about social and personal
relationships; social and personal relationships are continuous despite dis-
continuous periods of physical and interactional copresence. Sigman argued
that couples manage the discontinuous aspects of relationships by using rela-
tional continuity constructional units (RCCUs). RCCUs refer to behaviors
relational partners engage in before, during, and after an absence that func-
tion to construct the continuity of the relationship despite the absence.
Sigman divided RCCUs into three types: prospective units, introspec-
tive units, and retrospective units. Prospective units are behaviors that rela-
tionship partners perform before physical separation. Behaviors in this
category include farewells, agenda establishments (projections of future
interactions, such as see you at the office tomorrow morning”), the use
of tokens (e.g., wedding and engagement rings), and spoors (objects left
behind, such as a toothbrush in bathroom).
Introspective units occur during relational noncopresence and constitute
the continuity during periods of absence. The wearing of
wedding bands is an example of an introspective unit. Affiliative artifacts,
2-f +s=+ DINDIA

such as team jackets, wedding bands, and photographs of the relationship


partner all signal the existence of the relationship although the partner is
not present. A second type of introspective unit is mediated contact be-
tween relationship partners. Greeting cards, notes, phone calls, and e-mail
messages allow partners to remain connected even when face-to-face in-
teraction possible.
Retrospective units occur after the period of relational noncopresence.
Greetings and conversations that allow the partners to “catch-up” on what
happened during the period of absence are examples of retrospective units.
Gilbertson, Dindia, and Allen (1998) conducted a test of the-
ory. They studied the relationship between time spent apart, RCCUs, and
relational satisfaction for married and cohabitating couples. The results of
the study, similar to the results of other studies, indicated that time apart
was negatively related to relational satisfaction. This evidence indirectly
supports the claim that periods of absence threaten the continuity of rela-
tionships. More important to theory, a number of significant corre-
lations were found between prospective, introspective, and retrospective
RCCUs and relational satisfaction. In particular, when the effects of rela-
tional copresence were held constant, and reports of their
prospective RCCUs predicted female relational satisfaction and
reports of their prospective RCCUs predicted male relational satisfaction.
The concepts of relational continuity and relational continuity con-
struction units are particularly informative to theory and research on
long-distance relationships including commuter marriages and to all rela-
tionships in which couples spend considerable periods of time away from
each other (see Aylor, chap. 6, this volume). In world, couples
spend a considerable amount of time apart due to increased job demands.
For example, in the U.S. the average number of hours worked per week has
risen (from 43.6 in 1977 to 47.1 in 1997), more women work (28% in
1940, 40% in 1966, 51% in 1979, and 60% in 1998), more women work
full time year round (28% in 1969, 50% in 1997), and there are more
dual-earner couples (39% in 1970, 64% in 1999; Bond, Galinsky, &
Swanberg, 1997). Thus, couples are apart from each other for many hours
during the workday. In addition, more people engage in increased business
travel (nights away from home on business), which takes workers away
from their loved ones. Moreover, there are more job relocations, which of-
ten have the effect of inducing long-distance relationships and commuter
marriages (see Aylor, chap. 6, this volume). Similarly, the rise of
telecommuting, global organizations, and virtual teams has had a profound
effect on accessibility in the workplace (see Waldron, chap. 8, this vol-
ume). Managers often manage from a distance, teams often work at a dis-
tance, and individuals are often physically isolated from superiors,
subordinates, and co-workers. Thus, this theory is particularly relevant for
maintaining relational continuity in the 2 1st centurv.
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION ++I- 25

However, this theory assumes that relationships are initiated


face-to-face and that physical copresence is the sine qua non of relational
maintenance. This may no longer be the case. Today, relationships are initi-
ated and maintained on-line (see Rabby & Walther, chap. 7, this volume).
Some relationships never involve physical copresence, some do not get to
the point of meeting each other face-to-face until they are months, even
years, into the relationship. In some relationships, computer-mediated
communication, such as e-mail, is not the means for maintaining a relation-
ship during periods of physical and interactional noncopresence, it is the
means for maintaining the relationship all the time. Thus, although
theory assumes that physical and interactional copresence main-
tains relationships, there may be no primacy or even necessity to physical
and interactional copresence in maintaining relationships (Rabby &
Walther, chap. 7, this volume).
A more radical way to look at this is that mediated communication is no
longer an introspective relational continuity construction unit used to
maintain relationship during periods of absence; instead, for some relation-
ships, mediated communication constitutes the copresence of the rela-
tionship, virtual copresence, and it is the only copresence experienced.
Thus, relational continuity construction units become something that
must be done to maintain the relationship during periods of virtual
noncopresence.
Rabby and Walther (chap. 7, this volume) take a less radical view of rela-
tional maintenance in which computer-mediated communication (CMC)
is viewed as a relationship maintenance strategy. They hold that, ” [CMC]
functions alongside phone calls, letters, and FtF interactions to keep the
relationship going” (p. 153). Rabby and Walther view CMC as an intro-
spective RCCU, “friends, family members, and romantic partners use
e-mail as a means of staying in touch with each other between face-to-face
meetings and phone calls.” (p. 153)

DIRKTIONS I=OR l=UTURE RlE5lEARC.H


Typically, chapters such as this end with a section called “directions for fu-
ture research.” There is no need for that here because the rest of the chap-
ters in this volume constitute the directions for future research on
communication and relational maintenance. Future research on communi-
cation and relational maintenance begins with the rest of the chapters in
this volume and charts a path of greater expansion and diversity on the role
of communication in relational maintenance across relational types, con-
texts, and culture. Clearly, a solid foundation has been built on which the-
ory and research on relational maintenance communication will continue
to gain momentum as it branches out beyond the confines of traditionally
defined “close” relationships.
2c +== DINDIA

REl=tERENCES

Attridge, M. (1994). B arriers to dissolution of romantic relationships. In D. J. Canary &


L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational maintenance (pp. 141-164). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Ayres, J. (1983). Strategies to maintain relationships: Their identification and per-
ceived usage. Communication Quarterly, 3 1, 62-67.
Baxter, L. A. (1982). Strategies for ending relationships: Two studies. Western Journal
of Speech Communication, 46, 223-24 1.
Baxter, L. A. (1984). Trajectories of relationship disengagement. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 1, 29-48.
Baxter, L. A., & Dindia, K. (1990). Marital perceptions of marital mainte-
nance and repair strategies. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7,
187-208.
Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (1996). Relating: Dialogues and dialectics. New
York: Guilford.
Baxter, L. A., & Philpott, J. (1982). Attribution-based strategies for initiating and ter-
minating relationships. Communication Quarterly, 30, 2 17-224.
Baxter, L. A., & Wilmot, W. W (1984). “Secret tests”: Social strategies for acquiring in-
formation about the state of the relationship. Human communication Research, I I,
171-202.
Bell, R. A., & Daly, J. A. (1984). The affinity-seeking function in communication. Cont-
municution Monographs, 51, 91-115.
Bell, R. A., Daly, J. A., &G onzalez, C. (1987). Affinity-maintenance in marriage and its
relationship to marital satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
49, 4455454.
Bond, J. T., Galinsky, E., & Swanberg, J. E. (1997). The 1997 National Study of the
Changing Workforce. New York: Families and Work Institute.
Braiker, H. B., & Kelley, H. H. (1979). Conflict in the development of close relation-
ships. In R. L. Burgess & T. L. Huston (Eds.), Social exchange in developing relution-
ships (pp. 135-l 68). N ew York: Academic Press.
Bruess, C. J. S., & Pearson, J. C. (1995, November). Like sands through the hour glass:
Rituals in day-to-day marriage. Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the
Speech Communication Association, San Antonio, TX.
Buckingham, M., & Coffman, C. (1999). First, break all the rules: What the
greatest managers do diff erently. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Burleson, B. R., & Samter, W. (1994). A socials kills approach to relationship mainte-
nance: How individual differences in communication skills affect the achievement
of relationship functions. In D. J. Canary & L. S. Stafford (Eds.), Communication
and relational; maintenance (pp. 62-90). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1992). Relational maintenance strategies and equity in
marriage. Communication Monographs, 59, 243-267.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1993). P reservation of relational characteristics: Mainte-
nance strategies, equity, and locus of control. In I? J. Kalbfleisch (Ed.), Interpersonal
communication: Evolving interpersonal relationships (pp. 237-259). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Canary, D. J., Stafford, L., Hause, K. S., & Wallace, L. A. (1993). An inductive analysis
of relational maintenance strategies: Comparison among lovers, relatives, friends,
and others. Communication Research Reports, 10, 5-l 4.
Canary, D. J., & Zelley, E. D. (2000). C urrent research programs on relational mainte-
nance behaviors. Communication Yearbook, 23 (pp. 305-339).
1. RELATIONAL MAINTENANCE COMMUNICATION +e 2,-

Dainton, M. (1995, November). lnteruction in maintained marriages: A description of


type, relative routineness, and perceived importance. Paper presented at the annual
meeting of the Speech Communication Association; San Antonio, TX.
Dainton, M. (2000). Maintenance behaviors, expectations for maintenance, and satis-
faction: Linking comparison levels to relational maintenance strategies. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 827-853.
Dainton, M., & Stafford, L. (1993). Routine maintenance behaviors: A comparison of
relationship type, partner similarity, and sex differences. Journal of Social and Per-
sonal Relationships, 10, 255-27 1.
Dainton, M., Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1994). Maintenance strategies and physical
affection as predictors of love, liking, and satisfaction in marriage. Communication
Reports, 7, 88-98.
Davis, M. S. (1973). Intimate relations. New York: Free Press.
Dindia, K. (1994). A multiphasic view of relationship maintenance strategies. In D. J.
Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational maintenance (pp.
91-l 10). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Dindia, K., & Baxter, L. (1987). Strategies for maintaining and repairing marital rela-
tionships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 4, 143-l 58.
Dindia, K., & Canary, D. J. (1993). Definitions and theoretical perspectives on main-
taining relationships, Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, IO, 163-l 73.
Dindia, K., Gilbertson, J., Grob, L., Langan, E., Sahlstein, E., & Schuh, R. (1995,
June). The ritual of holiday greetings and the maintenance of weak ties. Paper pre-
sented at the International Network on Personal Relationships conference;
Williamsburg, PA.
Duck, S. (1988). Relating to others. Stratford, England: Open University Press.
Gilbertson, J., Dindia, K., & All en, M. (1998). Relational continuity constructional
units and the maintenance of relationships. Journal of SociaZ and Personal Relation-
ships, 15, 774-790.
Gottman, J. M. (1979). Marital interaction: Experimental investigations. New York:
Academic Press.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce: The relationship between marital pro-
cesses and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Guerrero, L. K., Eloy, S. V, & Wabnik, A. I. (1993). in in g maintenance strategies to
relationship development and disengagement: A reconceptualization. Journal of So-
cial and Personal Relationships, 10, 273-284.
Hess, J. A. (2000). Maintaining nonvoluntary relationships with disliked partners: An
investigation into the use of distancing behaviors. Human Communication Research,
26, 458-488.
Kaplan, R. E. (1975/l 976). Maintaining interpersonal relationships: A bipolar theory.
Interpersonal Development, 6, 106-I 19.
Knapp, M. L., & Vangelisti, A. L. (2000). Interpersonal communication and human rela-
tionships (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Lee, J., & Jablin, F. (1995). Maintenance communication in superior-subordinate work
relationships. Human Communication Research, 22, 220-257.
Levinger, G. (1965). Marital cohesiveness and dissolution: An integrative review. Jour-
nal of Marriage and the Family, 27, 19-28.
Levinger, G. (1976). A social psychological perspective on marital dissolution. Journal
of Social Issues, 32, 21-47.
Levinger, G. (1979). A social exchange view on the dissolution of pair relationships. In
R. L. Burgess & T. L. Huston (Eds.), Social Exchange in Developing Relationships
(pp. 169-l 93). New York: Academic Press.
Lewin, K. (1951). FieZd Theory in Social Science. New York: Harper.
2i? -e=+ DINDIA

Locke, H. J., & Williamson, R. C. (1958). Marital adjustment: A factor analysis study.
American Sociological Review, 23, 562-569.
Milardo, R. M., & Wellman, B. (1992). The personal is social. Journal of Social and Per-
sonal Relationships, 9, 339-342.
Montgomery, B. M. (1993). Relationship maintenance versus relationship change: Dia-
lectical dilemma. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 205-224.
Norton, R. (1983). M easuring marital quality: A critical look at the dependent variable.
Journal Of Marriage and the Family, 4.5, 14 l-l 5 1.
Oliker, S. J. (1989). B estf riends and marriage: Exchange among women. Berkeley: Uni-
versity of California.
Ray, G ., & Poulsen, S. (1994, May). Processes of maintaining social relationships: The
case ofAmerican Christmas letters. Paper presented at the Symbolic Interaction and
Ethnographic Research Conference.
Rusbult, C. E., & Buunk, B. P (1993) .C ommitment processes in close relationships: An
interdependence analysis. Journal ofSocial and Personal Relationships, IO, 175-204.
Shea, B. C., & Pearson, J. C. (1986). The effects of relationship type, partner intent,
and gender on the selection of relationship maintenance strategies. Communication
Monographs, 53, 354, 364.
Sigman, S . J. (199 1). Handling the discontinuous aspects of continuing social relation-
ships: Toward research of the persistence of social forms. Communication Theory, I,
106-127.
Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the qual-
ity of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 15-30.
Spiegelhoff, M., & Dindia, K. (2001, June-July). perceptions of relational
maintenance strategies and relationship satisfaction. Paper presented at the Interna-
tional Network on Personal Relationships Conference; Prescott, AZ.
Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1991). Maintenance strategies and romantic relationship
type, gender and relational characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 8, 2 17-242.
Thibaut, J. W, & Kelley, H. H. (1959). Th e social psychology of groups. New York:
Wiley.
Tolhuizen, J. H. (1989). Affinity-seeking in developing relationships. Communication
Reports, 2, 83-91.
Tolhuizen, J. H. (1992, November). The association of relational factors to intensifica-
tion strategy use. Paper presented at the annual convention of the Speech Commu-
nication Association; Chicago, IL.
Waldron, V (1991). A c h ieving communication goals in superior-subordinate relation-
ships: The multi-functionality of upward maintenance tactics. Communication
Monographs, 58, 289-306.
Wilmot, W W (1987). Dyadic C ommunication. New York: Random House.
Maintainin Different Types
o F Re-f ationships
This page intentionally left blank
Sally Vogl-Bauer
Uniuersi ty of Wisconsin-Whi tewater

fall the relation types studied, perhaps the ones most neglected,
overlooked, or taken for granted by individuals are those of familial origin.
Societal cliches about family relationships abound, such as “your family is
always there for you” or “blood is thicker than water.” Yet little time or re-
search has been done on how families maintain “the ties that bind.” In fact,
casual observers might postulate that people do not care about their fami-
lies because family members are often treated less favorably than individu-
als having no biological or legal connection.
When research first began on relational maintenance in the 198Os, stra-
tegically working on sustaining existing relationships was a relatively new
concept (Ayres, 1983; Dindia & Canary, 1993). As research on relationship
maintenance grew, the primary focus was on how to maintain marital or ro-
mantic relationships (Fitzpatrick & Badzinski, 1994). Strong marital rela-
tionships are extremely important for families. However, there are some
problems deducing family relational maintenance from what is known
about marital relational maintenance. First, relational maintenance activi-
ties might vary dramatically in accordance to the type of relationship under
investigation (Dindia & Canary, 1993). Second, the majority of family rela-
tionships are characterized as relationships of circumstance (McGoldrick,
52 +=e VOGL-BAUER

Heiman, & Carter, 1993; Peterson, 1986; Vangelisti, 1993). Marital rela-
tionships are perceived as relationships of choice. Particularly in western
culture, it is customary to choose marital partner. Although these
distinctions are not inherently problematic, it becomes an important issue
if other family relationships are ignored when attempting to explain and
examine how individuals maintain family relationships.
(1993) essay on communication in the family was one of the
first to discuss the maintenance of family relationships. This is a rather
large task and several approaches could be taken. The primary goal of this
chapter is to examine family relational maintenance by integrating re-
search on family communication patterns with the findings on relational
maintenance strategies. In either case, research examining family rela-
tional maintenance is still in the preliminary stages of development. This
chapter first examines the relationship between relational maintenance
and communication in families and developmental issues relevant to rela-
tional maintenance. Research in both areas has important implications for
understanding family maintenance across the lifespan (Stafford & Bayer,
1993). Second, this chapter explores three possible theoretical frame-
works for examining family maintenance: Systems Theory, Exchange The-
ories, and Relational Dialectics Theory. Each perspective has been
incorporated either implicitly or explicitly in scholarship relevant to rela-
tional maintenance (Dindia & Canary, 1993). The final section discusses
the role technology may play in family relational maintenance now and in
the future.

l=FAMILY MA1NTlENANC.E. COMMUNICATION


AND Dl3ELOPMENTAL ISSUlZS

When examining family relational maintenance two issues are at the fore-
front: communication in families and family developmental issues. Nu-
merous scholars have highlighted the importance of communication for
families (Bhushan, 1993; Fitzpatrick & Badzinski, 1985, 1994). Unfortu-
nately, very little research specifically focuses on communication
strategies for maintaining relationships. When families are studied across
the lifespan, different familial challenges may influence relational mainte-
nance (McGoldrick et al., 1993). Thus, each area is examined in greater
detail, to underscore the relationship of communication and developmen-
tal issues to family relational maintenance.

Communication in Families

The value of communication in family relationships is significant, however,


the importance of family communication has not always been acknowl-
2. FAMILY MAINTENANCE =c- 33

edged. When Fitzpatrick and Badzinski (1985) reviewed communication


behavior in families, they reported that many family theories either had ig-
nored or undervalued the role of communication, despite the fact that
families are the primary context wherein children learn effective interac-
tion skills (Booth-Butterfield & Sidelinger, 1997; Noller, 1995). When
Fitzpatrick and Badzinski (1994) updated their review of family communi-
cation research, the significance of communication in families was becom-
ing more apparent, as scholarship published both inside and outside of the
field of communication touts the importance of communication for fami-
lies (Bhushan, 1993; Booth-Butterfield & Sidelinger, 1997).
Relational maintenance behaviors provide an important avenue for ex-
amining the communication dynamics in families. Relational maintenance
behaviors are primarily demonstrated through the use of verbal and non-
verbal messages. What family members say to each other, as well as their
behavioral patterns, serve as a reflection for how families maintain their re-
lationships. Specifically, the exchange of messages between family mem-
bers is paramount to demonstrate three different strategies of relationship
maintenance: positivity, openness, and assurance. Although actual behav-
iors can supplement these efforts, family members typically satisfy these
patterns through frequent or routine verbal interactions between mem-
bers (Miller & Lane, 1991). Although the relationship between the rela-
tional maintenance strategies of positivity, openness, and assurance
identified by Stafford and Canary (1991) to communication is evident, the
relational maintenance strategies of shared networks and shared tasks can
be easily linked to family interaction. For example, when family members
participate in shared activities (e.g., eat dinner together, go on a family va-
cation) or interact with similar groups of people (e.g., attend a neighbor-
hood block party), the event serves as a catalyst for one-on-one exchanges
between family members. As a result, if research is going to address how
families maintain their relationships, communication should play an inte-
gral role in the process (Bhushan, 1993).
Unfortunately, there is a dearth of research explicitly studying mainte-
nance strategies by families. Thus, in order to make assumptions about
family maintenance behaviors, connections to relevant communication
variables influencing family maintenance must be made. One such exam-
ple is the significance of listening skills. Family listening skills are
critical to whether or not family relational maintenance strategies have the
opportunity to work effectively (Nelson & Lott, 1990). Effective listening
conveys involvement with and attention to other family members (Galvin
& Brommel, 2000; Steen SKSchwartz, 1995); behaviors inherently helpful
to maintaining family relationships. Although it is important for all family
members to listen effectively, parents may find that listening to their chil-
dren can be especially challenging. “Parents especially get extremely
ego-involved with their kids; they take things even more personally, be-
?I$ -IS+ VOGL-BAUER

cause they feel they may not be good-enough parents” (Nelson & Lott,
1990, pp. 229-230). A s a result, if family members believe that their voice
is not being heard correctly, either parent or child may withdraw, or mod-
ify their maintenance messages to coincide with the relational dynamics.
The maintenance strategies most likely to be impacted by this type of ex-
change are openness, positivity, and assurance because the reduction in
overall family exchanges is likely to have a negative impact on the degree of
positive or encouraging remarks made.
From a pragmatic perspective, if family members have poor listening
skills, their likelihood to withhold information from each other, or inhibit
conversation between family members is probably great (Steen &
Schwartz, 1995). According to communication boundary management,
individuals can determine who has access to personal information in fam-
ilies (Petronio, Ellemers, Giles, & Gallois, 1998). If relational dynamics
are questionable, family members are less likely to reveal personal infor-
mation to others. Research also suggests a positive relationship between
how frequently a topic is discussed and the degree of self-disclosure pro-
vided by the family member (Noller, 1994). Thus, if family members
only talk about a small number of topics with any regularity, family mem-
bers may place greater restrictions on what they consider private knowl-
edge. Such restrictions are problematic when families attempt to
develop relational maintenance patterns. Relational maintenance strate-
gies implicitly, as well as explicitly rely on family willingness to
self-disclose information to each other. Reduced disclosures are likely to
have a negative impact on the ability to maintain successful rela-
tions between its members.

DeveloDtnental Issues

Although families encounter transitional periods across the lifespan, two


stages are especially relevant to the study of family relational maintenance:
families with adolescent children, and families at midlife and beyond. The
period of adolescence and the transformation of families at midlife are par-
ticularly relevant to family maintenance for several reasons: (a) each stage
presents a series of changes for multiple family members; (b) all family
members may actively participate in the maintenance during each
stage; and (c) the research available on each stage allows for reasonable in-
ferences to family relational maintenance behaviors.

Adolescence. The period of adolescence is often accented due to vast


amounts of change occurring for family members (McGoldrick et al.,
1993). Adolescent development has been examined from several perspec-
tives, ranging from biological indicators of adolescence to dependency is-
2. FAMILY MAINTENANCE ++- $5

sues for parents and children (Collins & Repinski, 1994). During the years
often associated with adolescence (e.g., from ages 11 to 19; Ambert,
1997), a great deal can occur for individual family members that poten-
tially impact all other family members as well, as issues of responsibility
and social status change (Boxer & Petersen, 1986). The time frame may get
skewed if individuals associate the period of adolescence exclusively with
puberty, which typically occurs between the ages of 11 to 17, as opposed
to viewing the period of adolescence in terms of successfully establishing
autonomy or independence. For example, if financial dependency is a
marker for adolescence, the time frame may extend into a
midtwenties (Ambert, 1997).
One reason adolescence is such a highlighted developmental stage is
that there are behavioral, emotional, and value adjustments occurring
(Montemayor, 1986; Noller, 1995). Although changes occur at the indi-
vidual level, family interactions may be impacted on a larger scale. As a re-
sult, both parents and adolescents may find themselves modifying their
communication patterns to accommodate new situations (Bhushan,
1993), which can be stressful for all parties involved (Hartos & Power,
2000). Furthermore, parent-child exchanges may later influence spousal
interactions or sibling relationships. Thus, the ability to maintain family re-
lationships during adolescence can get rather complicated.
Research has shown that the influence over children varies dur-
ing adolescence. Parental ability to influence an behaviors may
be compounded by social changes occurring, as well as what is being valued
by society at the time (Ambert, 1997). 0 ne of the more salient features
studied during adolescence, the peer group, has received extensive cover-
age due to its strong influence over children during their early to midteens.
Family members are in a potentially precarious position during this period
if the relationship between parents and children was not reasonably estab-
lished early on. After a child reaches 16 to 17 years of age, the influence of
family members, parents in particular, could be re-emphasized or strength-
ened if the relationship was initially solid (Golish, 2000). Thus, family
maintenance may “look” different from a few years before. Attachment is-
sues between parents and children compound relational dynamics be-
tween family members. In addition, the dialectic between autonomy and
connectedness may potentially undermine how and what family members
do to maintain their relationships. For example, the type of relational strat-
egy may vary, or perhaps the significance of the strategy for family mem-
bers may change as family members cope with issues of independence.
The communicative dynamics during adolescence are also complex be-
cause parents and adolescents typically have divergent views about percep-
tions of their family (Noller, 1995). R esearch has shown that parents tend
to perceive and communicate about their families in more optimistic
tones, whereas adolescents tend to be more pragmatic to critical in their
36 -+=+ VOGL-BAUER

perceptions and communicative displays about their families (Bhushan,


1993). These discrepancies may make it inherently more complicated for
family members to successfully maintain their relationships. It is especially
problematic if one family member believes that things are running
smoothly, whereas another believes that there are numerous problems in
the relationship.
In summary, family relational maintenance experiences flux just like
any other relationship. However, marital couples or friendships do not en-
counter the relational stages encountered by families, in particular the de-
velopmental stage of adolescence. Thus, families encounter a set of
developmental issues that necessitate flexibility and adjustment in order
to accommodate the relational issues encountered during this period of
growth and transition.

Families at Midlife and Beyond. Families probably have the lon-


gest lifecycle of all relationship types (McGoldrick et al., 1993;
Vangelisti, 1993). Th us, as relationships between family members
change over time, family relational maintenance behaviors are also likely
to adjust accordingly (Weigel & Ballard-Reisch, 1999). Kinship ties offer
families an avenue for continued relational maintenance over time. Kin-
ship involves the sustained interaction of families with relatives and
friends, with the intent to share, participate in, and promote the welfare
of the family (Galvin & Brommel, 2000). Essentially, kinship mainte-
nance allows family members to stay in contact, keep abreast of changes
in one lives, and exchange family gossip. Depending on the level
of cohesiveness between family members, the expectations placed on
family members to maintain family relationships may vary. In some in-
stances, attendance at family gatherings may be mandatory, whereas in
other instances some family members may be excluded from events (or
never even told of their occurrence).
Often times, one person assumes the primary duties of maintaining
communication between family members. This person is known as the
kinkeeper (Leach & Braithwaite, 1996). Because kinkeepers maintain fam-
ily connections in a variety of ways, their significance in family relational
maintenance should be examined. Kinkeepers help maintain family rituals,
keep family members connected through family reunions, and facilitate in
establishing a lineage. In regards to family communication,
kinkeepers provide several important functions. Specifically, kinkeepers
(a) provide information to family members; (b) facilitate rituals, especially
family gatherings; (c) offer assistance, financially or physically; (d) help
maintain family relationships; and (e) continue a previous
work (Leach & Braithwaite, 1996).
One of the most recently recognized phases for families across the life
cycle, and also one of the longest, occurs at a midlife
2. FAMILYMAINTENANCE +=a- 3,7

(McGoldrick et al., 1993). The child rearing stage is near an end, yet
there still remains a large period of time for family members to cope with
prior to retirement. Golish (2000) examined the turning points in adult
child-parent relationships. The major turning points found were (a)
physical distance between parents and children; (b) the rebellious teen-
ager, pertaining to the decline and then increase in closeness as adoles-
cents age; (c) times of crisis for family members; (d) communication; and
(e) participating in activities together. Of the five turning points re-
ported, two events, communication and participating in activities to-
gether, can be directly associated with relational maintenance strategies.
The relationship between family relational maintenance and family turn-
ing points pertaining to closeness in adult child-parent relationships sug-
gests that relational maintenance strategies continue to play a pivotal role
in family dynamics across the life span of the family.
Once children reach adulthood, the potential for numerous entries and
exits from families occurs. Family members might marry, and perhaps have
children, increasing the size of the family, while long-standing family mem-
bers may die. As a result, family membership, as well as the status among
family members, may change. Either event can impact how families rees-
tablish and maintain themselves. Family roles may also change over time.
For example, parents traditionally provide the primary care giving for their
children when they are young, thus facilitating the majority of mainte-
nance behaviors occurring. These roles may be reversed when adult chil-
dren take care of their older parents (Cicirelli, 1993). The responsibility
for maintaining the family may be transferred to other family members. In
addition, the living arrangements for parents and their children could vary.
Each party may be living independently of the other; the child could be liv-
ing with the parents, or vice versa. Needless to say, the satisfaction with the
living arrangement could vary for one or both parties (Noller & Fitzpatrick,
1993), and family maintenance behaviors could be greatly impacted by the
change in proximity for family members.
Perhaps one of the most critical points for family relational maintenance
involves the death of the last parent. ‘A critical point for siblings comes af-
ter the last parent dies, when for the first time their relationships become a
matter of choice. Fostering such connectedness throughout the life cycle is
an important factor in cushioning families against the stressors of life”
(McGoldrick et al., 1993, p. 414). M ares (1995) identified several factors
that impact the likelihood of sibling contact: (a) proximity; (b) family size;
(c) sex of siblings; (d) presence of other relationships; and (e) ethnicity.
These factors are of interest because as siblings age, and as family connec-
tions are weakened, sibling relationships begin to look more like relation-
ships of choice as opposed to relationships of circumstance. As a result,
family members may consciously think about the maintenance of their
family relationships for the first time.
38 -I++ VOGL-BAUER

In summary, like families, relational maintenance behaviors are subject


to changes over time (Vangelisti, 1993). H ow families adjust to change is
often reflected in how their relationships are maintained afterwards.
Clearly, some changes are more traumatic for families to adjust to than oth-
ers. Therefore, the relational maintenance behaviors enacted may tran-
spire over a greater period of time. Yet if families are some of the most
enduring relationships that individuals have, it is important to understand
these relational maintenance transitions.

THlZOR~TICAL l=KAMEWORKS ASSOCIATED


WITH FAMILY MAINTIENANCIE

Three theoretical frameworks examine how relational maintenance can


be understood and applied within families. Each theory offers a different
perspective on how relational maintenance is assessed within the familial
context.

Systems Theory
One of the most popular theories for studying family dynamics is systems
theory (Galvin & Brommel, 2000). Systems theory is discussed in virtually
every family textbook, and it has intuitive appeal for understanding family
relationships. At the core of systems theory is the concept of interdepen-
dence; one part or person in the system relies on or impacts other parts or
persons in the system. Interdependence underscores the complicated na-
ture inherent when there are a variety of family subsystems to explore
(Galvin & Brommel, 2000). Essentially, family interactions become diffi-
cult to isolate because the implications from one behavior could
extend to the entire family (Peterson, 1986). Scholars have typically re-
searched various family subsystems in an effort to gain a greater under-
standing of family dynamics. These subsystems range from parent-child
dyads, sibling dyads, and same-sex-opposite-sex familial dyads. Thus, a
second component of systems theory may be applied: hierarchy. Hierar-
chy may be examined in family dynamics by assessing the age or power/sta-
tus of each family member to ascertain degrees of influence for each family
member. Research on each dyadic combination offers insight into how
family subsystems function. Depending on the subsystem under analysis,
relational maintenance may vary in both practice and application because
each family member has the potential to mutually influence one another
(Stafford & Dainton, 1995).
Inherent within the systems perspective is the concept of
nonsummativity. This concept suggests that the whole is greater than the
sum of its parts. The relational dynamics of the family create outcomes
5. MAINTAINING UNDESIRED RELATIONSHIPS ++i- 117

Task and Social Balance. One aspect of undesired relationships that


seems especially salient in the workplace is the difficulty of maximizing
task effectiveness when that task forces participation in an unwanted rela-
tionship. Unpleasant peer relationships in the workplace interfere with
successful task outcomes (Fritz & Omdahl, 1998). A case could be made
that this outcome should not necessarily follow, because keeping interac-
tions focused on task, rather than relational issues, is one way people create
distance (Hess, 2000). However, simply interacting on a task level is im-
possible. First, the general consensus among scholars is that virtually all
communication involves both content and relational information, so it is
impossible to remove the relational component from a communicative ex-
change (e.g., Burgoon & Hale, 1984; Watzlawick et al., 1967). Second, ef-
fective social interaction is a contributing factor in task success (Bormann,
1990). research shows that groups that tried to focus exclu-
sively on task concerns and eliminate any social dimension to their interac-
tion were less effective than counterpart groups that effectively balanced
task and social elements in their work. So, to maximize task success,
interactants in undesired relationships must find a balance between social
interaction and disengagement.

Multiple Audience Problem. The multiple audience problem is a


challenge for relational communication, whether the interaction happens
in the workplace or a social setting. It refers to a communicative situation
in which a speaker needs to simultaneously meet different, and usually
mutually exclusive, purposes with a single message (Fleming & Darley,
199 1; Fleming, Darley, Hilton, & Kojetin, 1990). The challenge is to ad-
dress the conflicting purposes in message construction so that all parties
are treated in ways that meet the social goals. Although this prob-
lem is not unique to undesired relationships, it is likely to present itself
when a mutual acquaintance is present for whom the relationship with
the target person is desired. In this case, a person may want to distance
herself or himself from the undesired partner without simultaneously
suggesting a desire to do so to the favored relational partner. The reverse
can also occur. If a third party is present who considers a relationship with
the target person unwanted, an individual may wish to show the third
party their dissociation from the target person (to avoid perceptions of
affiliation) while concealing that message from the target. Researchers
have found many creative ways that people attempt such deceit. For ex-
ample, people can word messages in a way that the target and the third
party would interpret differently, display nonverbal cues visible only to
the third party, or convey relational messages using indirect references
that the target person could not interpret (e.g., Clark & Schaefer, 1987;
Fleming & Darley, 1991).
4-o -c== VOGL-BAUER

Numerous exchange theories have been applied when studying family rela-
tional maintenance (Rusbult & Buunk, 1993; Vogl-Bauer, Kalbfleisch, &
Beatty, 1999). Although exchange theories possess different nuances and
characteristics, collectively they share many features. Klein and White
(1996) i d en tfi ie d several general features of exchange theories. First, ex-
change theories put the primary focus on the individual; families are collec-
tions of individuals. Second, in order to predict or understand an
choices, there must be a motivational factor present. Choices
are driven more by the motivations, as opposed to outside con-
straints. Third, self-interest directs individual choices. Therefore, family
members may be guided by their own desires, as opposed to the desires or
needs of the family. Fourth, individuals make rational choices. This sug-
gests that individuals are able to assess information logically when making
decisions within families. Fifth, inherent within every choice is the assess-
ment of rewards and costs. Individuals assess potential benefits or sanc-
tions from fellow family members based on choices made. Sixth,
individuals want to maximize their rewards or benefits. Essentially, family
members compare and assess their options to ensure that they receive an
overall net profit from familial interactions. Finally, individuals compare
their options and select the one that is the most beneficial or the least
costly to themselves; there is more regard for personal interests than those
of the family.
At first glance, exchange theories may seem calculated and self-cen-
tered (Peterson, 1986). Exchange theories are very individual centered
and appear contradictory to how families function. But on closer inspec-
tion, exchange theories provide an important perspective in understanding
how or why individuals choose to maintain their family relationships. For
example, a parent may decide to call his or her child when he or she gets
home from school on a regular basis because the parent feels comforted
knowing where his or her child is after school. Is this exchange costly to the
parent? Perhaps the telephone call may be considered a cost in terms of
time constraints or frustration and uncertainty if his or her child does not
answer the telephone. Yet the parent may feel that the benefits outweigh
the costs because he or she feels comforted knowing that his or her child is
safe, and it gives the parent a chance to touch base with his or her child dur-
ing the day. Granted, the rewards and costs should also be assessed for the
child as well. It may be more interesting to assess how consciously family
members actually think about the relational maintenance behaviors uti-
lized throughout the day. For example, do people consciously exchange
positive messages with family members in order to avoid a fight or get
someone to do a task around the house? Furthermore, is it a problem if
2. FAMILY MAINTENANCE =+i- 4-l

avoidance or strategic persuasion is the true motive for utilizing more rela-
tional maintenance strategies in families?
Exchange theories have been criticized for their insensitive approach to
family relationships (Klein & White, 1996; Peterson, 1986; Vogl-Bauer et
al., 1999). As a result, features have been added to respond to such feed-
back. For example, the time frame considered for exchanging rewards or
costs has been modified (Noller & Fitzpatrick, 1993). Family members are
often content to wait longer periods of time for a “return on their invest-
ment” than nonfamily members. In addition, norms of reciprocity have
been incorporated in order to examine how family members respond to
each other. In both examples, family members acquire greater flexibility
for demonstrating relational maintenance behaviors to each other. The ex-
panded time frame provides family members with a longer period to re-
turn maintenance behaviors to others. Presumably, one family
relational maintenance behaviors will encourage similar behaviors across
the family.

Equity Theory. One of the more popular exchange theories used


when studying family interactions is equity theory (Handel, 1992;
Vogl-Bauer et al., 1999). Equity theory incorporates the fundamental fea-
tures of exchange theories, however, equity theory emphasizes the impor-
tance of shared, comparable exchanges between relational partners
(Adams, 1965). A relationship is considered equitable if one ratio
of relational rewards to costs is comparable to the other relational
ratio of rewards to costs. Thus, both parties demonstrate comparable lev-
els of personal investment in the relationship, suggesting that the relation-
ship is balanced. Inequities are said to occur if one person feels
overbenefited (the reward/cost ratio is greater than their rela-
tional ratio) or underbenefited (the reward/cost ra-
tio is less than their relational ratio; Adams, 1965). Relational
partners adjust their rewards or costs accordingly in order to regain balance
with one another. When applying these features to family relational main-
tenance behaviors, equity theory suggests that family members are compa-
rably sharing the maintenance of family relationships in their efforts to
sustain familial ties.
Preliminary research by family scholars examining equity theory in fam-
ily relationships has identified qualifications for family application.
Vogl-Bauer et al. (1999) examined parent-adolescent relationships and as-
sessed the satisfaction of parents and adolescents when in equitable, as op-
posed to inequitable relationships and the perception of relational
maintenance strategies by parents and adolescents when in equitable, as
opposed to inequitable relationships. Although the child is typically
overbenefited while the parent is underbenefited in parent-child relation-
42 -c=+ VOGL-BAUER

ships, it was hypothesized that parent-child relationships would become


more equitable as children grow older. Vogl-Bauer et al. (1999) found that
although parents were the most satisfied when relationships with their
children become more equitable, at age 16 and 17, children were the most
satisfied when they overbenefited from the parent-child relationship. Re-
garding relational maintenance strategy usage, there was a greater likeli-
hood for the strategies of positivity and shared tasks to occur in equitable
relationships, as opposed to underbenefited relationships. Although the
primary responsibility for maintaining the parent-child relationship still
rested with the parent overall, it may not be perceived as a negative factor
for the parties involved. “If parents are equally comfortable with their role
as provider, it may be presumptuous to assume that adolescents and par-
ents can make this change quickly, or for that matter, that each will be sat-
isfied while this transformation occurs in their relationship” (Vogl-Bauer
et al., 1999, p. 42).
Handel (1992) examined the significance of equity in sibling relation-
ships. Equity theory may be demonstrated by observing how fairly siblings
treat each other, but it is also reflected in how fairly or comparably parents
treat each of their children. Handel found that when siblings perceived the
treatment from their parents as equitable, the sibling relationship was
strengthened. When parents did not treat all of their children in a compa-
rable fashion, negative ramifications were present in sibling relationships.
Thus, parental patterns of equitable behavior toward their children di-
rectly influence the ability of siblings to maintain relationships with each
other (Stafford & Dainton, 1995).
In summary, exchange theories offer a contrasting perspective for un-
derstanding how family members maintain their relationships. Al-
though some scholars believe that exchange theories neglect the
altruistic features present in family relationships (Peterson, 1986), oth-
ers suggest that the maintenance of families may be captured within the
parameters of exchange theory, in particular, equity theory. The chal-
lenges for researchers may be to understand the unique family dynamics
that impact how family interactions vary over developmental periods,
and across other family dyads to identify how maintenance behaviors
are exchanged in families.

Relational dialectics theory is a relatively new framework for examining the


dynamics in close relationships. The focal point of relational dialectics con-
cerns the bilateral tensions present in close relationships. These tensions un-
derscore the process as well as the contradiction between opposing forces
(Baxter, 1988). Th e metaphor of a “tug-of-war” captures the dynamic strug-
2. FAMILY MAINTENANCE =+ 43

gle inherent in relational dialectics. Relational partners are challenged to


manage the seemingly endless discrepancies that relationships encounter.
Although partners encounter many relational dialectics, three in particular
have been examined in relationship to relational maintenance: auton-
omy-connection, predictability-novelty, and openness-closedness (Baxter
& Simon, 1993; Montgomery, 1993).
The first dialectic, autonomy-connection, refers to the continuous
struggle to balance individual identity issues with relational partner con-
cerns (Baxter, 1988). In the context of families, how does someone main-
tain personal independence without alienating family members?
Depending on the role of the family member, the struggle to achieve bal-
ance between autonomy and connectedness may persist for an extended
period of time. Expectations pertaining to who should make the individual
sacrifices to their own autonomy may also exist. For example, children of-
ten think of their parents in fairly narrow ways. In fact, it may not be until
the child is older that he or she acknowledges that a parent holds interests
outside of the family. Of course, the reverse could also be said of parents;
the child could be expected to spend the majority of his or her free time
with the family. To make alternative plans that conflict with traditional
family get-togethers is very problematic.
The second relational dialectic is predictability-novelty (Baxter, 1988).
The predictability-novelty dialectic underscores the tensions between
what is certain versus what is new or spontaneous. The predictability-nov-
elty dialectic may be especially problematic for families because it is often
assumed that families will remain constant; as individuals, we can change,
but our family should stay the same. Thus, whenever a family member
does something deemed extreme in another family opinion,
there may be conflict to the extent that although relationships need nov-
elty to remain healthy (Baxter & Simon, 1993), the challenge for families is
to determine the amount of novelty desired.
The third relational dialectic is openness-closedness (Baxter, 1988).
The degree of openness in family communication may be assessed in two
ways. First, it could reflect the struggle to access or block information from
reaching family members. Depending on the age of the family member,
perceptions of openness could be associated with restrictions on television
programming or reading materials. The second way to examine open-
ness-closedness in the family is to identify the disclosure patterns of fam-
ily members. How open is the dialogue between and among family
members? Do some family members have greater liberties in which to dis-
close their views on particular topics, while others remain silent?
Although relational dialectics theory provides an interesting avenue to
assess family relational maintenance, it too has some problems. One of the
primary criticisms is that a great number of dialectics to examine in fami-
lies potentially exists. Thus, it is challenging to determine what to include
4-e -I+=+ VOGL-BAUER

and what to withhold (Griffin, 2000). Second, many of these dialectics are
subject to change over the lifespan of a family. Accordingly, it may be im-
portant to identify the relational dialectics most likely to occur across the
history, as well as to identify those dialectics that emphasize a par-
ticular period within the family.
Because change and flux in relationships are not uncommon, many ideas
proposed in relational dialectics theory have been examined as distinct fea-
tures in relational development, with a large body of research related to
each. For example, autonomy has been studied extensively by family
scholars (Bulcroft, 1991; Clasen & Brown, 1985; Montemayor, 1986).
Granted, regardless of the number of approaches taken, more is being
learned about autonomy. Unfortunately, the research being done across
disciplines on autonomy is not always integrated into a comprehensive as-
sessment. This issue is probably more problematic for relational dialectics
theory than the previous two discussed due to the structure and premise of
relational dialectics theory. In short, relational dialectics theory empha-
sizes the relational tensions that exist in family relationships. The struggle
for families is to determine how to maintain strong family relationships
successfully through these moments.

Circumplex Model of Family Functioning. Olson and his colleagues


in Family Sciences have been refining a model for assessing marital and fam-
ily interactions for approximately two decades (Barnes SKOlson, 1983).
The Circumplex Model of Marital and Family Systems emphasizes three
primary dimensions in marriages and families: (1) cohesion or together-
ness; (2) flexibility; and (3) communication (Olson, 1993). Optimal fam-
ily functioning occurs when balance is attained between levels of cohesion
and flexibility. Family communication patterns provide the means to attain
balance. Although model is traditionally placed within a subset of
Systems Theory, when contrasted with (1988) Relational Dialec-
tics Theory, many similarities exist. As in relational dialectics theory, fami-
lies struggle to balance feelings of extreme closeness (e.g., enmeshment)
with degrees of autonomy (e.g., independence). Families also have to man-
age degrees of flexibility ranging from rigid to chaotic (Olson, 1993). This
continuum is very comparable to the relational dialectic of predictabil-
ity-novelty. Although Relational Dialectics Theory has a third dimension,
openness-closedness because the Circumplex Model places major signifi-
cance on family communication, it appears that both approaches concur-
rently strive to understand how families manage the ebb and flow of
inherently dynamic relational features. The fact that each relies on com-
munication to ultimately accomplish this task for families suggests that
family relational maintenance strategies could be examined from either
perspective with potentially comparable results.
2. FAMILY MAINTENANCE ==i+ +,5

TKHNOLOGY AND FAMILY MAINTENANCE

Families do not maintain their relationships in a vacuum. Just like any rela-
tionship, families are impacted by changes occurring in society. As techno-
logical advances affect both our professional and private lives, these
advancements also change how family members maintain their relation-
ships. As the options available to maintain family relationships increase,
the scope and magnitude of family maintenance is impacted as well.

Utdizing TechtAg in FarniIy Maintenance

Presumably, family relational maintenance is accomplished through


face-to-face interaction. Although it could be conceded that other commu-
nication channels could be used to send family maintenance strategies, little
coverage has been given to these alternative channels. Yet as family members
age or leave home, the opportunity to rely exclusively on face-to-face inter-
action is problematic. Thus, families are adapting to these changes by utiliz-
ing channels that are better suited to their changing needs.
As family activities limit the amount of face-to-face interaction
among family members, technological devices are beginning to play an inte-
gral role in family maintenance (Carlson, 1999; Ticoll, 2000). Children are
faxing copies of their homework to parents in order to ask questions (or ver-
ify that the homework is being done). Parents may purchase cellular phones
or pagers for their children so that they may contact family members with
greater ease. For some families, technological tools have increased familial
interactions that would not have taken place otherwise.
Probably the most highly advertised technological tool available to fami-
lies to maintain their relationships is electronic mail, or e-mail. As access to
sending and receiving e-mail messages increases, more families are taking
advantage of this communication channel. Children can e-mail their par-
ents when they get home from school to let them know that they arrived
safely. E-mail is especially beneficial for families maintaining communica-
tion between members who are no longer available for face-to-face interac-
tions. E-mail is quick, accessible (in most areas of the United States), and
adaptive to individual constraints. Messages can be exchanged between
family members in a short period of time (Carlson, 1999). Time delays as-
sociated with traditional mailings are also significantly reduced. In addi-
tion, more and more families are purchasing computers. Finally, family
members are not restricted by when they can communicate with others
(Oravec, 2000). Traditionally, time constraints affected when family
members could call one another, due to availability factors. Now, family
members can send messages to one another at their leisure, without dis-
turbing the receiver of the message. The receiver can read the message at
d-6 -is+ VOGL-BAUER

his or her convenience as well. Thus, many constraints associated with


other communication channels, such as face-to-face access; time-zone
variances when making telephone calls, or the message delays associated
with traditional mailings are no longer obstacles.
Interesting solutions are being identified for using technologies to assist
parents and teachers in their communication about children in the class-
room. Effective communication between parents and teachers tends to lead
to more appropriate and cooperative behaviors by children in the classroom
(Cameron & Lee, 1997). Cameron and Lee found that voice mail offered
parents and teachers an additional means of communication to establish and
maintain contact with parents regarding specific messages, announcements,
and reminders. Although teachers and parents continue to place value on
other communication channels, voice mail provides an important outlet for
maintaining communication over the course of an academic year.
However, there are legitimate concerns associated with utilizing tech-
nology to maintain family relationships. For some, problems are associated
with learning and using technologies. A lack of basic computer competen-
ties may be the most salient for individuals who did not grow up with the
technologies (Neudecker & Burke, 1985). Contradictions also exist re-
garding the belief that technology will give people and families more lei-
sure time (Carlson, 1999; Neudecker & Burke, 1985). The assumption is
that families should be able to share more time together because technol-
ogy reduces the amount of time necessary to accomplish other tasks. But
other scholars argue that technology has just made life busier (Carlson,
1999) or more problematic due to the inherent struggles to manage tech-
nology (and the information it disperses; Oravec, 2000). Parents are given
the task of managing a technology for which they may have limited under-
standing or control. Thus, the roles, norms, or expectations placed on fam-
ily members could be questioned.
The role of technology in maintaining family relationships is still uncer-
tain. Some researchers view the role of technology very favorably (Ticoll,
2000), whereas others view the impact of technology on families with
greater concern or skepticism (Carlson, 1999; Oravec, 2000). The chal-
lenge for families and researchers alike is to identify when technological
tools are accessible and supportive of family maintenance and when tech-
nological tools become a detriment to effective family functioning.

CLOSING REMARKS

There is something intuitively appealing to studying relational mainte-


nance. Relational maintenance examines how individuals can keep the im-
portant relationships in their lives over time and, nonetheless, have the
potential to improve them. Thus, the ability to understand how families
2. FAMILY MAINTENANCE ++i- 47

accomplish relational maintenance is a compelling one. Although little in-


put exists regarding family selection, individual connections to families are
extremely strong. What can be done to nurture these relationships over
time has the potential to impact all parties involved, as well as society.
Clearly, if families are going to continue as a vital force in society, helping
family members maintain these relationships is paramount to their sur-
vival. Existing family theories may help to unravel the questions surround-
ing family relational maintenance. Systems theory, exchange theories, and
relational dialectics theory have the potential to reveal more about how
families maintain their relationships over time, as well as the obstacles fac-
ing families in their efforts to do so. As greater knowledge is acquired, the
impact for families and relational maintenance scholarship is great.
Two important items highlighted are communication and the use of
technology. In order for successful family maintenance to occur, mes-
sages need to be exchanged, either verbally or nonverbally. Advances in
technology offer potential tools to aid family maintenance. Family mem-
bers may incorporate technology to varying degrees to enhance family
maintenance contingent upon accessibility to each other or technological
tools. Perhaps cautious optimism should be used when aiding families in
how they choose to communicate with each other. What is known is that
without communication, the ability of families to maintain their relation-
ships is severely hindered.

Adams, J. S . (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in ex-


perimentaZsociaZpsychoZogy volume 2, (pp. 267-300). New York: Academic Press.
Ambert, A. (1997). Parents, children, and adolescents: interactive relationships and de-
velopment in context. New York: Haworth Press.
Ayres, J. (1983). Strategies to maintain relationships: Their identification and per-
ceived usage. Communication Quarterly, 3 1, 62-67.
Barnes, H., & Olson, D. H. (1983). P arent-adolescent communication and the
circumplex model. Child Development, 56, 438-447.
Baxter, L. A. (1988). A dialectical perspective on communication strategies in relation-
ship development. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook ofpersonal relationships: Theory, re-
search and interventions (pp. 257-273). Chichester, Great Britain: Wiley & Sons.
Baxter, L. A., & Simon, E. P (1993). Relationship maintenance strategies and dialecti-
cal contradictions in personal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 10, 225-242.
Bhushan, R. (1993). A study of patterns of family communication: Parents and their
adolescent children. Journal of Personality and Clinical Studies, 9, 79-85.
Booth-Butterfield, M., & Sidelinger, R. J. (1997). Th e relationship between parental
traits and open family communication: Affective orientation and verbal aggression.
Communication Research Reports, 14, 408-417.
Boxer, A. M., & Petersen, A. C. (1986). Pubertal change in a family context. In G. K.
Leigh & G. W Peterson (Eds.), Adolescence in families (pp. 73-103). Cincinnati,
OH: South-Western.
4-8 -I+=+ VOGL-BAUER

Bulcroft, R. A. (1991). The value of physical change in adolescence: Consequences for


the parent-adolescent exchange relationship. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20,
89-105.
Cameron, C. A., & Lee, K. (1997). Bridging the gap between home and school with
voice-mail technology. Journal of Educational Research, 90(3), 182-l 90.
Carlson, C. J. (1999). The influence of technology on families: An Asian perspective.
Family Journal, 7, 23 l-236.
Cicirelli, V G. (1993). Intergenerational communication in the mother-daughter
dyad regarding caregiving decisions. In N. Coupland &J. F. Nussbaum (Eds.), Dis-
course and lifespan identity (pp. 2 1 S-236). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Clasen, D. R., & Brown, B. B. (198.5). Th e multidimensionality of peer pressure in ado-
lescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14, 45 l-468.
Collins, W A., & Repinski, D. J. (1994). R e 1at ionships during adolescence: Continuity
and change in interpersonal perspective. In R. Montemayor, G. R. Adams, & T. I?
Gullotta (Eds.), Personal relationships during adolescence (pp. 7-36). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Dindia, K., & Canary, D. J. (1993). Definitions and theoretical perspectives on main-
taining relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 163-l 73.
Duck, S. (1994). Steady as (she) goes: Relational maintenance as a shared meaning sys-
tem. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), C ommunicution and relutional muinte-
nunce (pp. 45-60). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Fitzpatrick, M. A., & Badzinski, D. M. (1985). All in the family: Interpersonal commu-
nication in kin relationships. In M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of in-
terpersonal communication (pp. 687-736). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Fitzpatrick, M. A., & Badzinski, D. M. (1994). All in the family: Interpersonal commu-
nication in kin relationships. In M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of in-
terpersonal communication (2nd ed., pp. 726-771). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Galvin, K. M., & Brommel, B. J. (2000). Fumily communication: Cohesion and change,
(5th ed.). New York: Longman.
Golish, T. D. (2000). Changes in closeness between adult children and their parents: A
turning point analysis. Communication Reports, I 3, 79-97.
Griffin, E. (2000). A first look at communication theory (4th ed.). New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Handel, G. (1992). Equity and loyalty among brothers and sisters. In J. M. Henslin
(Ed.), Marriage undfumily in a changing society (4th ed., pp. 250-260). New York:
The Free Press.
Hartos, J. L., & Power, T G. (2000). Relations among single awareness of
their stressors, maternal monitoring, mother-adolescent communica-
tion, and adolescent adjustment. Journal of Adolescent Research, 1.5, 546-563.
Klein, D. M., &White, J. M. (1996). Fumily theories: An introduction. Thousand Oaks,
Sage.
Leach, M. S., & Braithwaite, D. 0. (1996). A bibdibg tie: Supportive communication of
family kinkeepers. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 24, 200-216.
Mares, M. (1995). The aging family. In M. A. Fitzpatrick & A. L. Vangelisti (Eds.), Ex-
plaining family interactions (pp. 344-374). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
McGoldrick, M., Heiman, M., & Carter, B. (1993). The changing family life cycle: A
perspective on normalcy. In F. Walsh (Ed.), N ormul family processes (2nd ed., pp.
405-443). N ew York: Guilford Press.
Miller, J. B., & Lane, M. (1991). Relations between young adults and their parents.
Journal of Adolescence, 14, 179-l 94.
Montgomery, B. M. (1993). Relationship maintenance versus relationship change: A di-
alectical dilemma. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 205-224.
2. FAMILYMAINTENANCE +=+- 49

Montemayor, R. (1986). D eveloping autonomy: The transition of youth to adulthood.


In G. K. Leigh & G. W. Peterson (Eds.), Adolescence in families (pp. 205-225).
Cincinnati, OH: South-Western.
Nelson, J., & Lott, L. (1990). I ‘m on your side. Rocklin, CA: Prima Publishing.
Neudecker, T E., & Burke, I? L. (1985). High technology: Opportunity or crisis for the
family? In L. E. Arnold (Ed.), Parents, children, and change (pp. 19-30). Lexington,
MA: D. C. Heath & Company.
Noller, P (1994). Relationships with parents in adolescence: Process and outcome. In
R. Montemayor, G. R. Adams, & T. I? Gullotta (Eds.), Personal relationships during
adolescence (pp. 37-77). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Noller, (1995). Parent-adolescent relationships. In M. A. Fitzpatrick & A. L.
Vangelisti (Eds.), Explainingfamily interactions (pp. 77-111). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Noller, P., & Fitzpatrick, M. A. (1993). Communication in family relationships.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
T. G., Hetherington, E. M., & Clingempeel, W. G. (1997). Systems and
bidirectional influences in families. Journal of Social and PersonaL Relationships, 14,
491-504.
Olson, D. H. (1993). C ircumplex model of marital and family systems: Assessing fam-
ily functioning. In F. Walsh (Ed.), N ormalfumilyprocesses (2nd ed., pp. 138-160).
New York: Guilford.
Oravec, J. (2000). Internet and computer technology hazards: Perspectives for family
counselling. British Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 28, 309-324.
Peterson, G. W (1986). F amily conceptual frameworks and adolescent development.
In G. K. Leigh & G. W. Peterson (Eds.), Adolescents in families (pp. 12-36).
Cincinnati, OH: South-Western Publishing.
Petronio, S., Ellemers, N., Giles, H., & Gallois, C. (1998). (Mis)communicating across
boundaries: Interpersonal and intergroup considerations. Communication Research,
25, 571-595.
Rusbult, C. E., &Buunk, B. P (1993). C ommitment processes in close relationships: An
interdependence analysis. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10,
175-204.
Stafford, L., & Bayer, C. L. (1993). Interaction between parents and children. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (199 I). Maintenance strategies and romantic relationship
type, gender and relational characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relution-
ships, 8, 217-242.
Stafford, L., & Dainton, M. (1995). P arent-child communication within the family
system. In T. J. Socha & G. H. Stamp (Eds.), Parents, children, and communica-
tion: Frontiers of theory and research (pp. 3-2 1). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Steen, S., & Schwartz, F! (1995). C ommunication, gender, and power: Homosexual
couples as a case study. In M. A. Fitzpatrick & A. L. Vangelisti (Eds.), Explaining
family interactions (pp. 310-343). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Ticoll, D. (2000). Th e 1 s are alright. teZe.com, 5(14), 92.
Vangelisti, A. (1993). Communication in the family: The influence of time, relational
prototypes, and irrationality. Communication Monographs, 60, 42-54.
Vogl-Bauer, S., Kalbfleisch, I? J., & Beatty, M. J. (1999). P erceived equity, satisfaction,
and relational maintenance strategies in parent-adolescent dyads. Journal of Youth
and Adolescence, 28, 27-49.
Weigel, D. J., & Ballard-Reisch, D. S. (1999). Th e influence of marital duration on the
use of relationship maintenance behaviors. Communication Reports, 12, 59-69.
This page intentionally left blank
Laura Stafford
Ohio State University

he quest to elucidate factors associated with marital stability or satis-


faction is long-standing and pervasive. Sophisticated investigations of fea-
tures of satisfying marriages, self-report measures of marital stability, and
applied programs aimed toward enriching marriages can be traced back to
the 1930s (Perlman, 2001). In addition, literally hundreds of investiga-
tions on stability or satisfaction in romantic relationships have been under-
taken in the last several decades (Canary & Stafford, 1994). This plethora
of previous research has served as the vanguard to study of a con-
ceptually related phenomenon: relational maintenance. As Canary and
Stafford observed, such studies explicitly or implicitly inform us about re-
lational maintenance.

I gratefully acknowledge the responses provided by Dan Canary and Marianne Dainton to earlier
drafts of this chapter. They have provided much stimulating discussion, deliberation and debate of
the material presented.

51
32 -+==s STAFFORD

Notwithstanding (1926) ob servation that any given family or mar-


riage is a constantly evolving and fluctuating process that may be best thought
of as a set of “interacting personalities” (p. 3) for several decades research
tended to focus on demographic characteristics or personalities of marital
partners to the neglect of interaction. It was not until the 1960s when the link
between interactional processes of the marital participants and martial satis-
faction became a focal point of relational research (Duck, 1985; Fitzpatrick &
Badzinski, 1985). Research during the 1970s indicated that communication is
strongly associated with marital satisfaction (Lewis & Spainer, 1979).
Reminiscent of (1926) conception of marriage as a constantly
changing entity, Duck (1985) reminded us that although we may be in-
clined to think of development and deterioration of romantic relationships
as processes; “It is worth pointing out that the maintenance and stability of
relationships are also processes” (p. 671). Furthermore, scholarly work
concerning interactive processes was directed far more often towards the
development or deterioration of relationships than toward the continu-
ance of relationships. This seems ironic given (1988) insightful re-
flection that people spend more time maintaining relationships than
developing or dissolving them. Yet the direct question of what do romantic
partners do to sustain these processual romantic relationships was largely
unexplored until the last 10 to 15 years and certainly the term maintenance
as a point of convergence for research on the role of linkages between com-
munication and the properties of personal relationships is relatively new
(Canary & Stafford, 1994), a point echoed by Perlman (2001).
Stafford and Canary (199 1) began a program of inquiry aimed at delin-
eating maintenance strategies. Albeit, certainly not the only scholars to ad-
dress issues of relational maintenance, their work serves as the focus of this
chapter. It is hoped that theoretical connections implied by their particular
model of maintenance will be made lucid in the upcoming discussion.
This chapter first provides a definitional and theoretical review. Then
Canary and (1994) overarching model of relational maintenance
is used as an organizational heuristic to discuss research invoking, expand-
ing, or challenging Canary and work on relational maintenance.
The primary focus of their research has been on romantic relationships,
and thus is the major focus of this chapter. However, this program has ex-
panded into other domains that will be given brief attention here as well.
Questions regarding this research are then raised. This chapter closes with
conclusions that can be drawn from this line of work.

THEORlETICAL UNDlXPINNINGS
AND A TiXMlNOLOGICAL ISSUE

From the beginning, Stafford and Canary (199 1) clearly laid out their guid-
ing principles. They have summarized the manner in which equity theory
3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS ++I- 23

informs their research (Canary & Stafford, 1992, 1994). Accordingly, a


highly abbreviated recap is offered here explicating two cornerstones of
the many principles that have continued to guide their efforts. First and
fundamentally, relationships require maintenance to continue; second, in-
dividuals are motivated to maintain relationships in accordance with social
exchange theory.

Duck (1988) suggested two models underlie relational stability: that rela-
tionships will stay together unless something tears them apart and that re-
lationships will deteriorate unless efforts are made to keep the relationship
intact. Although these two conceptions are not mutually exclusive (Duck,
1994) and Canary and Stafford (1994) concur with the proposal that barri-
ers act to keep relationships together, they focused primarily on the latter
of these two propositions. The statement that all relationships require
maintenance to continue seems intuitively obvious. Yet, given the number
of romantic relationships and marriages that end leaving one or the other or
both partners proverbially scratching their heads asking the question,
“What happened?” perhaps the necessity for maintenance is not as appar-
ent as it seems. In brief, a guiding principle is that some kind of mainte-
nance activity is necessary to keep relationships from deteriorating.
The second fundamental principle is derived from the tenets of equity
theory as laid out by Hatfield, Traupmann, Sprecher, Utne, and Hay
(1985); Sprecher (1986); Walster, Berscheied, and Walster (1973) among
others. Canary and Stafford (1994) explained, “equity theory predicts that
people are content when both persons have equal ratios of inputs to out-
comes, that people are distressed when involved in an inequitable relation-
ship and that people try to restore and maintain equity.” (p. 7).
Overbenefited individuals perceive they get more out of a relationship
than they put into it whereas underbenefited individuals perceive they get
more out of a relationship than they put into it. Being either overbenefited
or underbenefited should cause emotional distress (Sprecher, 1986). Eq-
uity should be satisfying. Furthermore, in a dyadic relationship one per-
inputs serve as rewards for the other person. Thus, maintenance
activities are conceived of as a cost or input for one person and hence a re-
ward for the other. It follows then, that individuals would adjust their
maintenance efforts in accordance to their perceived equity levels.

Issue

Stafford and Canary (1991) first used the term maintenance strategies and
offered their definition of maintenance as “efforts expended to maintain
54 +- STAFFORD

the nature of the relationship to the satisfaction” (p. 220). Canary


and Stafford (1992) o ff ere d a similar definition of maintenance strategies:
“communication approaches people use to sustain desire relational defini-
tions” (p. 243). Stafford and Canary (1991); Canary and Stafford, (1992)
were silent on the meaning of the term strategy. However, they did point
out that their investigations “focus on strategies and not routine interac-
tions” (Canary & Stafford, 1993, p. 242).
Dainton and Stafford (1993) expressed concern with these efforts di-
rected at ascertaining strategies, which they considered as behaviors in-
voked with the intent to sustain the relationship. They contended that
maintenance behaviors might also be less strategic. That is, although the
behavior itself may be intentional the “actor is not performing these behav-
iors with the express goal of maintaining the relationship. However, the
performance of these behaviors may indeed serve maintenance functions”
(Dainton & Stafford, 1993, p. 256). Drawing on (1988, 1992) dis-
cussion of routine interactions, they chose to label such nonstrategic activi-
ties as routine behaviors.
Thus, maintenance behaviors were viewed as a broader term encom-
passing both strategic and nonstrategic or routine activities by Dainton and
Stafford (1993) and the inclusion of both sets of behaviors was then set
forth as a central tenant by Canary and Stafford (1994).
Proposition 6: People use both strategic and routing interactions to main-
tain heir relationships. Maintenance behaviors are thus distinguished into
strategic and routine categories (Duck, 1988). That is, people maintain
their relationships both by using approaches they believe will function to
sustain their involvements and, through the practices of daily living, been
enacting particular routines which become part of the dyad (p. 10).
Nevertheless, the terms behaviors and strategies are ambiguously and
indiscriminately interchanged. The reason behind this ambiguous use of
the term strategy is centered in differing conceptions of the term strategy.
D. J. Canary (personal communication, August 2,200l) adheres to an ex-
pansive definition:

For me, a strategy is defined as an approach someone takes. In other words, stra-
tegic communication is implicitly learned and often mindlessly enacted. This is a
broad definition of “strategic” that encompasses a lot of behavior. Strategic ap-
proaches are often routinized but become more cognitively processed when the
routine plan does not work (Berger, 1997). So, I d 0 not see a necessary separation
between routine and strategic.

Stafford (see e.g., Stafford et al., 2000) however, invokes a more narrow
view of the term strategy. As Duck (1994) stated, “research which has fo-
cused on strategies impLies a conscious sustaining of relationships and con-
tinues a distinction between such strategies and the more automatic” or
3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS =+i- 55

“breezy allowance of the relationship to continue” (p. 46) that is the rou-
tine or nonstrategic behaviors. It is this implication that is at issue at hand.
For at least some relational maintenance scholars the distinction be-
tween strategic and routine appears to be a meaningful, albeit perhaps in-
tuitive one. (1994) distinction has already been alluded too.
Acitelli (2001) a1so d rew a distinction and argued the degree to which a be-
havior is strategic or routine is contingent on several factors such as the de-
velopment of the relationship and the situation. Indeed Dainton and
Stafford (1993; Dainton, Stafford, & McNeilis, 1992; Stafford et al.,
2000) proposed that the same behaviors may be invoked in both strategic
and routine manners depending on many factors. Dindia (chap. 1, this vol-
ume) also adheres to the primary delineation of strategic and nonstrategic
maintenance behaviors. However, she proposes the dichotomy between
strategic and nonstrategic may be too rigid and activities may be more or
less strategic or routine. Yet, she maintains the principle demarcation as
one grounded in the intent of the actor. Dainton underscores the intent of
the actor is the point of distinction between strategic and routine behav-
and has found little correlation between the strategic and routine use
of maintenance behaviors; some behaviors may indeed be invoked more of-
ten in a strategic manner whereas others may be invoked more routinely
(Dainton & Aylor, 2002).
The foregoing definitional issue is not raised as a point of substantive
theoretical division as both Stafford and Canary concur that both types of
maintenance activities occur. This is clearly evidenced in their principle
articulated earlier. Rather, the unease is with a potential ambiguity for
others. Therefore, the recommendation is offered that future work
should consider the terminology invoked and perhaps utilize the term
maintenance behaviors in place of strategies as more definitely encom-
passing term.
A final issue confounded with the earlier discussion is the extent to which
routine or nonstrategic behaviors operate within an equity framework. It is
unclear if nonstrategic maintenance efforts are also hypothesized to operate in
accordance with equity principles, or if this is reserved for maintenance strate-
gies. Duck (1994) contended that relationships continue, for the most part in
a taken for granted manner; that partners unlikely continue a relationship only
after explicitly sitting down with their calculators to determine their relative
costs and benefits. Nonetheless he noted that some unconscious accounting of

‘The choice of the term routine was not perhaps the best one. Routine seems to imply regular-
ity to actions or events. Duck (1992; Duck, Rutt, Hurst, & Strejc, 1991) discussed regularity and
routines in interactions, or patterns of interactions in a daily realm. Although it is recognized that
many such behaviors may be routine in a mundane day-to-day patterned manner, the distinction
within this line of work has been on the intent of the actor, or perhaps lack of intent, rather than
the regularity of the event. Possibly simply the term nonstrategic as the logical opposite may have
been a better choice than routine.
?c7 -e=+ STAFFORD

the overall fairness of the relationship is likely taken into consideration. Fur-
ther, he proposed that such mental accounting may become more salient
when a partner is considering changing the relationship. D. J. Canary (per-
sonal communication, August 2, 2001) offered a like opinion:

I do not think that exchange theorists, including equity theorists, believe that
people constantly take into account rewards and costs (or inputs and outcomes).
I think that most people have a vague running tab of who has done what, though
some of us have high exchange orientations (where daily ledgers are taken) and
others have low exchange (or communal) orientations. Equity is probably a vague
collection of inputs and outcomes that leads to both intentionally performed and
unintentionally performed actions. However, I would predict that it [equity the-
ory] is a more powerful predictor of intentional actions and planning. It probably
leaks through unintentional actions as well.

The extent to which relational participants view maintenance behav-


iors as intentional or unintentional and to which they accord a greater
role in the preservation of their relationships is yet to be ascertained.
Moreover, the extent to which intentional versus unintentional behav-
iors are affected by perceptions of in(equity) is also an empirical ques-
tion. Nonetheless, a point of convergence is the recognition that
individuals act both consciously and unconsciously to sustain their in-
volvements and these activities are likely influenced to a greater or
lesser degree of consciousness by overall perceptions of (in)equity re-
gardless of the terminology invoked. Attention is now turned directly to
the proposed model of maintenance.

CANARY AND RlELATlONAL


MAINTENANCE MODEL

Canary and (1994) overall model of maintenance is com-


prised of three main components: maintenance behaviors, antecedents
that may predict them, and salient relational characteristics that may be
predicted by them (see Canary & Stafford, 1994, for a discussion of
their central tenets.) These components are presented in Fig. 3.1. Al-
though the model conceptually begins with antecedents, the mainte-
nance behaviors are presented first as they have been at the heart of this
line of inquiry. A discussion of the antecedents to maintenance as well as
the outcomes of maintenance follows. This section concludes with the
proposal for a revised model.

The Maintenance Behaviors


This particular line of work began when Stafford and Canary (199 1) devel-
oped five factors (positivity, assurances, openness, sharing tasks, and social
networks) that individuals purportedly used in a strategic manner to sustain
3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS =a 2,;

Antecedent Factors Maintenance Activities

Equity

Underbenefited

Equitably Treated --b

Relational Type and

Individual Differences

Fig. 3.1. Canary and model.


Note: From Canary and Zelley (2000). Based on Canary and Stafford (1994).

desired relational characteristics. These five factors were refined and exam-
ined in conjunction with equity theory by Canary and Stafford (1992).
Following this initial effort to define and operationalize maintenance
strategies, efforts have been made to identify other maintenance behav-
iors. This has occurred in two primary ways. Sometimes an expanded con-
ceptualization of maintenance as “routine” or “nonstrategic” has resulted
in additional maintenance behaviors (e.g., Dainton & Stafford, 1993;
Stafford, Dainton, & Haas, 2000). Other times, maintenance research
moved outside the confines of White, middle-class heterosexual romantic
couples living in the United States in an attempt to identify various main-
tenance behaviors used in various populations. Research has expanded to
White, middle-class nonromantic relationships such as friends and family
(e.g., Canary, Stafford, Hause, & Wallace, 1993; Messman, Canary &
Hause, 2000), non-White romantic samples (e.g., Diggs & Stafford, 1998),
Non-Western romantic samples (Young & Canary, chap. 13, this volume),
and nonheterosexual Western romantic relationships (e.g., Haas &
Stafford, 1997, 1998.)
Table 3.1 presents the original five behaviors as well as the mainte-
nance behaviors generated within this program. Upon examination of Ta-
ble 3.1, it becomes readily apparent that category systems have at times
overlapped.
Exploration of behaviors occurred virtually simultaneously in two do-
mains. On one front, using inductive analyses, Canary, Stafford, Hause,
TABLE 3.1
ReIationaI Maintenance Behavior

Behavior Examples
-
Positivity” Try to act nice and cheerful.
Attempt to make our interactions enjoyable.
Ask how his or her day has gone.
-
Openness” Encourage him or her to disclose thoughts and
feelings to me.
Seek to discuss the quality of our relationship.
Remind him or her about relationship decisions
we made in the past.

Assurancesa Stress my commitment to him or her.


Imply that our relationship has a future.
Show myself to be faithful to him or her.

Social Networka Like to spend time with our same friends.


Focus on common friends and affiliations.
Show that I am willing to do things with his/her
friends and family.

Sharing Tasks” Help equally with tasks that need to be done.


Do my fair share of the work we have to do.
Perform my household responsibilities.

Conflict Managementb Apologize when I am wrong.


Cooperate in how I handledisagreements.
Patient and forgiving with my partner.

Adviceb Tell partner what I think she or he should do


about her or his problems.
Give him or her my opinion on things going on in
his/her life.

Focus on Self Working on a degree.


Focusing on spiritual or religious development.
I make sure I look good.

Joint Activities Spend time hanging out.


Attend Saturday football games.
Visit my brother when he is away at school.

Mediated Communication Write letters.


Use e-mail to keep in touch.
Communicate on the phone.

(continued on next page)


TABLE 3.1 (continued)

Behavior Examples

Avoidance/Antisocial Am not completely honest with him or her.


Avoid him or her.
Act badly so she or he want to get closer
to me.

Humor Call him or her by a funny nickname.


Tease him or her.
Be sarcastic in a funny way.

No Flirting Avoid flirting with him or her.


Do not allow myself to be in a romantic place
with him or her.
Do not encourage overly familiar behavior.

Give advice.
Seek advice.
Comfort him or her in time of need.

Share Activity‘ Share special rituals with him or her.


Share specific routine activities with him or her.
Share time with him or her.

Religion Attend church together.


Pray about our marriage.
Talk to a minister or priest.

Small Talk Talk about our day.


Talk about little things.

Affection Displays of fondness.


We kiss each other goodbye in the morning.

Gay/lesbian Supportive Living, working, or socializing in supportive


Environments settings.
Being in nonjudgmental settings.

Same as Heterosexual Modeling similar values as heterosexual parents.


Couples

Note. Table adapted and extended from Canary and Stafford (2001). Examples adapted
from Canary and Stafford (1992); Canary et al. (1993): Dainton and Stafford (1993), Diggs and
Stafford (1998), Messman et al. (2000), Stafford et al. (2000); and Haas and Stafford (1998).
Categories are those directly from work with Stafford or Canary as an author or coauthor.
Items without a superscript have not yet been developed into measurements.
aThe five original factors from Stafford and Canary (1991).
bAdditional factors from Stafford et al. (2000).
‘Additional factors from Messman et al. (2000).
53
60 -c=e STAFFORD

and Wallace (I 993) extended the repertoire of strategic maintenance be-


haviors to those used not only in romantic relationships, but also those used
among friends and relatives. In addition to the initial five categories, and a
mediated mode (noncontent specific use of cards, letters, and the tele-
phone), two more prosocial categories were derived (joint activities and
humor) as well as two less socially positive strategies (avoidance and anti-
social behaviors).
Building directly on this work, Messman et al. (2000) developed a
6-factor measure designed to tap how individuals maintain platonic oppo-
site-sex friendships. Two new factors were found: no flirting and suppor-
tiveness. The concept of supportiveness in general has emerged in other
work. For example, it is implied in Haas and (1998) study of gay
and lesbian romantic relationships. The examples or supportiveness of-
fered by Messman et al. are also quite similar to the factor labeled advice by
Stafford et al. (2000).
Mirroring Canary et (1993) efforts was an inquiry, also utilizing in-
ductive analyses, by Dainton and Stafford (1993). The primary difference
was that Dainton and Stafford believed that the shift in the conceptualiza-
tion of maintenance to include routine or nonstrategic behaviors would
yield additional behaviors in romantic populations. The nine categories re-
ported by Canary et al. (1993) were replicated and the repertoire of be-
haviors was extended with categories of affection and focus on self.
The effort to tap routine behaviors continued with Stafford et al.‘s
development of a 7-factor measure. This measure was constructed uti-
lizing the items from the Canary and Stafford (1992) scale along with
items developed from the deductive analyses of Dainton and Stafford
(1993). They f ound the original five factors of assurances, positivity,
sharing tasks, openness, and social networks. However, openness split
into two factors. They retained the label openness for one of these as it
seemed to reflect the self-disclosive aspect of openness. The other fac-
tor was labeled advice. Similarly, positivity broke into two factors: one
that seemed to reflect the original conceptualization of being upbeat
and positive and another that referred to conflict management such as
apologizing and being cooperative in disagreements. This refinement of
an instrument to assess romantic behaviors occurred virtually
simultaneously with Messman et development of the 6-factor scale
examining maintenance behaviors in friendships. Note in Table 3.1 the
similarity between the items labeled by Stafford et al. (2000) as advice
and those by Messman et al. (2000) as support. Indeed, several of the
items labeled support by Messman et al. directly mention “advice.”
A list of the maintenance behaviors that have evolved from their in-
vestigations was reported by Canary and Stafford (1994). Nix (1999)
began with this list in an effort to examine potentially negative mainte-
nance behaviors used to maintain friendships. He invoked Duck
3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS +=+I-

(1994), who noted a bias toward positive strategies in maintenance


work; although antisocial ones had been discussed in previous work,
they had received little attention. Sustaining friendships in light of a ro-
mantic involvement, particularly through potentially negative strate-
gies, was the heart of interests. In addition to the categories
previously reported by Canary and Stafford (1994), Nix identified one
other maintenance behavior: passive disassociation-emphasizing the
“fun and excitement” of being romantically unattached. Furthermore,
antisocial behaviors were found to be more direct and less covert than
previous research had indicated.
The attempt to identify maintenance behaviors has gone beyond
White, middle-class heterosexual romantic relationships. As noted, re-
search has sought to identify maintenance behaviors of friends and family
members. Other studies involving family members include Vogel-Bauer,
Kalbfleisch, and (1999) assessment of the factor measure of
maintenance with a White middle-class parent-adolescent population
and Hample and (2000) examination of the similarity of the
maintenance behaviors, using the -/-factor measure, between
and their adult children.
Another realm of study is homosexual romantic relationships. Haas
and Stafford (1998), using the same inductive method as Dainton and
Stafford (1993), questioned 30 gay or lesbian individuals in long-term,
committed, romantic and found the same behaviors as did
Dainton and Stafford and two behaviors unique to this sample: being in a
gay-lesbian supportive environment and being the same as heterosexual
couples. Hass and Stafford (1997) created matched homosexual and het-
erosexual samples and found, in a direct comparison, that the two sam-
ples engaged in virtually identical maintenance behaviors.
Maintenance has extended to non-White, middle-class romantic in-
volvements as well. Diggs and Stafford (1998), again using inductive analy-
ses, found middle-class African-American married couples to use
maintenance behaviors virtually isomorphic to those found in previous re-
search on Euro-American, middle-class marital relationships. One possible
exception was the sharing of tasks as a salient maintenancebehavior. Addi-
tionally, this sample stressed the role of religion and the church. Con-
founding this finding however is the fact that much of African-American
sample was recruited through church networks. Thus, although it has long
been argued that the church may play a greater role in African-American
marriages than in Euro-American marriages, this remains equivocal here
due to the nature of the sampling.
One similarity that emerges from the study of these two samples in
conjunction with previous research is that, when using open-ended sur-
veys, specific populations may mention behaviors that are particularly
salient to them. Obviously a goal of open-ended research is to uncover
02 -t+ STAFFORD

aspects that might not have surfaced when a previously developed mea-
sure is applied. The possibility that certain features might be unique for
differing populations is of course a major reason why measures previ-
ously designed for one population not necessarily appropriate for re-
search in another population. However, the failure of one sample to
mention certain behaviors, would not necessarily mean that such behav-
iors might be considered unimportant for that population. For example,
Diggs and Stafford (1992) proposed that sharing tasks may be men-
tioned more by Euro-American participants than by African-American
participants, not because sharing tasks is any less vital among Black mar-
riages than among White ones, but because historically, Black marriages
have had more egalitarian marital roles. Hence, sharing tasks may be
more of a challenge or issue for White couples. Similarly, gay and les-
bian samples have reported that participating in gay-lesbian friendly en-
vironments was important for the maintenance of their romantic
relationships. Given current cultural biases, heterosexuals may not feel
the need to mention a desire to be in environments that supported their
sexual orientation. Thus, the development of measures of maintenance
behaviors would ideally combine factors derived from the populations
about which the researcher is making inferences.
Whether friendships, romantic (heterosexual or homosexual), or kin-
ships, these studies have focused on close relationships in the U.S. At
least two studies have looked beyond the U.S. culture. Yum and Canary
(chap. 9, this volume) compared maintenance behaviors of Koreans with
those of North Americans. They found Koreans (versus U.S. partici-
pants) reported less reliance on maintenance strategies and found the as-
sociation between maintenance strategies and relational characteristics
to be less strong among the Korean sample. They propose that this may be
due to a Korean cultural belief that the partners form one unit compared
to a more individualized western belief.
Finally, Ballard-Reisch, Weigel, and Zaguidoulline (1999) took the
study of romantic maintenance behaviors to Tararstan, a part of the former
Soviet Union. Utilizing the S-factor scale, they found moderate amounts
of variance accounted for in the relational characteristics. It would be of in-
terest to determine if even greater amounts of variance could be accounted
for with additional of behaviors generated by a Tararstan sample.

-r-heAntecedents
Antecedents are theorized to influence the frequency and type of mainte-
nance behaviors invoked. Canary and Stafford (1994) specifically include
equity, type and history, and individual differences. Another focus has
3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS ++-

been social cognition, in terms of relational schemata (Dainton & Stafford,


2000; Weigel & Ballard-Reisch, 1999a) relational expectations (Dainton,
2OOO), gender-role orientation (Stafford et al., 2000), and perceptions of
behaviors (Dainton & Stafford, 2000; Weigel & Ballard-Reisch,
1999b, 1999c). These social cognitive variables are interrelated with rela-
tionship type and individual difference variables and are discussed with
those variables accordingly.

@n)equity. The most theoretically critical antecedent is equity. The


thrust of this research has been on the role equity plays in the prediction
of maintenance behaviors. One of the central tenets is that individuals
will adjust their maintenance efforts in accordance with their percep-
tions of relational equity. Canary and Stafford (1992) found support for
the overall contention that perceptions of equity were related to mainte-
nance behavior use. They found, for marriages defined as equitable by
wives, both wives and husbands reported greater user of maintenance be-
haviors than in inequitable marriages. Support was not as strong for hus-
band-defined equity.
Vogel-Bauer, Kalbfleisch, and Beatty (1999) reported similar findings in
that parental perceptions of equity-predicted maintenance behaviors; yet
perceptions of equity did not predict maintenance behaviors.
The hypothesis is offered that adolescents may expect to be overbenefited
in relationship to their parents. A parallel unflattering hypothesis was of-
fered by Canary and Stafford (1992) with regard to husbands. Just as teen-
agers simply may not expect to work as hard at their relationships with
their parents as parents expect to work on their relationships with their
teenagers, husbands may not expect to work on their marital relationships
to the same extent that wives do. Such expectations thus attenuate the ef-
fects of being overbenefited. Messman et al. (2000) also examined percep-
tions of equity and found platonic friends who believed their friendships to
be equitable reported a great user of positive maintenance behaviors than
individuals in overbenefited or underbenefited friendships.
Only one study, to date, has directly challenged the presumption that
equity is a primary influence on maintenance behaviors (Ragsdale, 1996).
Based in interdependence theory (Kelley & Thibaut, 1978), Ragsdale con-
tended satisfaction as determined by a favorable comparison of part-
ner or relationship against an imagined ideal partner or relationship would
predict maintenance more strongly than equity. He also critiqued Canary
and Stafford (1992) f or using an indirect assessment of equity. Several
concerns are raised about this particular study. As, Canary and Stafford
(2001) noted, Ragsdale did not conduct a comparison of equity verses in-
terdependence; using (1984) Marital Comparison Level Index
&I- +== STAFFORD

(MCLI) he only examined interdependence. Thus he was unable to test his


own hypothesis that interdependence would be more influential that eq-
uity. Also he did not find support for his hypothesis that interdependence
would predict maintenance behaviors in a linear manner. Finally he did not
test for a potential curvilinear relationship between interdependence and
maintenance behaviors.
To address (1996) c h a11en g e d irectly, Canary and Stafford
(2001) conducted another examination of equity in conjunction with rela-
tional maintenance using two direct indices of equity along with
(1985) MCLI. Th ey f ound both equity and satisfaction (i.e., interdepen-
dence) to predict perceptions of use of maintenance behaviors;
both of these social exchange models appear relevant to maintenance. Ca-
nary and Stafford (2001) thus provided the most direct evidence that eq-
uity and satisfaction are not disjunctive; they work together to predict
maintenance behaviors.
Dainton (2000) a1so p rovided some support that interdependence the-
ory may play a role in the prediction of maintenance behaviors. Dainton
modified Stafford and (1991) measure of relational maintenance
in order to test for comparison levels of relational mainte-
nance. That is, participants were asked to rate their use of main-
tenance behaviors, but she also asked participants the extent to which their
partners use of these behaviors meet their expectations. Dainton found a
positive relationship between expectations about maintenance be-
haviors being exceeded and relational satisfaction. Yet she reported that
performance of maintenance behaviors was a better predictor than the
comparison level (performance versus expectations) and thus provides
mixed support for the role of interdependence theory.

Relationship Type. Stafford and Canary (199 1) began with relation-


ship type referring to heterosexual “stages” of romantic rela-
tionship development (e.g., dating versus engaged versus married). They
did find an association between relationship development and the fre-
quency of use of various strategies. Specifically, Canary and Stafford
(199 1) found seriously dating or engaged couples perceived their part-
ners to be more open than did casually dating couples or marital partners.
Canary and Stafford also found that individuals who were engaged or
married perceived their partners to offer more reassurances than those in
dating relationships.
Attaching a different meaning to relationship type, Canary et al. (1993)
found maintenance behaviors to vary among friends, romantic partners,
and relatives. For example, relatives tended to offer assurances, share
tasks, and use cards, letters, and phone calls more often than friends. Ro-
mantic partners appeared to offer more positivity, openness, and assur-
ances than did frienrls.
3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS ==;r 6.5

A third manner in which maintenance behaviors have been explored in


conjunction with relationship type is (1988) typology.
Fitzpatrick developed a typology of marriages based on the ideologies indi-
viduals hold concerning marriage. Her typology is based upon the dimen-
sions of ideology, communication, and interdependence. Traditionals hold
conventional ideological values about family and marriage, report being
moderately expressive with their spouses (e.g., they may discuss impor-
tant issues and simultaneously attempt to manage conflict by avoiding in-
teraction), and report a high degree of interdependence. Independents
hold unconventional values, report a highly expressive communication
style, and are moderately interdependent. Separates hold relatively con-
ventional views about marriage, report little openness in the communica-
tion, and have very little interdependence. These ideologies are thought to
act as marital schemata, internal cognitive models, which then frame their
actions and perceptions within marriage (Fitzpatrick & Ritchie, 1994).
Given relatively strong support from a programmatic line of research for
the associations between these schemata and various communication be-
haviors, it seems logical that these same marital types might be related to
maintenance behaviors (Weigel & Ballard-Reisch, 1999a). Indeed, Weigel
and Ballard-Reisch found traditionals reported the most use of mainte-
nance behaviors, followed by independents, then separates who used the
least maintenance behaviors: Specifically, traditionalcouples were tended
to use of tasks more than either separates or independents. Independent
couples were more likely to use openness and assurances than separate
couples. Another difference was that traditionals were also more prone to
use networks than separates.
Also examining the role of relational schemata or ideology, Dainton and
Stafford (2000) invoked (1998) couple types to predict
maintenance behaviors. Unlike Weigel and Ballard-Reisch (1999a), they
did not find couple type to be a strong predictor of maintenance behaviors.
Dainton and Stafford (2000) reported perceptions of be-
haviors to have more influential role on own behaviors than couple
type. They offer the speculation that traditionals may report more mainte-
nance behaviors because they reciprocate their behaviors.
Several studies have sought to explore the link between perceptions of
behaviors and own maintenance behaviors. As already
discussed, Dainton and Stafford (2000) found the perception of a part-
particular maintenance behavior to have a strong association with
own use of that same behavior. Previously, Dainton and Stafford
(1993 j noted relational partners tended to offer similar strategies. More
directly, Weigel and Ballard-Reisch (1999c) reported mainte-
nance strategies predicted strategies. Furthermore, Canary and
Stafford (2001) f ound perceptions of maintenance behaviors to
be predictive of self reported maintenance behaviors.
-+a STAFFORD

Individual Differences. Sex as a predictor of maintenance use or per-


ceptions of maintenance use has been investigated several times; women
may use maintenance behaviors more frequently than men (e.g., Canary &
Stafford, 1992; Dainton & Stafford, 1993; Ragsdale, 1996; cf., Stafford &
Canary, 1991). Stafford et al. (2000) questioned these findings and noted
reliance on sex-role stereotypes to explain them. In a direct comparison of
sex verses gender, they found gender role orientation to be a significantly
better predictor of maintenance use than biological sex. It may be the cor-
relation between sex-role orientation and biological sex that accounts for
the previous sex differences findings.
Few individual differences have been studied aside from biological sex.
However, Prisbell(l995) 1in k ed maintenance use to some aspects of com-
munication competence. Stafford, Perry, Rankin, and Canary (1999)
found personal dependency to be positively related to perceptions of part-
ner positivity and openness for both husbands and wives. They found small
to modest correlations between structural dependency perceptions of
partner assurances and partner sharing in tasks.
Finally, although the model presented does not consider relational char-
acteristics as antecedents, a few studies have considered characteristics
such as satisfaction and commitment, for example, as antecedent influ-
ences on maintenance behaviors. This begins then to raise questions about
the direction of influence which will be returned to momentarily.

rhe dational characteristics


The final component of the Canary and Stafford (1994) model is relational
characteristics. Maintenance behaviors are, by definition, supposed to
“sustain desired relational definitions” (Canary & Stafford, 1994, p. 4).
These desired definitions involve facets of the relationship considered im-
portant, and perhaps universal, to close relationships. Drawing primarily
on previous research on relational topoi (Burgoon & Hale, 1984,1987) Ca-
nary and Stafford (1992, 1994) h ave maintained that numerous features
may be fundamentals of relationships, not only the often research marital
satisfaction. Features of romantic relationships that have been studied to
date as outcomes in this line of inquiry include control mutuality, commit-
ment, love, trust, liking and satisfaction. Prime consideration is given to
these as outcomes of maintenance behaviors.
Although satisfaction or some conceptual variation of satisfaction (ad-
justment, quality, etc.) is the most frequently studied outcome variable in
research on marriage (Fitzpatrick, 1987), C anary and Stafford have chosen
to examine additional relational features that may well be related to satis-
faction instead of only examining the broad construct of relational satisfac-
tion. The rational for the investigation of alternative relational
characteristics is their stance that such features comprise relational satis-
3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS =+ 67

faction. Moreover, they contend that various maintenance behaviors may


be variously related to different relational characteristics.
Research on nonromantic relationships has used simply keeping a friend-
ship at the status quo as the meaning of the sustaining a relationship as de-
sired. For example, Messman et al. (2000) examined strategies to keep
friendships between opposite sex heterosexual friends from transitioning to
a romantic interest. Nix (1999) a1so investigated keeping a friend as the de-
sired outcome in light of a concern that one is losing that friend due to the ar-
rival of a romantic partner on the scene. Such work is consistent with Canary
and definition of maintenance as these efforts are aimed at pre-
serving the relationship in the manner desired by the participants.
In sum, the foci of this research include the identification of categories
of behaviors utilized, in theory, to maintain relationships and exploration
of antecedents-especially (in)equity as predictive of maintenance use in
various populations. In the second half of this chapter, consideration is
given to the third and most important focal point: the association between
maintenance behaviors and relational characteristics.

DO MAINTENANCE BIEHAVIORS MAINTAIN


RlXATlONSHIPS?

It seems reasonable to pause at this juncture and reflect upon the obvious
yet heretofore unasked question: Are maintenance behaviors related to the
maintenance of relationships?

Are Maintenance Behaviors Associated


with Relational Characteristics?

The majority of studies have examined the associations between original


five maintenance behaviors and commitment, liking, control mutuality,
and relational satisfaction. Subsequent work on romantic relationships has
used both the original 5factor and the 7-factor scale in conjunction with
these features and others, such as love and trust.
Canary and Stafford (1994) summarized the consistent findings:
positivity, sharing tasks, social networks, and assurances predict control
mutuality and liking; assurances seem to have an especially strong link to
commitment. Positivity and assurances are associated with trust (Canary &
Stafford, 1993). Dainton, Stafford, and Canary (1994) found comparable
patterns. Similarly, Weigel and Ballard-Reisch (1999c) found that both in-
dividual and joint perceptions of maintenance behaviors were predictive of
the joint constructions of love, commitment, and satisfaction.
Stafford et al. (2000), using the 7-factor measure, again found assur-
ances to be a strong predictor of commitment and control mutuality and
68 +- STAFFORD

liking and also found assurances to be predictive of satisfaction. A new


finding was the association between conflict management and control mu-
tuality. Dainton and Stafford (2000), also using the 7-factor measure,
found assurances to be strongly linked to satisfaction. Research to date, as
Dainton and Stafford (2000) no t ed sh ows that assurances may hold the
most importance for sustaining a relationship. Positiveness seems to be
central as well. Openness, albeit discussed as a maintenance behavior, may
play a more equivocal role, as is discussed momentarily.

Esther-ea Len itudinabnkbetween Maintenance


Behaviorsan $ Relational characteristics?

Most studies have examined relational characteristics and maintenance be-


haviors simultaneously. A welcome exception is Canary, Semic, and
Stafford (1996) w h o assessed relational characteristics and maintenance
strategies in heterosexual married couples at three points in time at ap-
proximately 1-month intervals. They did not find maintenance behaviors
to be predictive of relational features at future times, nor did they find re-
lational features to predictive or maintenance behaviors at future times.
They found maintenance behaviors and relational characteristics to be as-
sociated with each other only when assessed concurrently. They argued the
effects of maintenance behaviors are short lived so they must be continu-
ously enacted. This seems quite plausible and comports to their contention
that maintenance behaviors are necessarily ongoing activities because rela-
tionships must constantly be maintained. This also leaves open this possi-
bility that the direction of causality runs from relational characteristics to
enactment (or perception) of maintenance behaviors.
Although they did not directly assess relational features, Guerrero, Eloy,
and Wabnik, (1993) may h ave offered the best evidence to date that mainte-
nance behaviors may affect relational outcomes. They investigated the con-
nection between the five maintenance factors at Time 1 and relationship
outcome at Time 2 (8 weeks later). Dating couples whose relationships had
remained stable or whose relationships had escalated since Time 1 reported
more positiveness, assurances, and sharing tasks at Time 1 than couples
whose relationships had ended or de-escalated. Their overall conclusion was
particular behaviors may play a facilitative role in sustaining or developing
the relationship or the lack thereof may contribute to relational demise.

lslher-ea Reciprocal Association Between Maintenance


Behavior-sand Relational Characteristics?
Although a fundamental premise, as outlined in the model presented by
Canary and Zelley (2000) is that maintenance behaviors influence rela-
tional characteristics, Stafford and Canarv (1991) speculated on the possi-
3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS =+t- 6.9

bility of the casual direction running the other way. Possibly relational
characteristics serve as antecedents to maintenance behaviors, rather than,
or in addition to, serving as outcomes. Despite this early speculation, this
possibility has been relatively neglected. Given the continued implicit in-
clusion of bidirectional&y, inquiry into relational characteristics as ante-
cedents, as well as the overall long recognized systemic nature of
communicative and relational processes, the potential chicken-and-egg
connection between maintenance behaviors and relational characteristics
deserves further attention.
Bidirectionality is implicitly theoretically evident in the inextricable
link between equity and satisfaction. According to equity theory, the
most satisfactory relationships are the most equitable ones and individ-
uals adjust their efforts (in this case maintenance behaviors) in accor-
dance to their perceived equity in the relationship (Adams, 1965;
Walster et al., 1973). Thus, operating within an equity theory frame-
work, level of perceived equity, and by definition, satisfaction, must be
considered not only as an antecedent to the use of maintenance behav-
iors, it must also be considered an outcome of those behaviors. An ex-
amination of whether equity is restored with adjustments in
maintenance behaviors is yet to be undertaken. Such an examination
would provide for a more complete test of the manner in which equity
operates in conjunction with maintenance behaviors and would be a
welcome addition to this literature.
Satisfaction has been considered both as an outcome and as an anteced-
ent of maintenance behaviors. Adhering to the viewpoint that the percep-
tions people have about their marriages influence the efforts they put forth
at maintaining them, Weigel and Ballard-Reisch (1999b) used a unique
dyadic design to explore the extent to which perceptions of marital satis-
faction, commitment, and love predicted the five maintenance behaviors.
They reported perceptions of satisfaction, as well as commitment and love
were predictive of maintenance behaviors. Specifically they found that
wives appear to engage in more positivity, openness, reassurances, use
more joint networks, and perform shared tasks when both they and their
husband report higher levels of commitment, love, and satisfaction. Also,
Dainton and Stafford (2000) f ound commitment to play a small role in
predicting assurances, networks, and conflict management in addition to
satisfaction as a predictor of assurances. However, given the minimal vari-
ance accounted for, they speculate that perhaps the direction is unidirec-
tional-from maintenance behaviors to relational characteristics. In the
Canary and Stafford (2001) investigation previously reported, they also
concluded both equity and satisfaction predict perception of partner main-
tenance behaviors. In the Canary et al. (1996) longitudinal study discussed
previously, links were not found in either direction leaving open the possi-
bility that the direction of causality may run both ways.
70 -I+=+ STAFFORD

The model presented in Fig. 3.1 as outlined by Canary and Zelley


(2000) needs to go a bit further to capture the implications of Stafford
and program of work and implications from equity theory. If in-
dividuals do indeed adjust their level of maintenance efforts in accor-
dance with equity that in turn changes assessments of equity, (and hence
satisfaction) and maintenance behaviors must operate in a systemic fash-
ion. Given satisfaction is conceptually both an antecedent of mainte-
nance behaviors and a relational outcome and has received some support
as both an antecedent and an outcome, it is not unreasonable that other
relational characteristics may also be related to equity in a reciprocal
fashion. The question is simply, which factors play a consequential role in
this system?
Fig. 3.2 presents a revised model to capture the proposed reciprocal sys-
temic link between outcomes and maintenance behaviors and to illustrate
that the outcomes may also serve as antecedents. The model has also been
revised to include the two additional maintenance behaviors of advice and
conflict management reflected in the Stafford et al. (2000) maintenance
scale as well as relational features such as trust and love, which have been
explored as outcomes of maintenance.

what Do ~eopIe Do to Maintain Their dationships?

The thrust of this research reflects the continued endeavor to answer the
simple question: What is it that people do to ensure the continuance of
their relationships in the manner they so desire (Canary & Stafford, I994)?
Unfortunately, the question that may have an answer is not the one of what

Antecedent Factors Mamtenance Activmes

Control Mutuality +

Social Networks Lhng ~pb


Trust-

Indwdual Differences

Contllct Management

Fig. 3.2 Revised Model to indicate reciprocal influence.


3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS =+ 71

people do to maintain their relationships? But rather: What is it that people


think they do, or report they do, to maintain their relationships?
Maintenance behaviors should, by definition, maintain relationships
(see 1994, d iscussion of this tautology). It is noteworthy that in
summarizing findings to that time, Canary and Stafford (1994) reported
that associations between openness and relational characteristics have fre-
quently been weakly inversely related to relational characteristics, after
controlling for positivity and assurances. Since then, Stafford et al. (2000)
also reported openness to be negatively associated with satisfaction and
commitment, after controlling for positivity. In a like vein, Dainton (2000)
found openness to be a negative predictor of relational satisfaction.
Such findings naturally lead to (2000) deduction: Perhaps
self-disclosure simply is not important to sustain desired relational fea-
tures. This observation follows from Stafford and (1991) pro-
posal that openness may be reported due to the widespread and deeply
rooted cultural belief in self-disclosure as the hallmark of a successful rela-
tionship (see Parks, 1982). Weigel and Ballard-Reisch (1999b) also noted,
self-disclosure is seen in the American culture as the sine qua non of a good
relationship. They raised the possibility that men might use behaviors such
as openness to fulfill such cultural expectations.
Men may not be the only ones suspect of adhering to cultural expecta-
tions. When probed either inductively or in response to scale items on ques-
tionnaires, the cultural emphasis on self-disclosure may serve as an impetus
for a social desirability effect by both men and women. Perhaps this strong
cultural expectation is at least part of the reason openness is reported by
both women and men. Despite the overwhelming evidence that openness is
believed to operate to sustain relationships or at least reported to be believed
to sustain relationships time after time openness, as previously noted, has
not been related to positive relational characteristics. Numerous studies
have reported slight negative associations between openness and relational
features, when controlling for other maintenance behaviors.
A curvilinear effect for openness has not been tested within this pro-
gram. Yet this appears to be the logical relationship. Complete and indis-
criminate openness has been questioned for some time in the field of
interpersonal communication (see e.g., Bochner, 1982; Parks 1982). Re-
search in marital satisfaction has long considered a curvilinear relationship
between openness and satisfaction with the assumption that increases in
self-disclosure include negatively valenced talk. A curvilinear relationship
would also be in accordance with a dialectal perspective that hypothesizes
a role for both openness and closedness in relationships (see Baxter &
Montgomery, 1996, for a review) or when considering the boundaries of
privacy or secrecy (Petronio, 199 1).
Thus, two issues are embedded in the earlier discussion. One is whether in-
dividuals are reporting the behaviors they actually engage in (or perceive their
72 -is+ STAFFORD

partners engage in), or do the answers reflect socially desirable responses in


accordance with cultural stereotypes. The other is the assumption of a linear
relationships between maintenance behaviors and relational characteristics
based in the perceived and self-reported frequency of the behaviors.
Although the behavior of openness has served as the point of focus here,
similar questions could raised with all of the behaviors. Do any of these be-
haviors actually play a role in the preservation of a relationship or of rela-
tional characteristics? Or perhaps are individual self-reports reflective of a
cultural ideology of behaviors thought to maintain relationships and it is
these behaviors that then are parroted back to researchers? And if they do
play a role, is it in a linear manner based on pure frequency or at least for cer-
tain variables, should curvilinear relationships be examined? And finally,
should the focus be upon frequency at all?
Virtually every study to date considers the frequency of maintenance
behaviors (see Dainton, 1998, for an exception using a diary based meth-
odology). Yet, according to Gottman (1994), in summarizing his long-term
efforts at predicting divorce, negative behaviors may play more of a de-
structive role than positive ones do a constructive role in relationships, in-
dicating that frequency of use may not be as salient as other features such
as perceived importance or valence. Perhaps the importance of the enact-
ment of various maintenance behaviors should be explored in addition to
the frequency of enactment.

SUMMARY

Canary and Stafford attempted to ascertain the communicative processes


that fill the space between the end of beginning a relationship and the begin-
ning of the end of the relationship. The specific behaviors of interest are
those that may play a role in sustaining relationships. They have offered sev-
eral tenets of their research (see Canary & Stafford, 1994) and proposed a
unidirectional model wherein antecedents influence maintenance behaviors
that in turn predict relational characteristics. The categories of behaviors
have been extended well beyond the five originally posed and various types
of relationships and populations have served as subjects of inquiry.
Nonetheless, several points of clarification are needed within this pro-
gram of work. The interrelationships among the characteristics and ante-
cedents, especially the role of satisfaction and (in)equity as both an
antecedent and outcome, must be clarified. Theoretically, if an equity
framework is utilized equity and satisfaction must be considered recipro-
cally related to maintenance behaviors.
Moreover, the terminology invoked should be carefully thought out. It is
argued here that strategies is not an all encompassing term and should not be
used interchangeably with the term behaviors. Strategies imply intent thus
3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS =+- ,q

implicitly excluding routine or nonstrategic behaviors from consideration.


Although position that strategies encompass both strategic and
nonstrategic behaviors is consistent with many other views,
position carries ecological weight (personal communication, Au-
gust 3,2OOI): “I am against using terms with a scholarly definition that does-
comport with everyday definitions . . . I believe in keeping definitions
and so intentional behaviors ARE conscious, because what ev-
eryday people would say.” In short, the term strategy likely does not hold
ecological validity as a broad definition. Although this distinction is relatively
unimportant within the halls of academe, the implications of terms take on
increasing importance as this line of study moves from theoretical to applied
settings.
Finally, at this time, strong evidence cannot be offered that these behav-
iors do indeed serve to sustain relational features across time. Obviously, it
would be naive and overly simplistic to hope to divine one set of behaviors
which serves to maintain relationships in all types of relationships in all
contexts. As Canary and Stafford (1994) pointed out, “not all relation-
ships may benefit from these strategies” (p. 19).
Canary and Stafford have not directly claimed to have strong evidence
these behaviors do indeed aid individuals in preserving or sustaining rela-
tionships in the manner they desire; they have consistently included cave-
ats. It is uncertain, if at this point in time, increasing the sheer frequency of
these five behaviors would serve to sustain desired relational features in
most romantic relationships. Nonetheless, sightings in undergraduate
texts of the five original factors as skills or competencies needed for suc-
cessful relationships are not infrequent. Even research in academic jour-
nals (e.g., Prisbell, 1995) h as referred to these behaviors as relational
competencies: “Maintaining personal relationships involves the use of five
strategies: positivity, openness, assurances, sharing tasks, and social net-
works” (p. 63).
Such global prescriptions are problematic and go beyond the claims put
forth by Canary and Stafford at this time. This program of research has
found that various behaviors may be differentially helpful pending the type
of the relationship (e.g., engaged versus married or independent verus tra-
ditional marriage), may vary with the life-stage or length or the relation-
ship, may be differentially linked to various relational characteristics, and
may not operate in a similar manner among various cultural groups. Thus,
although there is strong support of the overall linkages between equity,
maintenance behaviors, and relational characteristics, sweeping prescrip-
tions are not consistent with the extant findings.
Openness, when advocated as global maintenance behavior, may be the
most worrisome. It is difficult to imagine how sharing task activities, being
positive, sharing in each social networks, or offering assurances
might harm a relationship. However, the same cannot be said for the sim-
74 -is=+ STAFFORD

ple increase in frequency of openness. Overall, interpersonal communica-


tion and family scholars alike have retreated from prescriptions for
self-disclosure, and although the scale representing the factor openness in-
cludes items other than self-disclosure, self-disclosure seems to be the
crux of this factor labeled openness. Openness may well be virtually iso-
morphic with the construct of self-disclosure for a lay audience. Thus,
however unintended, this program of work may be not only be reflecting
the cultural myth of increased openness as healthy for relationships, it may
also be inadvertently perpetuating the myth of self-disclosure as a rela-
tional panacea. Perhaps the Stafford et al. (2000) scale that isolates
self-disclosure as a factor is a partial answer.

Notwithstanding the questions raised, several conclusions can be drawn


from this corpus of work: (a) a set of maintenance behaviors have been de-
rived using both inductive and deductive analyses to reflect the definition
of maintenance set forth; (b) these behaviors, and perceptions of them,
have been associated with equity in a theoretically meaningful manner; (c)
these behaviors are, for the most part, strongly associated with central rela-
tional characteristics, especially positivity and assurances; and (d) longitu-
dinal evidence supports the contention that these behaviors are
intertwined with relational characteristics.
Researchers are often their own best critiques and as such continue to re-
fine their theories that may in turn contribute to application. It is anticipated
this program will continue to evolve to address the concerns raised herein.

Acitelli, L. A. (2001). Maintaining and enhancing a relationship by attending to it. In J.


Harvey &A. Wenzel (Eds.), Close romantic relationships: Preservation and enhance-
ment (pp. 153-l 68). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Adams, J. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowtiz (Ed.), Advances in ex-
perimentaz social psychology, (Vol. 2; pp. 267-299). New York: Academic Press.
Ballard-Reisch, D. S., Weigel, D. J., & Zaguidoulline, M. G. (1999). Relational mainte-
nance behaviors, marital satisfaction, and commitment in Tatar, Russian, and mixed
Russian-Tartar Marriages: An exploratory analysis. Journal of Family Issues, 20,
677-697.
Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (1996). ReZating: Dialogues and dialectics. New
York: Guilford Press.
Berger, C. R. (1997). Pl anning strategic interaction: Attaining goals through communi-
cative action. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bochner, A. (1982). On the efficacy of openness in close relationships. In M. Burgoon
(Ed.), Communication Yearbook (Vol. 2; pp. 79-108). New Brunswick, NJ: Transac-
tion Press.
Burgess, E. W (1926). The familv as a unit of interacting; personalities. Familv. 7.3-9.
3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS +=+ 75

Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1984). Th e f un d amental topoi of relational communica-


tion. Communication Monographs, 51, 193-2 14.
Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L (1987). Validation and measurement of the fundamental
themes of relational communication, Communication Monographs, 54, 19-4 1.
Canary, D. J., Semic, B. A., & Stafford, L. (1996, June). Continuity of maintenance strute-
gies and their associations with relational characteristics. Paper presented at the bian-
nual conference of the International Network of Personal Relationships; Seattle, WA.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1992). Relational maintenance strategies and equity in
marriage. Communication Monographs, 59, 243-267.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1993). Preservation of relational characteristics: Mainte-
nance strategies, equity, and locus of control. In F?J. Kalbfleisch (Ed.), Interpersonal
communication: Evolving interpersonal relationships (pp. 237-259). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1994). Maintaining relationships through strategic and
routine interaction. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relu-
tional maintenance (pp. 3-22). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (2001). Equity in the preservation of close relationships. In
J. Harvey & A. Wenzel (Eds.), Close romantic relationships: Preservation and en-
hancement (pp. 133-l 5 1). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Canary, D. J., Stafford, L., Hause, K., &Wallace, L. (1993). An inductive analysis of re-
lational maintenance strategies: A comparison among lovers, relatives, friends, and
others. Communication Research Reports, 10, 5-14.
Canary, D. J., & Zelley, E. D. (2000). C urrent research programs in relational mainte-
nance. In M. E. Roloff (Ed.), Communication Yearbook 23 (pp. 305-339). Thousand
Oaks: Sage.
Dainton, M. (1998). Everyday interaction in marital relationships: Variations in relative
importance and event duration. Communication Reports, 11, 101-l 10.
Dainton, M. (2000). Maintenance behaviors, expectations, and satisfaction: Linking
the comparison level to relational maintenance. Journal of Social and Personal Relu-
tionships, 17, 827-842.
Dainton, M., & Aylor, B. (2002). Routine and strategic maintenance efforts: Behavioral
patterns associated with relational length, and the prediction of relational character-
istics. Communication Monographs, 52-66.
Dainton, M., & Stafford, L. (1993). Routine maintenance behaviors: A comparison of
relationship type, partner similarity, and sex differences. Journal of Social and Per-
sonal Relationships, 10, 255-272.
Dainton, M., & Stafford, L. (2000). Predicting maintenance enactment from relational
schemata, spousal behavior, and relational characteristics. Communication Research
Reports, 17 1-l 80.
Dainton, M., Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1994). Maintenance strategies and physical
affection as predictors of love, liking, and satisfaction in marriage. Communication
Reports, 7, 88-98.
Dainton, M., Stafford, L., & McNeilis,
K. S. (1992, November). The maintenance of re-
lationships through routine Paper presented at the annual convention of
behavior.
the Speech Communication Association, Chicago.
Diggs, R. C., & Stafford, L. (1998). Maintaining marital relationships: A comparison
between African American and European American married individuals. In V J.
Duncan (Ed.), Towurds achieving Muut (pp. 192-292). Dubuque, IA: Kend-
all/Hunt.
Duck, S. W (1985). S ocial and personal relationships. In M. L. Miller & G. R. Knapp
(Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal communication (pp. 655-786). London: Sage.
Duck, S. W. (1988). Relating to others. Chicago: Dorsey.
7~7 +=+ STAFFORD

Duck, S. W. (1992). H uman relationships. London: Sage.


Duck, S. W (1994). Steady as s(he) goes: Relational maintenance as a shared meaning
system. In D. J. Canary &L. Stafford (Eds.), C ommunication and relational mainte-
nance (pp. 45-60). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Duck, S. W., Rutt, D. J., Hurst, M. H., & Strejc, H. (1991). Some evident truths about
everyday conversation: All communications are not created equal. Human Commu-
nication Research, 18, 228-267.
Fitzpatrick, M. A. (1987). Marital interaction. In C. R. Berger & S. H. Chaffee (Eds.),
Handbook of communication science (pp. 564-618).
Fitzpatrick, M. A. (1988). Between husbands and wives: Communication in marriage.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Fitzpatrick, M. A., & Badzinski, D. M. (1985). All in the family: Interpersonal commu-
nication in Kin Relationships. In M. L. Miller &G. R. Knapp (Eds.), Handbook ofin-
terpersonal communication (pp. 687-736). London: Sage.
Fitzpatrick, M. A., & Ritchie, D. (1994). C ommunication schemata within the family:
Multiple perspectives on family interaction. Human Communication Research, 20,
275-301.
Guerrero, L. K., Eloy, S. V., & Wabnik, A. I. (1993) J ournal of Social and Personal Rela-
tionships, 10, 273-284.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The measures: Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Haas, S. M., & Stafford, L. (1997, November). Relationship maintenance behaviors in
same-sex and heterosexual couples: An exploratory comparison. Paper presented at
the annual conference of the National Communication Association, Chicago.
Haas, S. M., & Stafford, L. (1998). An initial examination of maintenance behaviors in
gay and lesbian relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15,
846-855.
Hample, D., & Dallinger, J. D. (2000, November). An investigation of taking confZict
personally and relational maintenance strategies for parents and their adult children.
Paper presented at the annual convention of the National Communication Associa-
tion; Seattle, WA.
Hatfield, E., Traupmann, J., Sprecher, S., Utne, M., & Hay, M. (1985). Equity in close
relationships. In W Ickes (Ed.), Compatible and incompatible relationships (pp.
9 l-l 7 1). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Kelley, H. H., & Thibaut, J. W. (1978). interpersonal relations: A theory of interdepen-
dence. New York: Wiley.
Lewis, R. A., & Spainer, G. (1979). Theorizing about the quality and the stability of
marriage. In W. Burr, R. Hill, F. I. Nye, & I. R. Reiss (Eds.), Contemporary theories
about the family (Vol. 1, pp. 268-294). New York: Free Press.
Messman, S. J., Canary, D. J., & Hause, K. S. (2000). Motives to remain platonic, eq-
uity, and the use of maintenance strategies in opposite-sex friendships. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 67-94.
Nix, C. (1999, November). But a “real” friend treat me that way?: An exami-
nation of negative strategies employed to maintain friendship. Paper presented at the
National Communication Association Convention; Chicago, IL.
Parks. M. (1982). Ideology in Interpersonal Communication: Off the couch and into
the world. In M. Burgoon (Eds.), Communication Yearbook 5 (pp. 79-108).New
Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books.
Perlman, D. (2001). Maintaining and enhancing relationships: Concluding commen-
tary. In J. Harvey &A. Wenzel (Eds.), Close romantic relationships: Preservation and
enhancement [pp. 357-378). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
3. MAINTAINING ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS =+-

Petronio, S. (1991). C ommunication boundary management: A theoretical model of


managing disclosure of private information between marital couples. Communica-
tion Theory, I, 31 l-335.
Prisbell, M. (1995). Strategies for maintaining relationships and self-rated competence
in ongoing relationships. Psychological Reports, 76, 63-64.
Ragsdale, J. D. (1996). G en d er, satisfaction level, and the use of relational maintenance
strategies in marriage. Communication Monographs, 63, 354-369.
Sabatelli, R. M. (1985). The Marital Comparison Index: A measure for assessing out-
comes relative to expectations. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 46,65 l-662.
Sprecher, S. (1986). The relation between equity and emotions in close relationships.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 49, 309-32 1.
Stafford, L. (1994). Tracing the threads of spider webs. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford
(Eds.), Communication and relational maintenance (pp. 297-306). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1991). Maintenance strategies and romantic relationship
type, gender, and relational characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 8, 217-242.
Stafford, L., Dainton, L., & Haas, S. M. (2000). Measuring routine and strategic rela-
tional maintenance: Scale revision, sex versus gender roles, and the prediction of re-
lational characteristics. Communication Monographs, 3, 306-323.
Stafford, L., Perry, B. J., Rankin, C., &Canary, J. (1999, November). Equity, relational
dependency, and perceptions of relational maintenance. Paper presented at the an-
nual conference of the National Communication Association, Chicago.
Vogel-Bauer, S., Kalbfleisch, I? J., & Beatty, M. J. (1999). P erceived equity, satisfaction,
and relational maintenance strategies in parent-adolescent dyads. Journal of Youth
and Adolescence, 28, 27-49.
Walster, E., Berscheid, E., & Walster, G. W. (1973). N ew directions in equity research.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2.5, 15 l-l 76.
Weigel, D. J., & Ballard-Reisch, D. S. (1999a). All marriages are not maintained equally:
Marital type, marital quality, and the use of maintenance behaviors. Personal Relu-
tionships, 6, 29 l-303.
Weigel, D. J., & Ballard-Reisch, D. S. (1999b). H ow couples maintain marriages: A
closer look at self and spouse influences upon the use of maintenance behaviors in
marriages. Family Relations, 48, 263-269.
Weigel, D. J., & Ballard-R eisch, D. S. (1999c). Using paired data to test models of rela-
tional maintenance and marital quality. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
26, 175-191.
This page intentionally left blank
Marianne Dainton
Elaine Zelley
La Salle University

Emily Langan
University of Texas, San Antonio

f the various relationships that people experience throughout their


lives, friendships remain the most prevalent type (Blieszner & Adams, 1992).
For example, a typical high-school student has four close friends (Weiss &
Lowenthal, 1975). Thi s number tends to increase in college, with college stu-
dents having relatively more friends than do young adults in the workforce. In
fact, a curvilinear relationship exists between age and the number of
friends identified in network, with the peak occurring during late ado-
lescence (Blieszner & Adams, 1992; Weiss & Lowenthal, 1975). Despite its
prevalence, however, friendship has received a secondary role in relationship
research, overshadowed by marriage and family relationships. Indeed, the ma-
jority of the existing literature on relationship maintenance has focused on
maintaining romantic or marital relationships (e.g., Canary & Stafford, 1992;
Dindia & Baxter, 1987) with only limited attention given to the strategies
used to preserve the voluntary, nonromantic bonds of friendship (e.g., Canary,
Stafford, Hause, & Wallace, 1993; Messman, Canary, & Hause, 2000).
80 -c=+ DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

This chapter highlights the means by which friendships are maintained.


First described is the nature and functions of friendship. Next, an overview
is provided of prominent theories used to understand the maintenance of
friendships before offering conclusions and areas of future research. Sub-
sequent sections focus on sex differences in enacting friendships, cross-sex
friendships, and variations across the lifespan. Finally, specific mainte-
nance strategies identified in the research are discussed.

THE NATURE AND l=UNCTIONS Ol= l=RlENDSHlP

Definitional uncertainty plagues much of the research on friendship. For


example, friendship has been defined as nonkin, Fehr (1996),
nonromantic, Blieszner and Adams (1992), or nonprimary attachments,
Weiss (1998). Hays suggested a succinct, although strategically liberal con-
ceptualization of friendship, saying that friendship is “voluntary interde-
pendence between two persons over time, that is intended to facilitate
social-emotional goals of the participants, and may involve varying types
and degrees of companionship, intimacy, affection, and mutual assistance”
(p. 395). This definition aptly summarizes friendships and encompasses a
variety of relational participants at various stages of life.
Indeed, the variety of friendship types is a central issue in the literature.
The four variables identified by Hays (1988)-companionship, intimacy, af-
fection, and assistance-differentiate among friendship types. Berger, Weber,
Munley, and Dixon (19 77), f or example, found distinctions between acquain-
tances, friends, and close friends based on the sociability and supportiveness
of the relationship. Rose and Serafica (1986) argued for similar distinctions;
they found that casual friends, close friends, and best friends varied in the ex-
tent to which the partners perceived the relationship as self-maintaining and
based on affection, as well as the extent to which a reduction in contact af-
fected these three relational types. Likewise, Wright (1984) suggested that
such distinctions may be a function of whether the friendship is based on ex-
change needs (i.e., maintained based on mutual rewards) or communal needs
(i.e., concern for the welfare; appreciation of the unique and
irreplaceable qualities). Casual friends and acquaintances probably function
more on an exchange basis, whereas close and best friends most likely function
more on a communal basis. In short, varying types of friendships exist, which
implies that likely distinctions occur in the ways that maintenance is achieved
in these differing relational forms.
Despite variations in friendship types, however, we find several areas of
commonality across friendships. Fehr (2000) argued that four factors must
exist for a friendship to emerge and develop. Specifically, friendship devel-
opment requires environmental factors (e.g., initial contact), individual
factors (e.g., attraction, skills), situational factors (e.g., anticipation of fu-
4. MAINTAINING FRIENDSHIPS ++i- 81

ture interaction), and dyadic factors (e.g., similarity and self-disclosure).


Further, Rawlins suggested that friendships are distinguished from other
relational types by their voluntary involvement; friends are chosen, not in-
herited or otherwise assigned. Unlike other relational types, friends often
reactivate the relationship after experiencing periods of dormancy
(Rawlins, 1994). Moreover, Wright (1998) argued, “friendship carries
with it much less requiredness and urgency than do involvements in more
clearly regulated personal and social relationships” (p. 57). That is, friend-
ship transcends social and legal constraints, allowing the partners more
freedom to cocreate a unique relationship. Because friendships cannot rely
on structural forces for survival, relational maintenance must be an active
enterprise, even if this means simply evoking memories of the friendship as
a means of maintaining the relationship during aforementioned latent peri-
ods (Rawlins, 1994). Without social boundaries and increased vulnerabil-
ity to dissolution, friends are responsible for defining, refining, and
maintaining the parameters of their relationship.
Finally, friendship boundaries are more fluid and defy discrete classifi-
cation. Friends frequently play overlapping roles. For example, Rawlins
(1994) distinguished between friendships for sake as com-
pared to friendships that complement other relationships, such as mar-
riage or work relationships. The focus of the present chapter concerns the
former, although researching the complex ways in which relationships are
maintained with partners who serve multiple roles in our lives should be
undertaken. Next, presented are four theoretical frameworks commonly
used in the study of friendship maintenance.

THtEORETICAL UNDlERPINNINGS

Although research of friendship is not as plentiful as research of other rela-


tionship types (e.g., marriage, dating, kin relationships), four theoretical
approaches to relationship maintenance have been applied successfully to
the study of friends. Accordingly, the following section of this chapter fo-
cuses on how equity theory, a relational dialectics perspective, a social
skills approach, and an attachment theory framework have been instru-
mental in guiding current knowledge of friendship maintenance.

Equity Theory as App le d to Friendship Maintenance

Perhaps the most common theoretical approach used to explain friendship


maintenance, equity theory posits that a balance of rewards and costs is
necessary to continue a close relationship over time (for reviews, see Ca-
nary & Zelley, 2000). Based on the principle of distributive justice,
Deutsch (1985) argued that individuals should receive relational rewards
82 -e=+ DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

equivalent to their own relational contributions. Accordingly, equitable re-


lationships are those in which both partners perceive that their ratios of
outcomes dived by inputs are equal. The ratio of outcomes to inputs can
then be used to predict relational satisfaction, as well as the degree to
which friendships will be favorably maintained. For example, when both
friends believe that they both get out of the relationship (outcomes) as
much as what they each put into the relationship (inputs), then the friend-
ship is termed equitable. Also, one person might make more efforts but re-
ceive greater rewards relative to his or her friend, in which case the
friendship remains equitable. However, if Friend A puts forth more effort
into the friendship than does Friend B, and receives the same outcomes
from the relationship, it can be said the Friend A is underbenefited because
the ratio of outcome/inputs exceeds Friend Continuing with this ex-
ample, Friend B is receiving more rewards but doing less as compared to
Friend A; as a result, Friend B is overbenefited because Friend out-
come/input ratio is less than Friend outcome/input ratio.
Predictably, relational partners become distressed when either type of
perceived inequity persists over time and report higher satisfaction when
involved in equitable relationships (Canary & Stafford, 1994). Equity
theory suggests, therefore, that not only must individuals be active in the
maintenance of their friendships, they are also more motivated to main-
tain equitable as opposed to inequitable relationships (Canary &
Stafford, 1994).
A number of studies have used this understanding of equity to explore
the maintenance of various types of friendships. In a study of motivations
for maintaining cross-sex, platonic friendships, for example, Messman et
al. (2000) noted that participants in equitable friendships reported a
greater number of positive and proactive maintenance behaviors when
compared with participants reporting on either overbenefited or
underbenefited friendships.
Coinciding with the marginalized nature of friendship in terms of re-
search, however, a large majority of friendship studies compare friendships
to other relationship types (e.g., romantic relationships). In their compari-
son of friends, family, and romantic relationships, for example, Canary et
al. (1993) identified 10 strategies used by partners. Analyses revealed that
friends reported significantly fewer uses of positivity, openness, and assur-
ances when compared with romantic partners. Friends also reported using
significantly fewer assurances; shared tasks; and (c) cards/letters/calls
when compared to respondents describing the maintenance of family rela-
tionships. Likewise, (1997) study of long-distance relationships
found that e-mail messages contained fewer assurances but more
references to joint activities than did romantic email messages.
Importantly, equity theory predicts friendship maintenance across the
lifespan. For example, Pataki, Shapiro, and Clark (1994) compared first
4. MAINTAINlNG FRIENDSHIPS +=$I- 83

and third graders and found that both groups divided shared tasks more
equally among friends than with acquaintances. Significantly, third-grade
friendship pairs were also more likely to distribute rewards equitably.
Buunk and Prins (1998) examined the effect of friendship inequity on
loneliness and found that college students in underbenefited and
overbenefited friendships felt significantly more lonely than participants
who reported providing and receiving equitable amounts of help. Equity
also appears salient in older and elderly adult friendships. For example,
Roberto and Scott (1986) f ound that individuals in equitable friendships
perceived significantly fewer trouble in their friendships regarding aspects
of helping, emotional impact, and the relationship overall. Clearly, re-
search demonstrates the importance of perceived equity in the mainte-
nance of friendship across the lifespan and in different types of friendships
(e.g., long distance, cross-sex).

A Dialectical Look at Friendship Maintenance


Dialectical theories provide the second major theoretical approach to un-
derstanding the maintenance of friendships. A dialectical approach pre-
supposes that change, opposing tendencies, and instability characterize all
social relationships (see Baxter & Montgomery, 1996; Montgomery, 1993;
Rawlins, 1989, 1994). U n 1-k1 e 1inear or causal models of relationship devel-
opment and maintenance (e.g., equity theory) wherein relational partners
are thought to seek a specified state of satisfaction or level of interaction, a
dialectical approach assumes that partners experience patterns of redun-
dancy while spiraling between opposing, yet interdependent tendencies
(Canary & Zelley, 2000). Th us, relational partners continuously realize and
react to conflicting yet interdependent influences; in brief, partners are
constantly being pulled together and simultaneously being pushed apart
(Montgomery, 1993).
Stated differently, a dialectical approach to relationship maintenance
indicates that mutually negating, opposing tendencies are abundant within
personal relationships such that relational partners seek autonomy and
connection, openness and closedness (Baxter, 1990). Specifically, Baxter
and Montgomery (1996, also see Rawlins, 1989) identified four key as-
sumptions of a relational dialectics perspective: (a) contradiction, (b)
change, (c) praxis, and (d) totality. Contradiction implies opposing, mutu-
ally negating, yet interdependent tendencies wherein one cannot experi-
ence one tendency, or pole, without understanding the presence of its
counterpart, for example, needing both expression and privacy in close re-
lationships (Baxter, 1990). With regard to change, because it is impossible
to address these interdependent yet mutually negating tendencies simulta-
neously, tension and change are always present. Praxis implies that individ-
8-t- +=s DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

uals both create and react to their social worlds; unlike passive bystanders,
intimates have choices and make decisions while also responding to part-
choices and decisions. Last, totality represents the understanding
that “social phenomena are defined by the relations among their character-
istics, not by the characteristics themselves” (Montgomery, 1993, p. 206).
Consequently, Montgomery argued that even the term relational mainte-
nance inaccurately portrays relationships as static entities; instead, Mont-
gomery and other dialecticians (e.g., Rawlins, 1994) observed that
relationships are sustained.
Extending a dialectical view of friendship, Rawlins (e.g., 1989, 1992,
1994) has investigated how friends sustain their relationships amid
ever-present and ever-changing tensions. Looking across stages of young
adult friendship in particular, Rawlins (1989) posited that two broad ana-
lytical classes of dialectic tensions exist: (a) contextual dialectics and (b)
interactional dialectics. First, Rawlins stated that two contextual dialectics
emerge based on how friendship is defined within American culture: Pub-
lic-private and Ideal-Real. Because public roles constrain friendships, ten-
sion arises between the private negotiation of a close, voluntary
relationship and the public display of the relationship within the confines
of social appropriateness (Rawlins, 1989). This “double agency” of friend-
ship is readily visible in cross-sex friendships where both friendship part-
ners are married to others (Rawlins, 1982, 1989). Although definition of
and closeness with the friendship is private, the friendship pair also must
operate within culturally bound social conventions (Rawlins, 1989).
Stated differently, some may view adult cross-sex friendships with suspi-
cion, whereas no suspicion would likely arise if the same “friendship” were
presented as a relationship between colleagues. The Ideal-Real dialectic
refers to the discrepancies between what is desired from a friend and the
daily realities of sustaining a voluntary relationship that must compete
with more formal social ties. With regard to interactional dialectics,
Rawlins (1989) offered f our specific tensions thought to characterize
young friendships: Independence-Dependence, Affection-Instru-
mentality, Judgment-Acceptance, and Expressiveness-Protectiveness
(Rawlins, 1989, 1992).
Baxter et al. (1997) identified similar tensions in young adult friend-
ships within the context of a Loyalty-Disloyalty dialectic. The two most
frequently reported tensions with regard to conflicting loyalties within
friendships were general time dilemmas and specific time demands. More
specifically, perceived obligations (general) as well as previously made
commitments (specific) appeared to constrain respondents, thereby creat-
ing a dilemma between wanting to be independent and feeling obligated to
spend time with friends.
In a similar vein, Rawlins (1994) a1so examined friendship dialectics
within middle adulthood (30 to 40 years old). One central dialectic that
4. MAINTAINING FRIENDSHIPS +=a- 8,5

encompasses both the Independence-Dependence and the Public-Pri-


vate tensions emphasized in young adult friendships is the notion of “be-
ing there” and “growing apart.” Specifically, descriptions of
their middle-adult friendship experiences revealed that “friendship dis-
continuities are shaped by other social circumstances, like marital and
family status, occupation, emerging interests, and friendship circles” (p.
285). Accordingly, middle-adult friendships appear to be strained by the
desire to have friends who are there for each other while faced with life
demands that prevent such closeness.
(1989, 1994) and (e.g., Baxter et al., 1997; Wise-
man, 1986) research gives some insight into how friendship mainte-
nance is neither steady nor static. Instead, a dialectical view of
friendship suggests that numerous interdependent contradictions tug at
partners throughout the lifespan. However, dialectics in
friendships have not been investigated. Instead, most dialectical re-
search involves young and middle adults.

A social skills A ppr-oath to Maintaining Friendship

Turning to the third major theoretical approach to friendships, Burleson


and Samter (1994) proposed a social skills similarity model to understand
friendship maintenance. They argued that possessing the same communi-
cation skill level as friend has a greater impact on relational satisfac-
tion than does the absolute level of skill sophistication. Stated
differently, it is not the sophistication level of communication skills itself
that predicts relationship satisfaction; rather, it appears more important
that two friends demonstrate similar degrees of skill (or lack of skill). Un-
like an equity approach to maintenance, a social skills perspective focuses
on nonstrategic routines whereby “relationship maintenance occurs when-
ever persons enact behaviors that service the particular tasks or functions
defining a particular relationship” (Burleson & Samter, 1994, p. 66). Fur-
thermore, because individuals vary in their skillfulness in handling relation-
ship tasks, the relationships themselves will be more or less maintained.
Using close friendships as an example, Burleson and Samter (1994) held
that a primary function of friendship is to give and receive social support.
According to the skills similarity model, friendships would be maintained
based on similar skill levels of support seeking and receiving. In other
words, similarity of social skills should influence the extent to which the
friendship is rewarding. Because relationships develop and change over
time, expectations also change and thereby differentiate close friends from
acquaintance friends (Burleson & Samter, 1994). Burleson and Samter
posited that only when both partners fulfill the expectations of a particular
friendship does the friendship remain intact for further development.
86 -c== DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

Empirical studies have demonstrated support for this model; however,


a meager amount of research has investigated the social skills similarity
model and friendships. For example, Burleson and Samter (1996) found
that among college students, not only were friendship pairs similar with
regard to their skillfulness with affective expression and the management
of emotional states but also, pairs of highly skilled friends were no more
satisfied with the relationship than were friendship pairs in which both
partners were equally unskilled. Burleson (1994) reported similar results
in a study of third graders, noting that children were more attracted to
peers with similar social skills as their own, and that pairs of friends
showed similar skills in the expression and management of emotional
states. Likewise, Burleson and Samter (1994) found that college coeds
“were both attracted to and maintained reciprocated friendships with
those having levels of social skills similar to their own” (p. 83). More pre-
cisely, Burleson and Samter found significant effects due to similarity in
comforting and conflict management. The skills similarity approach to
relational maintenance has also found support within marital relation-
ships (e.g., Burleson & Denton, 1992).

/utachmentThecx-ynd rriendshp
A final theoretical perspective is attachment theory. Initially framed
within the infant-caregiver relationship (Bowlby, 1973, 1982), attach-
ment theory (AT) posits that the bonds developed between a child and the
primary caregiver provide a context from which all other close relation-
ships must be understood (Collins & Read, 1990). Specifically, AT predicts
that sense of security depends on the initial infant-caregiver bond
whereby security and stimulus reduction are provided through consistent,
comforting responses from the caregiver (attachment figure) during an in-
times of need or distress, particularly as the infant develops and ex-
plores (Armstrong & Roth, 1989).
The degree to which young children recognize and react to (in)consistent
comfort from their attachment figure creates the foundation for under-
standing three primary attachment styles: secure, anxious or ambivalent,
and avoidant (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Bowlby, 1973,
1982). Whereas secure infants have confidence in their attachment
ability to comfort or soothe based on the consistent support
when needed, avoidant children have had limited exposure to comfort from
the caregiver when distressed; subsequently, avoidant children display little
desire to achieve closeness and do not seek comfort. In between these two
styles, anxious or ambivalent infants perceive their caregivers to be unreli-
able sources of support and, therefore, are easily upset, are difficult to
soothe, and demonstrate high degrees of separation anxiety.
4. MAINTAINING FRIENDSHIPS ++ 8;

Somewhat differently, Bartholomew and Horowitz (199 1) developed a


model to define attachment styles based on view of self versus
view of others. In their model, four distinct styles emerge: secure (positive
view of self and others), preoccupied (negative view of self; positive view of
others), dismissive (positive view of self; negative view of others), and
fearful avoidant (negative view of self and others) attachments. Despite
classification differences, both views of attachment styles suggest that the
initial bonding has a long-lasting influence on the atti-
tudes and behaviors of individuals within close relationships, including
friendships. For example, AT has also been used to explain adult relation-
ships in terms of relationship attitudes, self-disclosure, and openness (e.g.,
Collins & Read, 1990); intimacy (Guerrero, 1996); conflict styles (Levy &
Davis, 1988; Pistole, 1989); competitiveness (Zelley, 2001); and social
support (Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992).
Although attachment clearly applies to the infant-caregiver relation-
ship, research has investigated the influence of attachment style on other
types of close relationships, including friendships. Children need exten-
sive social and emotional skills to sustain friendships, and attach-
ment to a parent or primary caregiver influences initiation and
maintenance of friendships (Shulman, Elicker, & Sroufe,
1994; Wright, 1984).
For example, observational analysis has revealed that attachment style
differences surfaced along two primary dimensions that appear to influ-
ence the maintenance of friendship: affective tone and de-
gree of closeness between friendship pairs, and nature of tasks friends
jointly pursue (Shulman et al., 1994). Importantly, secure children ap-
peared to display more positive affect as well as engaged in more joint, cre-
ative play than those with insecure attachment styles. Mikulincer and
(2001) th ree-part study of attachment style and affiliation
among best friendships revealed that anxious or ambivalent
participants overemphasized attachment goals (e.g., security and manage-
ment of distress), whereas avoidant participants minimized attachment
goals. Further, Mikulincer and Selinger noted that anxious-ambivalent re-
spondents reported greater difficulty in maintaining the friendship than ei-
ther secure or avoidant participants. Among preschool children, Kerns
(1994) found that attachment pairing significantly predicts friendship
quality with secure-secure pairs having more positive and coordinated in-
teractions than secure-insecure friendship pairs.
Attachment style, at least to some degree, influences the development
and maintenance of childhood and young adult friendships. AT has not
been used widely to address individual differences in adult friendships,
perhaps because attachment is largely a developmental theory, and, as dis-
cussed earlier friendship is not as heavily researched as romantic and kin
relationship. However, a number of findings have shown attachment styles
58 +=s DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

influence adult relationships such as marriage (e.g., Davila & Bradbury,


2001). It is quite likely that attachment style differences influence the
maintenance of middle and older adult friendships.
Having reviewed theoretical viewpoints common to friendship re-
search, attention is now turned to the ways in which individuals maintain
their friendships over time and distance.

MAINTENANCE STKAT~GIES

Various definitions of maintenance imply alternative understandings of


how friendships are sustained. For example, Dindia and Canary (1993) ar-
gued that one definition of maintenance is to keep a relationship in exis-
tence. This definition of maintenance as relationship existence
corresponds with both (1988) and (1984) conceptualiza-
tions of maintenance in their studies of friendship. Other researchers focus
on maintenance as keeping the relationship in a specified state or condition
(e.g., Fehr, 1999; Messman et al., 2000). Finally, some scholars have fo-
cused on maintaining satisfactory friendships (e.g., Burleson & Samter,
1996; Cole & Bradac, 1996; Jones, 199 1).
Despite variations in how maintenance is explicitly conceived, a tenta-
tive consensus exists regarding how relational maintenance might be
achieved in friendships. Thus far, four strategies have consistently
emerged as in the friendship maintenance literature: time together, open-
ness, social support, and avoidance.
First, several scholars have highlighted the importance of time to-
gether as a maintenance strategy (e.g., Canary et al., 1993; Fehr, 2000;
Hays, 1984; Johnson, 2000; Messman et al., 2000; Rawlins, 1992; Rose,
1985). This research has emphasized the need for shared activity and on-
going interaction in order to sustain the relationship. Indeed, absence of
interaction is consistently cited as a basis for termination in friendships
(Hays, 1988; Rawlins, 1994; Rose, 1985). Interestingly, Rawlins found
that middle-adult friendships could be sustained without frequent con-
tact as long as friends were perceived as “being there” or “still there” for
each other. However, he concluded that visitation and interpersonal con-
tact afford the greatest chances for sustaining friendship, which suggests
that spending time together serves as a central means by which friend-
ships are maintained.
A second consistent maintenance strategy identified in the literature is
openness (e.g., Ayres, 1983; Canary et al., 1993; Fehr, 2000; Hays, 1984;
Johnson, 2000; Messman et al., 2000; Rose, 1985). In short, self-disclosure
and discussions about life events seem central to the maintenance of friend-
ships, and the use of openness as a maintenance strategy is important for ca-
sual, close, and best friendships. Indeed, Hays (1984) found that openness
predicted friendship intensity 3 months later for all types of friends.
4. MAINTAINING FRIENDSHIPS =+I- 89

Although it is important for friends to talk about their own lives, re-
search also suggests that the provision of social support (i.e., being
other-oriented) offers a central means by which friendships are maintained
(Barbee, Gulley, & C unningham, 1990; Burleson & Samter, 1994; Canary
et al., 1993; Fehr, 2000; Hays, 1984; Messman et al., 2000; Nardi &
Sherrod, 1994). Scholars have found that comforting, giving advice, and
providing ego support are primary functions of friendships.
The final strategy identified consistently in the literature is avoidance
(e.g., Ayres, 1983; Canary et al., 1993; Messman et al., 2000; Nix, 1999).
Although this strategy may be perceived as a less than ideal means to main-
tain relationships, research supports that avoiding particular topics or peo-
ple might help to sustain relationships. Such a perspective is consistent
with a dialectical perspective on maintenance (discussed previously),
where time together might be balanced by time apart, and openness bal-
anced by closedness (see Baxter, 1994).
Although these four strategies appear consistently throughout the litera-
ture, several other strategies have emerged that might also function to sus-
tain friendships. For example, several studies have noted that antisocial
strategies can be used to maintain friendships (e.g., Canary et al., 1993; Nix,
1999), whereas others have explicitly identified affection as a maintenance
strategy (e.g., Hays, 1984; Rose, 1985). 0 ne explanation for the inconsis-
tent reporting of this latter strategy could stem from the assumption that
friendships are affectionate simply by definition (Hays, 1988); therefore, it
might appear redundant to list this as a maintenance strategy. Additionally,
the inconsistent reporting of both strategies in the literature might be be-
cause such strategies are used less frequently to maintain friendships. Con-
versely, social desirability effects might explain why these strategies appear
less frequently. Individuals responding to surveys might be unwilling to ac-
knowledge their use of antisocial strategies with someone they presumably
hold dear. On the other end of the spectrum, friends might not be willing to
acknowledge the role of overt affection in their relationships due to social
norms against displays of affection for same-sex relationships (Fehr, 1996).
Several other maintenance strategies should be noted. Burleson and
Samter (1994) argued that conflict management was a vital social skill in
the maintenance of friendships. Canary et al. (1993) found that friends re-
ported using humor, sharing tasks, social networks, assurances, positivity,
and cards/letter/calls. In partial support for these categories, Messman et
al. (2000) a1so f ound the reported use of positivity as a means for maintain-
ing cross-sex friendships, and Johnson (2000) found the use of telephone
calls and e-mail as a means for maintaining long-distance friendships. Fu-
ture research should strive to ascertain the extent to which these addi-
tional strategies might be used to maintain friendships, as well as the
extent to which such strategies foster desired relational characteristics
such as satisfaction and commitment.
‘-?o +e DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

SEX Dlt=l=lXENCES IN ENACTING l=KIENDSHlP

One frequently studied topic in the friendship literature is sex differences


in the enactment of friendships. Despite popular stereotypes of large dif-
ferences in the ways that men and women maintain relationships, research
results are far from consistent. This section highlights some of the research
into sex differences and offers conclusions regarding what is known and
where it might go from here.
First, a number of studies have suggested that female friendships are
emotionally closer than male friendships. Buhrke and Fuqua (1987) found
that female friendships have more contact under stress, are closer, and are
more satisfactory to the friends than are male friendships. Similarly, Hays
(1988; Hays & Oxley, 1986) h as f ound that female friends are perceived as
more supportive than are male friends. Indeed, both female and male
friends reported being less lonely when they spend time with female
friends (Wheeler, Reis, & Nezlek, 1983). In short, friendships that involve
at least one woman were more satisfying than male-male friendships
(Elkins & Peterson, 1993), primarily because of the provision of support
and the openness associated with them.
Regarding the maintenance of friendships, Johnson (1999) did not con-
duct a direct comparison, but she found that female-female student
friendships were more likely to describe maintenance as being achieved by
phone calls and discussion of life events and romantic interests, whereas
male-male student friendships described discussion of romantic interests,
talking about sports, and going to a bar or drinking as the most frequent
ways of maintaining their relationships. In summarizing sex differences in
friendships, Fehr (1996) argued that female friends prefer talking, and
male friends prefer engaging in activities. This was partially supported by
(1999) research. Fehr (1996) also suggested that when women
talk they discuss personal and relational matters, whereas men talk about
impersonal matters such as sports and work, which is also partially consis-
tent with the results of (1999) study.
Conversely, however, Barbee et al. (1990) found that both male and fe-
male friends preferred talking to a same-sex friend than an opposite sex
friend, diminishing the proposed superiority of female friendship. Further,
Derlega, Barbee, and Winstead (1994) f ound that male friends were more
often sought as a source of support for achievement-related stressors.
Finally, Jones (1991) f ound that although male friends self-disclosed less
than do female friends, self-disclosure remained a significant indicator of
male friendship satisfaction.
Complicating these inconsistencies, several studies have found no sig-
nificant differences in the maintenance of friendships (Ayres, 1983; Cole
& Bradac, 1996; Rose & Serafica, 1986). Further, marital status likely in-
4. MAINTAINING FRIENDSHIPS -+- 91

teracts with sex when looking at friendship maintenance. For example,


Tschann (1988) f ound that married men disclose less than do unmarried
men, married women, and unmarried women. In short, the picture of sex
differences in the maintenance of friendships is opaque at best.
Methodological differences may contribute to this muddled view. Derlega
et al. (1994) proposed that sex differences might be a function of the
self-report methods used to study friendships. In a series of laboratory studies,
these authors found few sex differences in friendship enactment. Accordingly,
it may be that people respond in stereotypical ways when responding to a sur-
vey but might not exhibit stereotypical behavior when observed.
Moreover, although some sex differences might exist in the mainte-
nance of friendships, similarities likely outweigh any differences (Wright,
1998). Wright also proposed that views of what constitutes affec-
tionate and communal exchanges might be skewed toward feminine ideals
(e.g., a weekend fishing trip can also be seen as communal). Accordingly,
sex differences in such activities might not be experienced as such by rela-
tional participants. Finally, as with other relational types, gender probably
plays a larger role in predicting maintenance enactment than does sex (e.g.,
Stafford, Dainton, & Haas, 2000). That is, gender (i.e., masculine,
feminine, or androgynous) might influence friendship behavior more than
does biological sex (i.e., male or female).

CROSS-SEX l=RlENDSHIP.S

Although sex differences per se may not have a clear impact on the ways
that relationships are maintained, clear differences emerge in the enact-
ment of same-sex and cross-sex friendships. For heterosexual individuals,
maintaining a cross-sex friendship involves the affection, companionship,
intimacy, and assistance found in same-sex relationships, but it also in-
volves downgrading sexuality (Monsour, 1992, 1996). Indeed, one of the
fundamental challenges of cross-sex friendships is confronting the expec-
tation that such relationships should ultimately lead to romantic or sexual
relationships (Monsour, 1996). Yet, friendships themselves are typically
exemplified by a lack of sexual intimacy (Gaines et al., 1998). Such is the
paradox of cross-sex friendships.
Werking (1997) argued that opposite-sex friendships (as compared to ro-
mantic involvements) can be characterized in four ways: They involve attrac-
tion of the spirit, not the body; they are more egalitarian than romantic
relationships; they do not entail exclusivity, as do romantic relationships; and
they are an end in themselves, not a means to an end. Buhrke and Fuquo
(1987) found that both men and women have an average of three close oppo-
site-sex friends, although, single women and married men and women tend to
prefer same-sex friendships to opposite-sex friendships (Rose, 1985).
92 -is=+ DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

Women and men appear to perceive cross-sex friendships differently.


For example, Rose (I 985) f ound that women perceive cross-sex friend-
ships as providing less acceptance than do men in cross-sex friendships.
Men, on the other hand, perceive cross-sex friendships as closer than do
their female friends (Buhrke & Fuqua, 1987). Jones (199 1) found that al-
though women reported greater trust in their male friends, a more com-
munal orientation, and greater satisfaction, such differences were not
apparent when considering the actual functioning of cross-sex friendships.
Finally, regarding sex differences in the communication in cross-sex friend-
ships, Egland, Spitzberg, and Zormeier (1996) found that women more of-
ten reported flirting in cross-sex friendships than did men. Interestingly,
the avoidance of flirting was identified by Messman et al. (2000) as one
means for maintaining platonic cross-sex friendships.
When comparing the composition of the dyad (i.e., same-sex and
cross-sex friends), some clear differences emerge. Rose (1985) reported
that individuals in opposite-sex relationships more frequently reported
few, if any, maintenance efforts and the use of affection as maintenance
than did individuals in same-sex friendships. She found that same-sex
friendships used rnore acceptance, effort, communication, and common
interests as maintenance activities compared to cross-sex friend-
ships. Conversely, Winstead, Derlega, Lewis, Sanchez-Hucles, and Clarke
(1992) found that emotion-based talk occurred most frequently in
cross-sex friendships and least frequently in female-female friendships,
with the frequency of male-male emotion-based talk falling in between
that of the other two friendship compositions. This clearly is against ste-
reotypes of friendships.
Despite these differences, Monsour (1992) found similarity in the
meaning of intimacy for cross-sex and same-sex friends, although (predict-
ably) sexual contact was viewed as appropriate for cross-sex friends and
--L -- --- ----f:- - 1- l?-
not same-sex rrienus. ror _----example,
-- -l- -lLl--
alrnougn.-l- e-l-L:mm-lmm
relarlvely --.--
uncommon --- - I?-
ror -l- Dow
-Ll

sexes, several scholars have found that men are somewhat more motivated
by sexual attraction in their pursuit of cross-sex friendships than are
women (Buss, 1994; Rose, 1985).
Indeed, it appears that one of the major challenges in cross-sex friend-
ships is dealing with sexuality. Some cross-sex friendships fail this challenge,
as both men and women report having had sex with cross-sex, ostensibly pla-
tonic friends (Buhrke & Fuqua, 1987). Much of the research on cross-sex
friendship suggests that sexual activity between opposite sex friends is a rel-
atively infrequent occurrence, however (Messman et al., 2000). For exam-
ple, according to Fuiman, Yarab, and Sensibaugh (1997), only one in seven
respondents noted having engaged in sexual activity with a friend of the op-
posite sex. On the other hand, Afifi and Faulkner (2000) found that one half
of participants (5 1%) had h ad sex with at least one platonic friend, and one
third (34%) of those who had engaged in sex reported having had sex with a
4. MAINTAINING FRIENDSHIPS 3t 93

friend on more than one occasion. The difference in these proportions is


likely due to measurement; Afifi and Faulkner asked participants to report
on the frequency of sexual activity with all cross-sex friendships, whereas
other researchers typically ask participants to report on the frequency of sex
with only one cross-sex friend. Moreover, the varying definitions of platonic
friends could influence the reports of sexual behavior between friends. For
example, Messman et al. (2000) defined platonic as “non-sexual involve-
ment” (p. 73) thereby eliminating any cross-sex friendships in which there
had been any physical involvement. Conversely, Afifi and Faulkner (2000)
defined platonic friendship as a cross-sex relationship in which the partici-
pants were not dating and “had no intentions of dating” (p. 2 11). This more
liberal definition may explain the large proportion of cross-sex friends who
had reportedly engaged in sex with a friend on at least one occasion.
A discussion of the role of sexual activity in cross-sex friendships relies
on the presumption that the friends are heterosexual. Nardi and Sherrod
(1994) argued that when focusing on the friendships of gay men and les-
bian women, same-sex friendships are also challenged by the role of sexual
activity. Yet, friendships in general are likely to play an even greater role in
the lives of gay men and lesbians than in heterosexual individuals, as friend-
ships often substitute for family in the gay and lesbian population. Nardi
and Sherrod found that gay men were more likely than lesbian women to
have had sex with casual and close friends, but not with best friends. Les-
bians were more likely to say their best friend was a former lover, whereas
gay men were more likely to say their best friend was their current lover
(for more about the maintenance of gay and lesbian relationships, see
Haas, chap. 10, this volume).
In sum, it appears as though sexual activity does play at least a modest
role in adult friendships for both heterosexual and homosexual individuals.
However, it is important to emphasize that a vast majority of heterosexual
cross-sex friendships lack sexual intimacy. Further, and discussed next,
friendship composition preferences and appropriateness change through-
out the lifespan; such changes in preference as well as social appropriate-
ness may influence the degree to which sexuality is or is not emphasized
between friends.

l=KllZNDSHlP ACROSS THE LIl=J5PAN

How friendships are enacted changes over the lifespan (e.g., Matthews,
1986; Patterson et al., 1993; Rawlins, 1992). If the nature of friendships
changes as people age then it is important to highlight how the means to
maintain friendships might also vary over time. The next section highlights
variations in how friendships are sustained in four periods: childhood, ado-
lescence and young adulthood, adulthood, and older adulthood.
‘74 -ee DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

childhood
It is inappropriate to collapse all childhood friendships into one category
because friendships themselves vary a great deal throughout the lifespan.
Still, as a whole, children view friends as playmates, whereas individuals in
other developmental stages view friends as confidants (Burleson & Samter,
1994). In the following paragraphs several views are presented that distin-
guish friendship during childhood from friendship occurring in other de-
velopmental stages.
For instance, Burleson (1994) o b served that children count as friends
virtually all people with whom they come in frequent contact, engage in
mutual activities, and share material resources such as toys. In other
words, children view friendship as consistent with repeated contact and
mutual interest in playthings and pastimes. Similarly, Gottman and
Mettetal(l986) f ound that early childhood peer relations focus on coordi-
nated interaction. Even more precisely, Rawlins (1992) categorized the
features of childhood friendship into three groupings based on age range
and behavior. For example, he grouped the friendships of 3 to 7 year olds as
momentary physical playmates, whereas the friendships of 6 to 9 year olds
are characterized by activity and opportunity. The friendships of 8 to 12
year olds are characterized by equality and reciprocity. These distinctions
coincide with the literature reviewed previously (e.g., Burleson, 1994;
Gottman & Mettetal, 1986).
In each of the above descriptions, young friendships appear as
rather simple and lacking in sophistication. In fact, Rawlins (1992) sug-
gested that friendships often end over quarrels and negative ex-
changes because young children lack the ability either to repair the damage
or to imagine the friendship enduring beyond the conflict. Conversely,
other researchers have suggested that friendships are more highly
developed than they appear. For example, Whaley and Rubenstein (1994)
argued that toddlers are quite capable of complex and committed friend-
ships and that these young children worked at sustaining their relation-
ships through rituals and routines. Similarly, Howes, Droege, and
Matheson (1994) found that children in long-term friendships demon-
strated more efficient communication. The children in Howes et study
extended and clarified each behaviors; as a result, they had little
need for negotiation or conflict.
As children age, affective tone and degree of closeness between the chil-
dren, as well as the nature of the tasks that they pursue suggests friendship
closeness (Shulman, Elicker, & Sroufe, 1994). Moreover, younger children
prefer same-sex partners, but by eighth-grade cross-sex preferences
emerge, with boys preferred for telling jokes and stories, and girls pre-
ferred for giving advice and lifting spirits (Clark, 1994).
4. MAINTAINING FRIENDSHIPS =+ 95

Much of the research into friendships follows strong theoreti-


cal components. Although these theories were discussed earlier, some of
the results of this research warrant additional discussion. First, Shulman et
al. (1994) f ound that preadolescents with a secure attachment style are
more competent at establishing and maintaining close friendships,
whereas preadolescents with anxious attachment styles had difficulty
making and keeping friends. Second, using a cognitive complexity ap-
proach, Burleson (1986) f ound that children lacking appropriate social
skills are more likely to be rejected by their peers than were children who
are more skilled. More importantly, Burleson (1994) found that similarity
in social skills was a better predictor of friendship than were objective lev-
els of such skills.

Ad&scence and voungAdulthood


The transition from childhood to adulthood brings with it a shift in notions
of friendship. Tesch (1983) suggested that friendships during these years
are defined by intimate disclosures and expectations for support. Like-
wise, Rawlins (1992) posited that mutuality and understanding constitute
the sine qua non of adolescent and young adult friendships. He argued that
this is a period of articulating identity and learning to develop inti-
macy, and friends provide the major resource for accomplishing these
goals. In fact, adolescence and young adulthood represents a time of high
contact with friends (Verbrugge, 1983).
Much of the research on friendship maintenance has focused on adoles-
cence and young adulthood. Not surprisingly, the four main maintenance
strategies described earlier in this chapter (time together, openness, sup-
port, and avoidance) clearly align with maintaining the types of friendships
most often experienced by individuals in this age group. That is, because
young people experience high expectations for intimate communication,
social support, and shared experiences (Rawlins, 1992), it is not a surprise
that time together, openness, support, and even avoidance have been iden-
tified in the literature. All of these strategies can assist with the develop-
mental goals of this period.
Young adult friendships encounter challenges, however. Rawlins
(1992) noted that this timeframe is associated with competition be-
tween friends, as well as competition between friends and their romantic
partners. For example, Nix (1999) f ocused on the ways that individuals
maintain friendships when the other friend is involved with a romantic
partner. He found that romantic involvements challenge existing friend-
ships, and that most friends actively seek to maintain the friendship bond
despite this challenge.
93 -e- DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

The young adult timeframe is also associated with the complexities of


transitioning to and away from college (Paul & Kelleher, 1995; Rawlins,
1992). Because of such transitions, Johnson (1999, 2000) has initiated a
series of studies focusing on the maintenance of long-distance friendships.
She has found that, like romantic relationships, long-distance friendships
(LDFs) and geographically close friendships (GCFs) did not vary in the ex-
tent to which they reported closeness, satisfaction, or the expectation that
the relationship would continue (for a discussion of long-distance romantic
relationships, see Aylor, chap. 6, this volume). However, she did find varia-
tions in the types of routine and strategic maintenance behaviors that indi-
viduals utilized in LDFs and GCFs.
Finally, Burleson and Samter (1996) focused on the maintenance of
young adult friendships. They found that similarity in comforting and con-
flict management skills was important in friendships among young adults,
whereas similarity in ego support and persuasion were less important.

Adulthood
The friendships of middle adulthood, defined by Rawlins (1992) as matu-
rity to middle age (30 to 6.5 years old), are less frequently studied than
those of the other age groups. However, several key areas of concern asso-
ciated with relational maintenance can be identified. First, Rawlins (1992)
argued that, for this age group, friendships can most clearly be distin-
guished as either communal (i.e., emotionally supportive) or agentic (e.g.,
socially facilitative, activity oriented). Many adult friendships are those of
convenience, meaning that friendships are the byproducts of
other roles such as co-workers, neighbors, or kin. This reliance on agentic
friendships throughout middle adulthood most likely results due to the
time constraints adults face in the wake of family and work pressures.
Maintaining friendships becomes particularly problematic during adult-
hood as dating and marriage result in withdrawal from the friendship net-
work (Milardo, Johnson, & Huston, 1983). Indeed, unmarried friends rely
on time together as a maintenance strategy to a much greater extent than
do married friends (Rose, 1985). On the other hand, married friends re-
port greater use of affection as a maintenance strategy than do single
friends (Rose & Serafica, 1986). Ironically, divorce also compounds the
difficulties of maintaining friendships (Rawlins, 1992). Divorced individu-
als frequently feel isolated from the friendship networks they maintained
when they were married. Given focus on romantic pair bonding
during adulthood, adult friends have lower expectations regarding the
need to spend time together to maintain their friendships (Rawlins, 1994).
In terms of managing problematic events, adult friends argue over nu-
merous issues including contrasting ideas, inappropriate disclosures, indi-
vidual freedoms, rule violations, third parties, and time management
4. MAINTAINING FRIENDSHIPS =a- 97

(Berger, Shaffer, Freeman-Witthoft, & Freund, 1998; Blieszner & Adams,


1992; Rawlins, 1992; Rose, 1985). Conversely, Tesch (1983) indicated
that similarity, power, and acceptance become less important in adult
friendships than they were previously.

older Adulthood
Researchers more frequently study friendship in older adulthood, most
notably because of the presumed health benefits associated with friend-
ship. Patterson et al. (1993) f ound that friendships between elderly people
are most often characterized by devotion, reciprocity, closeness, under-
standing, shared experience, and attraction. These authors argued that el-
derly people have more complex views of friendship than do younger
people, as older adults focus both on reciprocity and the consequences of
friendship loss. Moreover, given a lessening of time constraints, as well as
the opportunities that retirement communities and nursing homes pro-
vide, older adulthood may offer a time where friendships are more active
and numerous (Matthews, 1986). Of course, these opportunities are also
tempered by the more frequent deaths of those in friendship circle
(Matthews, 1986). Finally, and relative to younger cohorts, older adults re-
port fewer cross-sex friendships, and their friends also tend to be similar in
terms of age (Matthews, 1986).
Older friendships fall into three categories (Matthews, 1986;
Rawlins, 1992). First, independent friendship types are agentic; that is,
they are activity oriented and socially facilitative. Those with discerning
friendships are characterized by deep attachments to the friend. Finally,
acquisitive types include friends from the past, as well as new relation-
ships. Importantly, Matthews (1986) f ound that elderly people may main-
tain established friendships differently than new friendships. Specifically,
the maintenance of established relationships involved more self-disclosure
and the provision of more services.

CONCLUSIONS

The maintenance of friendship is complicated by several factors. First, few


social or legal constraints affect the enactment of friendships, making it an
amorphous relational type that is likely to be a unique construction for the
relational partners. Second, there are numerous levels and types of friend-
ships. Just as variations exist in the ways that casual daters, serious daters,
and married couples maintain their relationships (e.g., Canary & Stafford,
1994), variations likely occur in the ways that casual friends, close friends,
and best friends maintain their friendships. Third, whether sex differences
exist in friendship enactment or not, biological sex and gender influence
,Q8 -!+a DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

the friendship process, as same-sex friendships confront different chal-


lenges and tensions than do cross-sex friendships. Finally, the meaning and
enactment of friendship changes over the lifespan, and our understanding
of friendship maintenance must take these changes into account.
Numerous areas for future research abound. First, scholars should co-
alesce and collapse existing maintenance typologies into a clear typology
that can be applied with some consistency. The four consistent strategies
described in this chapter, for example, are probably not exhaustive. Once a
clear typology is created, perhaps research should turn to variations in the
use of these maintenance strategies across friendship levels and over the
life span. Finally, the four theories described in this chapter provide a pre-
liminary means for understanding maintenance processes. Nevertheless,
more programmatic research using these theories will likely yield more fo-
cused and clear answers regarding the various simple and complex ways
that friendships are maintained.

Adams, R. G., & Blieszner, R. (1994). An integrative conceptual framework for friend-
ship research. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 163-l 84.
Afifi, W. A., & Faulkner, S. L. (2000). On being “just friends”: The frequency and im-
pact of sexual activity in cross-sex friendships. Journal of Social and Personal Rela-
tionships, 17, 205-222.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attach-
ment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Armstrong, J. G., & Roth, D. M. (1989). Attachment and separation difficulties in eat-
ing disorders: A preliminary investigation. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
8, 141-155.
Ayres, J. (1983). Strategies to maintain relationships: Their identification and per-
ceived usage. Communication Quarterly, 3 1, 62-67.
Barbee, A. P, Gulley, M. R., & Cunningham, M. R. (1990). Support seeking in close re-
lationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationship, 7, 531-540.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A
test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61,
226-244.
Baxter, L. A. (1990). Dialectical contradictions in relationship development. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 69-88.
Baxter, L. A., Mazanec, M., Nicholson, J., Pittman, G., Smith, K., & West, L. (1997).
Everyday loyalties and betrayals in personal relationships. Journal of Social and Per-
sonal Relationships, 14, 655-678.
Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (1996). Relating: Dialogues and dialectics. New
York: Guildford.
Berger, C. R., Weber, M. D., Munley, M. E., & Dixon, J. T. (1977). Interpersonal rela-
tionship levels and interpersonal attraction. In B. Rubin (Ed.), Communication year-
book 1 (pp. 245-262). N ew Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books.
Berger, H. A., Shaffer, L. S., Freeman-Witthoft, B., & Freund, H. A. (1998). Friends
and lovers. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 623-636.
4. MAINTAINING FRIENDSHIPS 3t, y?

Blieszner, R., & Adams, R. G. (1992). Adult f riendship. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss, Vol. I : Attachment (2nd ed.). New York: Basic
Books. (Original work published 1969)
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss, Vol. 2: Separation. New York: Basic Books.
Buhrke, R. A., & Fuqua, D. (1987). S ex d i ff erences in same- and cross-sex supportive
relationships. Sex Roles, 17, 339-352.
Burleson, B. R. (1994). F riendship and similarities in social-cognitive and communica-
tion abilities: Social skill bases of interpersonal attraction in childhood. Personal Re-
lationships, 1, 371-389.
Burleson, B. R., & Denton, W H. (1992). A new look at similarity and attraction in mar-
riage: Similarities in social-cognitive and communication skills as predictors of at-
traction and satisfaction. Communication Monographs, 59, 268-287.
Burleson, B. R., & Samter, W. (1994). A social skills approach to relationship mainte-
nance: How individual differences in communication skills affect the achievement
of relationship functions. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and
relational maintenance (pp. 61-90). New York: Academic Press.
Burleson, B. R., & Samter, W. (1996). S imilarity in the communication skills of young
adults: Foundations of attraction, friendship, and relationship satisfaction. Commu-
nication Reports, 9, 127-l 39.
Buss, D. M. (1994). Th e evolution of desire. New York: Basic Books.
Buunk, B. & Prins, K. S. (1998). L oneliness, exchange orientation, and reciprocity in
friendships. Personal Relationships, 5, 1-14.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1992). Relational maintenance strategies and equity in
marriage. Communication Monographs, 59, 243-267.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1994). Strategic and routine interaction. In D. J. Canary &
L. Stafford (Eds.), C ommunication and relational maintenance (pp. 3-22). San
Diego, CA: Academic.
Canary, D. J., Stafford, L., Hause, K. S., &Wallace, L. A. (1993). An inductive analysis
of relational maintenance strategies: Comparisons among lovers, relatives, friends,
and others. Communication Research Reports, 10, 5-14.
Canary, D. J., & Zelley, E. D. (2000). C urrent research programs on relational mainte-
nance behaviors. Communication Yearbook, 23, 30.5339.
Clark, R. A. (1994). 1 and gender preferences for conversational
partners for specific communicative objectives. Journal of Social and Personal Rela-
tionships, 11, 313-319.
Cole, T., & Bradac, J. J. (1996). A lay theory of relational satisfaction with best friends.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 13, 57-83.
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and relation-
ship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58,
644-663.
Davila, J., & Bradbury, T. N. (2001). Attachment insecurity and the distinction be-
tween unhappy spouses who do and do not divorce. Journal of Family Psychology,
15, 371-393.
Derlega, V J ., Barbee, A. I?, & Winstead, B. A. (1994). Friendship, gender, and social
support: Laboratory studies of supportive interactions. In B. R. Burleson, T. L.
Albrecht, & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Communication of social support: Messages, inter-
actions, relationships, and community (pp. 136-l 72). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Deutsch, M. (1985). Distributive justice: A social-psychological perspective. New Ha-
ven, CT Yale University Press.
Dindia, K., & Baxter, L. (1987). Strategies for maintaining and repairing marital rela-
tionships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 4, 143-l 58.
loo s=+ DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

Dindia, K., & Canary, D. J. (1993). Definitions and theoretical perspectives on main-
taining relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, IO, 163-l 73.
Egland, K. L., Spitzberg, B. H., & Zormeier, M. M. (1996). Flirtation and conversa-
tional competence in cross-sex platonic and romantic relationships. Communication
Reports, 9, 105-l 17.
Elkins, L. E., & Peterson, C. (1993). Gender differences in best friendships. Sex Roles,
29, 497-508.
Fehr, B. (1996). Friendship processes. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Fehr, B. (1999). Stability and commitment in friendships. In J. M. Adams & W. H.
Jones (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal commitment and stability (pp. 259-280).
New York: Academic/Plenum Press.
Fehr, B. (2000). The life cycle of friendship. In C. Hendrick & S. S. Hendrick (Eds.),
Close relationships: A sourcebook (pp. 7 l-82). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Fuiman, M., Yarab, P, & Sensibaugh, C. (1997, July). Justfriends?An examination ofsex-
ual, physical, and romantic aspects of cross-gender friendships. Paper presented at the
annual meeting of the International Network on Personal Relationships, Oxford, OH.
Gaines, S. O., Bledsoe, K. L., Farris, K. R., Henderson, M. C., Kurland, G. J., Lara, J. K.,
Marelich, W. D., Page, M. S., Palucki, L. J., Steers, W N., & West, A. M. (1998).
Communication of emotion in friendships. In P A. Andersen & L. K. Guerrero
(Eds.), Hundb oo k of communication and emotion: Research, theory, applications,
and contexts (pp. 5 1 O-53 1). New York: Academic.
Gottman, J., & Mettetal, G. (1986). Sp eculations about social and affective develop-
ment: Friendship and acquaintanceship through adolescence. In J. M. Gottman& J.
G. Parker (Eds.), Conversations of friends (pp. 192-237). New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Guerrero, L. K. (1996). Attachment-style differences in intimacy and involvement: A
test of the four-category model. Communication Monographs, 63, 269-292
Hays, R. B. (1984). Th e d eve 1o p ment and maintenance of friendship. Journal of Social
and Personal Relationships, 1, 75-98.
Hays, R. B. (1985). A longitudinal study of friendship development. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 48, 909-924.
Hays, R. B. (1988). Friendships. In S. W. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relation-
ships: Theory, research, and interventions (pp. 391-408). Chichester: Wiley.
Hays, R. B., & Oxley, D. (1986). S ocial network development and functioning during a
life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 305-3 13.
Howes, C., Droege, K., & Matheson, C. C. (1994). Pl ay an d communicative processes
within long- and short-term friendship dyads. Journal of Social and Personal Rela-
tionships, 1 I, 401-410.
Johnson, A. J. (1999, November). Using routing and strategic communication and ac-
tivities to maintain friendships: Examining geographically close and long-distance
friendships. Paper presented at the National Communication Association Confer-
ence; Chicago, IL.
Johnson, A. J. (2000, July). A role theory approach to examining the maintenance of geo-
graphically close and long-distance friendships. Paper presented at the International
Network on Personal Relationships Conference; Prescott, Arizona.
Jones, D. C. (1991). Friendship satisfaction and gender: An examination of sex differ-
ences in contributors to friendship satisfaction. Journal of SociaZ and Persona2 Rela-
tionships, 8, 167-l 86.
Kerns, K. A. (1994). A longitudinal examination of links between mother-child attach-
ment and friendships in early childhood. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 11, 379-381.
4. MAINTAINING FRIENDSHIPS -a- 101

Levy, M. B., & Davis, K. E. (1988). L ovestyles and attachment styles compared: Their
relations to each other and to various relationship characteristics. Journul ofSocial ?X
Personal Relationships, 5, 439-471.
Matthews, S. (1986). Friendships through the life course. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Messman, S. J., Canary, D. J., & Hause, K. S. (2000). Motives to remain platonic, eq-
uity, and the use of maintenance strategies in opposite-sex friendships. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 67-94.
Mikulincer, M., & Selinger, M. (2001). Th e interplay between attachment and affilia-
tion systems in same-sex friendships: The role of attachment style.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 18, 81-l 06.
Milardo, R. M., Johnson, M. I?, & Huston, T. L. (1983). Developing close relationships:
Changing patterns of interaction between pair members and social networks. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 964-976.
Monsour, M. (1992). Meanings of intimacy in cross- and same-sex friendships. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 9, 2 77-295.
Monsour, M. (1996). C ommunication and cross-sex friendships across the life-cycle: A
review of the literature. In B. Burleson (Ed.), Communication Yearbook 20 (pp.
375-414). Th ousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Montgomery B. M. (1993). Relationship maintenance versus relationship change: A di-
alectical dilemma. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 205-223.
Nardi. P M., & Sherrod, D. (1994). Friendships in the lives of gay men and lesbians.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 185-200.
Nix, C. L. (1999, November). But a “real” friend treat me that way?An exami-
nation of negative strategies employed to maintain friendship. Paper presented at the
National Communication Association Convention; Chicago, IL.
Pataki, S. P, Shapiro, C., & Clark, M. S. (1994). Ch i Id acquisition of appropriate
norms for friendships and acquaintances. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 11, 427-442.
Patterson, B. R., Bettini, L., & Nussbaum, J. F. (1993). The meaning of friendship
across the life-span: Two studies. Communication Quarterly, 41, 145-160.
Paul, E. L., & Kelleher, M. (1995). Precollege concerns about losing and making friends
in college: Implications for friendship satisfaction and self-esteem during the college
transition. Journal of College Student Development, 36, 5 13-52 1.
Pistole, M. C. (1989). Attachment in adult romantic relationships: Style of conflict res-
olution and relationship satisfaction. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6,
505-510.
Rabby, M. K. (1997, November). Maintaining relationships via electronic mail. Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association; Chi-
cago, IL.
Rawlins, W K. (1982). Cross-sex friendship and the communicative management of
sex-role expectations. Communication Quarterly, 30, 343-352.
Rawlins, W. K. (1989). A dialectical analysis of the tensions, functions and strategic
challenges of communication in young adult friendships. In J. A. Anderson (Ed.),
Communication yearbook, 12 (pp. 157-l 89). Newbury, CA: Sage.
Rawlins, W. K. (1992). F riendship matters: Communication, dialectics, and the life-
course. Hawthorne, NY Aldine de Gruyter.
Rawlins, W K. (1994). Being there and growing apart: Sustaining friendships during
adulthood. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relutionul
maintenance (pp. 275-294). New York: Academic Press.
Roberto, K. A., & Scott, J. l? (1986). Equity considerations in the friendships of older
adults. Journal of Gerontology, 41, 241-247.
102 w DAINTON, ZELLEY, LANGAN

Rose, S. M. (1985). S ame- and cross-sex friendships and the psychology of


homosociality. Sex Roles, 12, 63-74.
Rose, S. M., & Serafica, F. C. (1986). Keeping and ending casual, close, and best friend-
ships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 3, 275-288.
Shulman, S., Elicker, J., & Sroufe, L. A. (1994). Stages of friendship growth in
preadolscence as related to attachment history. Journal of SociaZ and Personal Rela-
tionships, 1 I, 341-361.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support-seeking and sup-
port-giving within couples in an anxiety-provoking situation. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 62, 434-446.
Stafford, L., Dainton, M., & Haas, S. (2000). M easuring routine and strategic relational
maintenance: Scale development, sex versus gender roles, and the prediction of re-
lational characteristics. Communication Monographs, 67, 306-323.
Tesch, S. A. (1983). R eview of friendship development across the lifespan. Human De-
velopment, 26, 266-276.
Tschann, J. M. (1988). Self-disclosure in adult friendship: Gender and marital status
differences. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 5, 65-81.
Verbrugge, L. M. (1983). A research note on adult friendship contact: A dyadic per-
spective. Social Forces, 62, 78-83.
Weiss, R. S. (1998). A taxonomy of relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Rela-
tionships, 15, 67 l-683.
Weiss, L., & Lowenthal, M. F. (1975). 1 e - course perspectives on friendship. In M. F.
Lowenthal, M. Thurnher, & D. Chiriboga (Eds.), Four stages of life. San Francisco:
Jossey Bass.
Werking, K. J. (1997). just good friends: Women and men in nonromantic relation-
ships. New York: Guilford Press.
Whaley, K. L., & Rubenstein, T. S. (1994). H ow toddlers friendship: A descriptive
analysis of naturally occurring friendships in a group child care setting. Journal of So-
cial and Personal Relationships, 11, 383-400.
Wheeler, L., Reis, H., & Nezlek, J. (1983). L oneliness, social interaction, and sex roles.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 943-953.
Winstead, B. A., Derlega, V J., Lewis, R. J., Sanchez-Hucles, J., & Clarke, E. (1992).
Friendship, social interaction, and coping with stress. Communication Research, 19,
193-211.
Wiseman, J. I? (1986). F riendship: Bonds and binds in a voluntary relationship. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 3, 191-211.
Wright, P H. (1984). S e If- re f erent motivation and the intrinsic quality of friendship.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 1, 115-l 30.
Wright, P H. (1998). Towards an expanded orientation to the study of sex differences
in friendship. In D. J. Canary & K. Dindia (Eds.), Sex differences and similarities in
communication: Critical essays and empirical investigations of sex and gender in in-
teraction (pp. 41-64). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Zelley, E. D. (2001, July). Friend or foe: The relationship between competitiveness in
close friendships and the occurrence of disordered eating. Paper presented at the In-
ternational Conference on Personal Relationships, Prescott, AZ.
ndes‘ire
hIps

Jon A. Hess
LTriversity of Missouri-Columbia

i s social creatures, we spend our lives in the company of others, rather


than in isolation. Consequently, we maintain many relationshps out of need
rather than desire. Unfortunately, some of these relationshps are ones that we
would not maintain if given a choice. Although a considerable amount of re-
search on relational dynamics can be applied to unwanted relationshps, schol-
ars have made little attempt to generate an integrated overview of what
communication characteristics typify such relationshps, how they differ
from desirable relationshps, or how they should best be maintained.
The maintenance of unwanted relationshps piques public interest. Articles
with titles such as You Bug Me! (Precker, 2000) and Do YouAttract People You’d
Rather Repel? (Finello, 2000) that are scattered throughout the pages of news-
papers and magazines, and books such as Dealing With People You Can’t Stand
(Blvlkman & Kirschner, 1994), serve as a testament to the attraction such rela-
tionshps have on people’s attention. But unwanted relationshps should catch
scholars’attention as well because a closer examination of these relationships
could broaden and enrich our understandmg of personal relationshps. Relation-
shps people want to maintain pose challenges (e.g., managing dalectical ten-
sions or dealing with confict), but greater challenges can arise in relationshps
that one or both parties wish &d not exist. It seems llkely that at both an indi-
vidual and societal level, more problems arise from relationships people would
not maintain if given a choice than from relationships that people choose to nur-
ture. The widely documented tensions in Ireland, the Middle East, and the for-
mer Yugoslavia may illustrate some problems that result from social groups
being unwillingly forced to coexist. At an interpersonal level, individuals face
undesirable relationships on a regular basis and often experience negative conse-
quences from them (Hess, 2000; Levitt, Silver, & France, 1996).
Research on unwanted relationships and their challenges offers an opportu-
nity for theoretical advances in the study of personal relationships. Unwanted
relationships provide a rich context for the study of many communication
challenges, and they offer a venue assessing the generalizability of theory. At
present, some theories of relational phenomena apply only to voluntary and
desired relationships (Wiseman & Duck, 1995). Studying unwanted relation-
ships can help scholars learn more about communication under difficult cir-
cumstances and can help scholars discern which principles of relational
maintenance are universal and which are context-specific. Duck (1994b) ar-
gued that “the and sides of relationship need to be incor-
porated together theoretically into one set of principles that can deal with
both” (p. 4). Doing so entails testing theories in a wide range of relational con-
texts (Wood & Duck, 1995), especially those that differ in significant ways
from the more traditional contexts studied by researchers.
This chapter provides a foundation from which to study such relation-
ships. A diverse set of constructs and theories are pulled together to help il-
luminate the characteristics that differentiate undesired relationships from
their more desirable counterparts. This chapter examines the assumptions
that underlie the study of undesired relationships, delineates the factors that
give rise to such relationships, discusses the nature of communication pro-
cesses in such relationships, and suggests directions for future research.

ASSUMPTIONS l=OR STUDYING UNDESIRED

The study of undesired relationships is founded on a set of assumptions


that may differ from ones scholars often make when studying maintenance
of more traditional relationships. These assumptions are as follows.

Assumption I: Relationships d-ten Exist


as Nonbohntary Associakons

Few scholars would deny that some relationships are nonvoluntary, but the
majority of relational communication theory focuses on relationships
formed by voluntary association (Galvin & Cooper, 1990). Family scholars
(e.g., Coleman SKGanong, 1995; Galvin & Cooper, 1990) often discuss the
impact that nonvoluntary association has on families, but by and large, the
5. MAINTAINING UNDESIRED RELATIONSHIPS 3t- 102

relational maintenance literature focuses on what forces can hold relation-


ships together or tear them apart, how relationships develop, or how they
deteriorate, rather than on how people sustain a relationship when separa-
tion is not an option. If scholars approach the study of relational mainte-
nance from an assumption that relationships are often nonvoluntary
associations, then a broader range of relationships must be studied so that
the theory developed can be applied to all relationships.

Assumption~:closeand Ongoin Relationships


canSometimesbeCharacterized % y NegativeAffect

Many scholars suggest that liking is an essential quality of close relation-


ships (e.g., Bell, Daly, & Gonzalez, 1987; Byrne & Murnen, 1988; Dickens
& Perlman, 1981; Rubin, 1973). This stipulation is unwarranted. Un-
doubtedly, the majority of close relationships are affectively posi-
tive, as are the relationships that people most highly value, so the
characterization of close relationships as involving liking is often appropri-
ate. However, the assertions that liking constitutes a necessary condition
for a close relationship or that all close relationships are affectively positive
inaccurately represent the social milieu of most lives. As
Berscheid (1983) noted:

It is clear that strong negative affect experienced more or less regularly, perhaps
even exclusively, in a relationship many would consider as close on other grounds
is not unusual. At the least, a classification scheme that excluded such relation-
ships from the domain of close relationships would exclude many family rela-
tionships. (p. 115)

In attempt to delineate the factors that make relationships close, Kelley


et al. (1983) f ocused on causal interdependence rather than liking. In their
definition, relationships are close when they have frequent, strong, di-
verse, and enduring causal interconnections. Although some of these au-
thors later questioned the necessity of duration in this definition
(Berscheid, Snyder, & Omoto, 1989), they stated explicitly that affect was
irrelevant to the definition of closeness.

Assumption 3: Relational Development and Maintenance


Sometrmes Involve Fluctuatingor Even Decking
Levels of Intimacy
Many theories of personal relationships have stated that relational devel-
opment and dissolution are characterized by increases or decreases in inti-
macy level (e.g., Altman & Taylor, 1973). These theories typically saw
relationships as continuously in a process of growth, and thus, gradually in-
creasing in intimacy, unless they were left to stagnate or deteriorate. Ayres
(1983) suggested that instead, relationships develop to a certain level of in-
timacy and then enter a maintenance phase of stable intimacy levels. The
common assumption among all these theories is that intimacy increases or
stabilizes during relational development and maintenance, and that a re-
duction in intimacy signals relational deterioration.
More recent perspectives (e.g., Baxter & Montgomery, 1996) propose
that many relational qualities are dialectical in nature, and thus they vary
over time as relational partners attempt to satisfy competing tensions be-
tween opposing forces. For example, the needs for autonomy and interde-
pendence may drive partners to increase or decrease intimacy at different
points of their relational lives. Thus, intimacy may go through periods of
increase and decrease during the maintenance phase of a long-term rela-
tionship. Research on relationships with disliked partners suggests that
people often try to minimize intimacy throughout the course of an ongoing
relationship (Hess, 2000).
Although existing evidence suggests that most healthy and desired per-
sonal relationships do indeed experience steady or increasing levels of inti-
macy throughout their development and maintenance, theory and
research also show that some relationships may be characterized by part-
attempts to minimize or reduce intimacy as one way of maintaining
the relationship (Hess, 2000). Such a trend might seem like evidence of re-
lational decline, but reduction of intimacy as a coping mechanism for an
undesired relationship may be seen as a way of reducing conflict and thus,
preventing relational dissolution.

Assumption+:U-wanted Relationships
Can be HeaIthy Relationships

A substantial amount of research suggests that unpleasant or undesired re-


lationships have detrimental effects on people. For example, unpleasant
relationships at work and school have been linked to workplace cynicism,
decreased work effectiveness, and decreased psychosomatic well-being
(Fritz & Omdahl, 1998; Kinney, 1998; Schwartz & Stone, 1993). Is this
negative impact inevitable? Unwanted relationships will probably never be
pleasant, but it seems realistic to believe that researchers can identify the
causes of negative impacts and provide ways to minimize their effects so
that some of these relationships can be maintained without such unhealthy
consequences. Duck (1987) observed that:

For something like 10,000 years, people have been warring with each other,
fighting other nations, sparring with their neighbors, hating their colleagues,
quarreling with their loved ones, arguing with one another, and suffering the
pangs of despised love without the benefit of scientific research into relation-
ships and their problems. (p. 278)
5. MAINTAINING UNDESIRED RELATIONSHIPS 3t, 107

The study of unwanted relationships is one area where research has the
potential for significantly improving the quality of human life. One pur-
pose of this chapter is to suggest research directions that might help people
learn how to make undesired relationships healthy relationships.

CONCIZPTUAL FXAMIEWORK

The investigation of undesired relationships must begin by answering two


fundamental questions: What conditions cause a relationships to be un-
wanted?; Why do people maintain unwanted relationships? These ques-
tions define the context in which the undesired relationship exists.
Understanding the conditions that create unwanted relationships allows us
to better understand their internal forces, because these relationships de-
velop within the constraints defined by those conditions.

why Certain ReIationships Are Perceived asunwanted

Relationships can be unwanted for rational and/or emotional reasons. The


rational reasons can be described as interference with personal goals, and
the emotional reasons share the common factor of negative affect.

Goal Interference. The rational side of human behavior is governed


by logical thought processes. The purpose of cognition is to for-
mulate alternative choices for behavior and to select among those options
(Greene, 1984). Scholars characterize the rational thought process as be-
ing goal-driven in nature, noting that our rational choices are made to
achieve certain goals (e.g., Berger, 1997; Bogdan, 1994). These goals en-
compass a wide range of objectives. Task-related goals, such as getting a job
done, come to mind easily, but virtually all other reasoned and intentional
human behavior can be described in terms of goals. For example, social be-
haviors such as maintaining a certain identity, interacting in socially appro-
priate ways, maintaining or increasing valued resources, and regulating
arousal are all goal-driven processes (Dillard, Segrin, & Harden, 1989).
Thus, any relationship that poses an ongoing obstruction to the accom-
plishment of these goals can become unwanted. Relational partners who
disconfirm a desired identity, cause anxiety, or deplete a desired
resources may be unwanted. Sometimes this goal interference is brought
about because of mutually conflicting goals between two people.
The perception of a relationship as unwanted emerges from a
goal-directed perspective as follows. Goals are hierarchically organized
(Berger, 1997), meaning that some goals supersede others. Overtly avoid-
ing another person or terminating a relationship goes against social eti-
quette and may have negative consequences for people. In lieu of reason to
108 -ii=+ HESS

eliminate social ties with someone, people are likely to interact with that
person when social norms make such behavior expected. However, when
maintaining a relationship interferes with higher-order goals, such as ac-
complishing a task or presenting a certain face, the relationship becomes
undesired. For example, a student who was talking in class about undesired
relationships reported an incident with a friend who needed temporary
housing, but became a nuisance after moving in. When this lifestyle
began interfering with the plans, the relationship became unwanted.
Another student mentioned a work relationship that was undesired be-
cause the co-worker interfered with the objectives she was trying to ac-
complish (task goals). Other people have spoken of relationships that were
unwanted because friends and family did not approve (social interaction
goals) or because they were publicly embarrassed by the other be-
haviors (impression management goals).
For goal interference to make a relationship unwanted, the interference
must have a lasting effect over time. Goals are not always consistent, and
they can change suddenly from one time to another (Berger, 2000). If a re-
lationship interferes with a goal on one or two occasions, then it is more
likely to be an interaction that is undesired rather than the relationship it-
self. For instance, a person may wish to avoid talking to a close friend when
he or she has pressing deadlines, but still value the relationship. More en-
during objectives must be obstructed for the relationship to be undesired
on the basis of goals.

Negative Affect. It would be a mistake to describe behavior


only on the basis of rational thought (i.e., choices based on goal assess-
ments) . One of the hallmarks of human behavior is that people often base
actions on emotional impulses, behaving in ways that defy any sane reason.
This tendency can cause unwanted relationships. Relationships that are
neutral or even beneficial with respect to goal success may be unwanted
because of negative affect. Fritz and (Fritz, 1997; Fritz &
Omdahl, 1998) research on negative coworkers provides a good example.
Despite the importance of coordinating work for task effectiveness, many
people report relationships in the workplace that they would prefer not to
maintain. While this chapter was being written, a department at a univer-
sity received a large donation from a wealthy alumnus to endow a program
that would host business executives for annual seminars. However, when
the donor visited the department he was so offensive that the faculty
hoped he would not return. Despite the goal-related benefits (funding a
program to improve education), the negative affect he aroused
meant that people did not want to have a personal relationship with him.
Although disliking may result in seemingly irrational behavior, the de-
sire for dissociation in such circumstances makes rational sense. Theories
of cognitive consistency (e.g., Heider, 1958; Newcomb, 1968) state that
5. MAINTAINING UNDESIRED RELATIONSHIPS +=a- 109

people prefer that their perceptions fit together harmoniously. For rela-
tionships, two perceptions are relevant: affect and relational association
(Heider, 1958). Wh en affect is negative, people prefer a lack of relational
association. Thus, continued maintenance of the relationship is seen as un-
desirable.
Negative affect can arise from a variety of sources. Wiseman and Duck
(1995) reported that when asked to describe friends and enemies, people
typically reported endearing qualities of friends (e.g., loyal, caring) and
malicious actions by enemies (e.g., inflicted emotional pain, lied to oth-
ers). When discussing the subject of relationships with disliked partners,
students often talk about disliking others because of incompatible per-
sonalities, antisocial behavior, or heinous actions by the other, such as be-
ing judgmental, pushy, or harassing. Once people develop an enduring
dislike for another person, relational interaction with that person be-
comes unwanted.

why undesired Relationships Are Maintained


If people would prefer not to associate with certain others, why do they
continue to maintain these relationships? It is because these relationships
are seen as nonvoluntary associations (Hess, 2000). Many scholars (e.g.,
Levinger, 1965, 1976; Rusbult, 1987; Thibaut & Kelley, 1959) have sug-
gested that relationships are held together by barriers that prevent them
from coming apart. This explanation makes good sense-the forces tearing
the relationship apart are overcome by forces holding it together. The
forces that act as barriers to relational dissolution can be classified into two
broad categories, external and internal.

External Barriers. External barriers are forces that originate outside


the individual and make the person feel constrained to that relationship.
These forces of connection can come from at least three sources: social
ties, work ties, and proxemic ties. Social ties refer to elements of social life
that bind people together, such as friendships, family relations, and mar-
riages. In a review of external barriers that hold marriages together,
Attridge (1994) cited financial burdens of divorce (e.g., lack of economic
self-sufficiency), difficulty in disentangling networks of mutual friends,
and legal ties that must be severed as forces that can hold a marriage to-
gether when it might otherwise have broken apart.
In addition to these social barriers, people may maintain relationships be-
cause of their work. The desirability of the present job or the difficulty of find-
ing a new one may make it worthwhile for a person to endure an unwanted
relationship. Athletic teammates can face this situation acutely because the
two may work together very closely and there might be no opportunity for a
person to be traded, especially in high school or college athletics.
110 + HESS

Finally, people are often constrained to relationships by physical prox-


imity. Whether it is due to residential area (e.g., residing in a small town) or
living arrangements (e.g., family member, roommate), people can be
forced into relationship just by the inevitability of encountering each
other. A student once talked about how she maintained an undesired rela-
tionship throughout high school because she lived in a small town and
could not avoid the other person. She was happy when she could end the
relationship by moving away from home for college.
Undesired relationships caused by external ties cause a collision of
psychological and social forces. Internally, the person may prefer not to
have the relationship, but external pressures force the interaction. Such a
situation is bound to be stressful, as research has demonstrated (Hess,
2000). Ultimately, though, these situations can often be tolerable if han-
dled in a constructive manner. Despite the conflict between the desire
not to relate and the externally generated need to do so, these situations
are ultimately resolved through the rational prioritization of goals. Re-
gardless of whether the relationship is unwanted because of goal interfer-
ence or negative affect, people in these circumstances choose to
subordinate their disdain for maintaining the relationship to their desire
to satisfy more important objectives or social needs. Those needs may
range from providing for dependents to presenting a socially desirable
face or treating people according to certain moral standards, but in all
cases the external barriers are constraints only because other goals over-
ride the desire to terminate the relationship. Recalling (2000)
point that goals are hierarchically organized, it can be said that what hap-
pens in cases of external constraints is that the goal of ending the relation-
ship is subordinated to some higher-level goal.
At face, the discussion of goal subordination calls into question whether
any but a few atypical relationships (e.g., people who have been institu-
tionalized) are truly unwanted. After all, if people choose to maintain these
relationships because of higher-order goals then the relationship seems to
be at least partially desired. However, if the term unwanted relationship
were restricted to relationships that were undesired to the degree that
ending the relationship overrode all other considerations, then the term
would encompass so few relationships that it would be practically useless.
The term unwanted relationship is used in this chapter to describe a rela-
tionship that a person would choose to discontinue if nothing extraneous
to the relationship were taken into account.

internal Barriers. In contrast with external barriers, these forces


arise from within the individual. In these cases, people experience conflict
with their own desires. Attridge (1994) identified factors such as
self-identity goals, religious beliefs, and sense of commitment. As with the
external ties, these forces hold a relationship together because relational
5. MAINTAINING UNDESIRED RELATIONSHIPS 31, 111

satisfaction is subordinated to goals that are perceived as being more im-


portant.
Some internal barriers function in a different way. These barriers pri-
marily center on safety and security, fear of making changes, or a lack of
faith in the ability to leave the relationship. For example, one student
talking about such a situation discussed how she sustained a relationship
because it was difficult for her to deviate from the history of closeness
she had with the person. Another recalled maintaining a relationship with
a mutual friend whom she disliked. In attempting to explain why she con-
tinued in this relationship, she could only say that she did not know why
she did it. It was an unidentified fear of ending the relationship that pro-
pelled ongoing interaction. In other cases, fear of making changes or de-
sire not to hurt the other led to relationships that were unhealthy for the
individual who found the relationship undesirable. In these cases, peo-
reasons for maintaining undesired relationships seem less rational
and sometimes even dysfunctional. Relationships maintained under such
circumstances might have little chance of being healthy for the individual
who sees it as undesirable.

IMPLICATIONS l=OR THEORY AND RESlEARCH

“P ecial characteristics of undesiredtdationships


Unwanted relationships are characterized by the goal conflict or negative
affect that makes them undesirable and the barriers that keep them to-
gether. As a result, these relationships cause discomfort to those who find
the relationship undesirable, whether that is only one person or both part-
ners. Because undesired relationships are sustained by forces counteract-
ing the pressures that would otherwise tear the relationship apart, they
exist in the battleground of opposing forces. That tension creates an emo-
tionally-strenuous situation. Although any relationship may be a source of
discomfort from time to time, undesired relationships cause discomfort
throughout their entire existence (e.g., Hess, 2000).
Undesired relationships are also characterized by a number of commu-
nicative behaviors that seem to set them apart from other relationships.
Most notable among these behaviors is a greater tendency to create dis-
tance with relational messages (Hess, 2000). Because these relationships,
by virtue of their existence, are closer relationships than people want, they
are characterized by attempts to make themselves more distant
from the unwanted partner. This characteristic and other communication
behaviors that seem to differ from those in more desired relationships are
discussed later when specific communication characteristics of unwanted
relationships are addressed.
112 -I+=+ HESS

One assumption made in this chapter is that a relationship need not be


unhealthy (or dysfunctional) just because it is unwanted. A relationship
is dysfunctional when its interactions have harmful effects on its mem-
bers. These harmful effects can include psychological trauma, physio-
logical symptoms of stress, or physical injury from abuse (Gottman,
1994; Kinney, 1998; West, 1995). 0 ne worthwhile objective in the
study of undesired relationships is to address the question of what fac-
tors cause dysfunctions and what can be done to make such relation-
ships healthier.
The conceptual framework proposed in this chapter suggests one factor
that may be finked to relational dysfunction is the creation of an undesired
relationship due to self-contradictory internal barriers (e.g., fear of making
changes, a lack of faith in the ability to leave the relationship, low
self-esteem, etc.). These barriers represent self-supplied impulses to sus-
tain the relationship that contradict the self-supplied desire to escape from
it. This set of contradictory beliefs seems likely to result in a high rate of
dysfunctional relationships because self-contradiction is a common factor
associated with psychological pathologies (Krippendorff, 1989;
Watzlawick, Beavin, & Jackson, 1967). The account of one survivor of an
abusive relationship typifies this situation. She recalled, “by the time the
whole thing ended, I just felt like a rag. I feel attractive at all.. . . I felt
totally worthless. How could I possibly get out of this marriage, I was
worthless. How could I possibly have any kind of life outside of him now?”
(Lempert, 1997, p. 156).
When external barriers create an undesired relationship, the situa-
tion is out of the control, at least in the present and immedi-
ate future (actors may plan long-term strategies to change the situation
and eliminate the undesired relationship). At face, that contrast sug-
gests that relationships that are unwanted due solely to external barriers
might be less likely to be unhealthy than those maintained because of in-
ternal barriers. However, research on abusive relationships shows that
both internal barriers (e.g., feelings of commitment) and external barri-
ers (e.g., economic dependence, lack of child care) play a role in
decisions to stay in abusive relationships (e.g., Rusbult &
Martz, 1995; Strube & Barbour, 1983). So, the question of whether cer-
tain types of barriers more strongly predispose a relationship to be un-
healthy is unanswered at present. This question is worth addressing
with future research, because if certain types of barriers can be identi-
fied as leading to more or less healthy outcomes, then scholars can begin
to form a set of risk factors for negative outcomes from unwanted rela-
tionships. In addition, researchers may also wish to examine what per-
5. MAINTAINING UNDESIRED RELATIONSHIPS +=+I- 113

sonality traits or interactive behaviors predict health-related outcomes


from unwanted relationships. It seems likely that a combination of all
three factors will predict the healthiness or unhealthiness of these rela-
tionships. For instance, Thomsen and Gilbert (1998) found that
neuroticism was associated with negative marital outcomes (e.g., satis-
faction), but also that a combination of neuroticism (a personality trait)
and dominance (an interactive behavior) “explained more variance in
marital dissatisfaction than did either factor separately” (p. 85 1).

s e&c Communicative Processes in the Maintenance


o P ti ndesired Relations hips

Coping. Research applicable to unwanted relationships suggests that


at least two behaviors should be universal in this context. The first of these
is coping. Unwanted relationships cause stress, and stress demands some
form of coping by the individual. Coping is “a stabilizing factor that can
help individuals maintain psychosocial adaptation during stressful periods;
it encompasses cognitive and behavioral efforts to reduce or eliminate
stressful conditions and associated emotional distress” (Holahan, Moos, &
Schaefer, 1996, p. 25). W iseman and (1995) study of enemies
showed that people coped by shaping perceptions in ego-protective ways,
which helped reduce stress and cognitive dissonance. For instance, they re-
ported that most people saw enmity as unilateral-they were innocent,
and the malice was solely due to the actions and intentions. Wise-
man and Duck also noted that people were more likely to focus their en-
ergy on maintaining their own self-esteem than on reducing the enmity. In
many cases, people responded with self-pity and other forms of
nonproductive reflection on the situation.
Another method of coping people may use in unwanted relationships is
drawing support from social networks (Pierce, Sarason, & Sarason, 1996),
For example, talking with others is a common way people cope with enemy
relations (Wiseman & Duck, 1995). Several consequences of this strategy
are noteworthy. First, such communication can serve as a catalyst to im-
prove matters or it may actually aggravate the problem. Talking with others
about an enemy might provide a more neutral perspective or ideas for rec-
onciliation, but it can also strengthen a convictions about mis-
treatment. As Wiseman and Duck noted, talking with others “may cement
enmity by making it impossible to ‘talk out (p. 70). Second,
utilization of social support can cause the impact of an undesired relation-
ship to spill out into other parts of a social network. Involving others in the
matter may change their relations with those parties, and may even create
challenging situations when the involved third parties must interact with
the recipient of the disinterest.
114 -c=+ HESS

Distance. The other behavior that seems to be universal in undesired


relationships is effort to distance oneself from the unwanted partner. Dis-
tance can be seen as a coping behavior, because people use it to reduce
stress (e.g., Hess, 2000). H owever, it is addressed separately from coping,
because distancing can result from causes other than stress.
People seeking greater separation reported many ways of distancing
themselves from the relational partner (Hess, in press). Some of these
were avoidant strategies, such as making interactions shorter in duration,
staying away from the other person as much as possible, or simply ignoring
the other. When avoidance was not an option, people reported trying to
make the interaction as disengaged as possible. For example, people re-
ported using nonverbal cues that indicated dissociation (e.g., less smiling,
standing further away, less eye contact, less touch), restricting the amount
of information they shared about themselves, or focusing their attention
away from the disliked partner. Finally, people indicated that sometimes
they simply tried to alter their perceptions of the interactions, such as by
feeling detached or by mentally degrading the person (Hess, in press; see
Table 5.1). Wiseman and Duck (1995) f ound that people preferred avoid-
ance whenever possible when dealing with enemies, but also used disen-
gaging behaviors when necessary. For example, people reported disclosing
less information, becoming involved in different social circles, and trying to
show the enemy that they have less in common with each other. Interest-
ingly, few people reported trying to resolve differences with their enemy.
The challenge people face in these circumstances is that a certain degree
of relational closeness is necessary to maintain the relationship. So, people
must find ways to achieve distance without sacrificing the minimal levels
of closeness required to sustain the relationship. In some cases, such as
with disliked relatives, avoidance might often be a feasible distancing be-
havior. But in a case such as a blended family where siblings might dislike
step-siblings who live in the same household, avoidance can be difficult to
do. In cases such as those, dissociative behaviors or even just perceptual
strategies might prove most effective.

Antagonism. One interactive behavior that warrants attention in the


study of unwanted relationships is antagonism. Antagonism can range from
negative remarks or jokes at another expense to verbal and physi-
cal abuse. Although justified revenge is sometimes socially sanctioned
(Axelrod, 1984; Tripp & Bies, 1997), overt and ongoing hostility is rarely
acceptable unless the relationship involves members of hostile social
groups, in which case hostility against the outgroup is approved by ingroup
members (although not necessarily by third parties). Despite the general
disapproval of antagonism, such behavior is quite common in our society.
Many scholars talk about the prevalence of relational or family violence
(e.g., Johnson, 1995; Rusbult & Martz, 19951, and Berscheid (1983) con-
TABLE 5.1
Distancing Tactics ldent&ed by Hess (in press)

Tactic Definition

Avoidance Trying not to be in the presence of the other person

Deception Lying to or misleading the other person on


information about oneself

Degrade Perceiving the other person as less than human,


such as by ignoring her/his feelings, or seeing the
other person as incompetent

Detachment Perceiving or feeling a lack of attachment with the


other

Discount message Disregarding or minimizing what the other person


says

Group interaction Avoiding one-on-one interactions with the person

Humoring Considering the other person to be eccentric and


someone just to be tolerated, but not taken
seriously

Ignoring Acting as if the other person is not there

Impersonal Treating the other person like a stranger; that is,


interacting with her/him as a role rather than as a
unique individual

Inattention Giving as little attention as possible to the other


person

Nonimmediacy Displaying verbal or nonverbal cues that minimize


closeness or availability

Reserve Being unusually quiet and uncommunicative when


with the other person

Restraint Curtailing social behaviors that one would


normally do, which (if done) would have led to
greater relational closeness

Restrict topics Limiting to conversation to topics that are not


intimate

Shorten interaction Doing what it takes to end the interaction as


quickly as possible

115
tended that the family is one of the most violent institutions an ordinary
person is likely to encounter. Berscheid claimed that most of the anger and
hostility people experience in daily life is directed toward a relative.
Well-documented communication behaviors that are antagonistic or hos-
tile include chronic disconfirmation and double-binds (Watzlawick et al.,
1967), verbal aggressiveness (Infante & Wigley, 1986), and boundary vio-
lations (Peterson, 1992).
One study on the maintenance of relationships with disliked partners
found that all respondents reported using hostile tactics from time to time
(Hess, 2000). Although most people reported antagonizing their disliked
partners only occasionally (possibly only when most frustrated or when an
enticing opportunity presented itself), a few respondents indicated favor-
ing antisocial tactics more often. Research suggests that such behavior will
often invite counterattacks and escalation (DeRidder, Schruijer, &
Rijsman, 1999), w h ic h means that it is not usually the most rational inter-
action strategy. So, it may be that people interact this way when they feel
immune to retaliation or when they cannot control their anger. It is also
possible that some people use antisocial acts as a way of expressing or
achieving control, as is often the case with abusive relationships (Johnson,
1995). Closer examination of these relationships might reveal the causes
of hostility and the effects it has on the people involved. Although the re-
search on verbal and physical abuse makes it clear that such behavior has
detrimental outcomes in relationships (Cahn, 1996), the range of impacts
that small to moderate degrees of nonabusive hostility has in unwanted re-
lationships is less clear.

Communication and Self-Image. Another factor that seems likely to


have an important impact on communication in unwanted relationships is the
management of meaning related to self-presentation and self-image. Because
unwanted relationships put people into situations that contradict their
interactional preferences, they may face situations that test their
self-concepts and pose difficulties with presentation of face more than in ordi-
nary relationships. These situations can entail contradictory goals or feelings,
and they impact how people communicate with each other. People who con-
sider themselves good people but act antagonistically toward an undesirable
person, people who consider themselves tolerant but find themselves being
short with an unwanted co-worker, or people who consider themselves loving
but find themselves stewing in anger at an annoying relative all may face cogni-
tive dissonance about their own definition of self. The challenge to manage
meanings in these circumstances may impact the communication that hap-
pens between the actor and the undesired relational partner. As Duck (1994a)
noted, “the disembodied social psychological concepts that we read about as
impression management, self-disclosure, interdependence, and social ex-
change are also created or served mostly in talk” (p. 10).
5. MAINTAINING UNDESIRED RELATIONSHIPS ++i- 117

Task and Social Balance. One aspect of undesired relationships that


seems especially salient in the workplace is the difficulty of maximizing
task effectiveness when that task forces participation in an unwanted rela-
tionship. Unpleasant peer relationships in the workplace interfere with
successful task outcomes (Fritz & Omdahl, 1998). A case could be made
that this outcome should not necessarily follow, because keeping interac-
tions focused on task, rather than relational issues, is one way people create
distance (Hess, 2000). However, simply interacting on a task level is im-
possible. First, the general consensus among scholars is that virtually all
communication involves both content and relational information, so it is
impossible to remove the relational component from a communicative ex-
change (e.g., Burgoon & Hale, 1984; Watzlawick et al., 1967). Second, ef-
fective social interaction is a contributing factor in task success (Bormann,
1990). research shows that groups that tried to focus exclu-
sively on task concerns and eliminate any social dimension to their interac-
tion were less effective than counterpart groups that effectively balanced
task and social elements in their work. So, to maximize task success,
interactants in undesired relationships must find a balance between social
interaction and disengagement.

Multiple Audience Problem. The multiple audience problem is a


challenge for relational communication, whether the interaction happens
in the workplace or a social setting. It refers to a communicative situation
in which a speaker needs to simultaneously meet different, and usually
mutually exclusive, purposes with a single message (Fleming & Darley,
199 1; Fleming, Darley, Hilton, & Kojetin, 1990). The challenge is to ad-
dress the conflicting purposes in message construction so that all parties
are treated in ways that meet the social goals. Although this prob-
lem is not unique to undesired relationships, it is likely to present itself
when a mutual acquaintance is present for whom the relationship with
the target person is desired. In this case, a person may want to distance
herself or himself from the undesired partner without simultaneously
suggesting a desire to do so to the favored relational partner. The reverse
can also occur. If a third party is present who considers a relationship with
the target person unwanted, an individual may wish to show the third
party their dissociation from the target person (to avoid perceptions of
affiliation) while concealing that message from the target. Researchers
have found many creative ways that people attempt such deceit. For ex-
ample, people can word messages in a way that the target and the third
party would interpret differently, display nonverbal cues visible only to
the third party, or convey relational messages using indirect references
that the target person could not interpret (e.g., Clark & Schaefer, 1987;
Fleming & Darley, 1991).
118 -E=+ HESS

CONCLUSIONS AND DIRECTIONS


l=OR I=UTURlZ RlES~AKCH

Undesired relationships present a challenging context for communication


because they force people into situations that are uncomfortable at best,
dangerous at worst. Negotiating the competing tensions of contradictory
goals, emotional temptations, and social constraints requires successfully
dealing with complex challenges in relational communication. So many
variations in relational definitions and demands, personality traits, and so-
cial demands exist that it is difficult to propose a small set of conclusions
about such relationships or recommendations for productive actions.
However, one conclusion seems reasonable: that these relationships bring a
greater than average share of communicative challenges. Thus, they should
be a rich ground for extending our knowledge about the communicative
phenomena that can be observed there.
What we do know about undesired relationships can be summarized as
follows. They may be caused by obstruction of goals, negative affect, or
both. People see them as essential to maintain despite their undesirability
due to barriers that arise from external forces, internal forces, or both. Un-
wanted relationships cause stress to those people who would prefer not to
maintain them. Although people are likely to act antagonistically at least
some of the time, distance is the primary way people cope with the stress
these relationships create, and thus, sustain the relationship. Other com-
municative aspects of these relationships vary widely, but such issues as
image management, task-social balance, and multiple audience problems
seem to be likely tensions for a person to face. The combination of input
variables (personality traits and the conditions making a relationship both
unwanted and nonvoluntary) and process variables (interactive behaviors
by the two people) determine the personal and social outcomes from the
relationship. Closer examination of these issues seems to offer the possi-
bility of improving the quality of lives. How, then, might research
best proceed?
One of the important contributions the study of undesired relationships
can make is to create a better understanding of what communication be-
haviors best contribute to the well-being of those involved, and what peo-
ple must do to achieve that type of communication. Such communication
not only benefits psychological and physical health (Gottman,
1994), it also reduces the chances of negative experiences leading to in-
creased hostility among the partners or others in their social networks
(Berscheid, Boye, & Walster, 1968). Th us, a useful first step in research
would be identification of what communication behaviors are associated
with relational health or dysfunction in these relationships. Wright and
Wright (1995) did this type of work for the study of codependent relation-
5. MAINTAINING UNDESIRED RELATIONSHIPS ++ 11T

ships. They argued that although codependency is usually studied as a per-


sonality syndrome, it is more useful to study codependent interaction as it
exists within a certain relationship. Although certain people might be more
predisposed to enter codependent relations (valuable information in its
own right, they noted), it may be more informative to first understand
what makes a relationship codependent. Such knowledge can help people
identify and change the behaviors that cause unhealthy outcomes. The
same approach could work well with undesired relationships. Are there
identifiable patterns of communication that are common to such relation-
ships, perhaps associated with certain causes of the undesirability or rea-
sons for maintenance, that signal problematic outcomes? If so, identifying
them will have both practical and theoretical benefits.
Another avenue of research that could be productive is to identify per-
sonality traits that are associated with either the likelihood of maintain-
ing undesired relationships or the enactment of certain communication
behaviors. Several factors seem ripe for investigation. For example, hav-
ing an external locus of control may predict the likelihood or prevalence
of undesired relationships in a social life. People who have an ex-
ternal locus of control see themselves as being helpless to control many
things that happen to them (Hewitt & Flett, 1996; Rotter, 1966). So,
these people are less likely to pursue some valued goals, and research sug-
gests that they have less ability to cope with stressful experiences in their
lives (Lefcourt, 1991).
A factor that might predict a propensity to stay in an undesir-
able relationship is risk aversion (Kahneman & Tversky, 1984). Research
has suggested that high aversion to risk taking may prompt people to com-
promise their relational desires (e.g., as in maintaining a platonic relation-
ship; Messman, Canary, & Hause, 2000). For some people, the safety and
security of what is known may form an internal barrier, causing them to
stay in undesired relationships, despite any negative outcomes that result.
Emotional intelligence is another personality trait that may relate to how
people respond to undesired relationships. Salovy, Bedell, Detweiler, and
Mayer (1999) argued that people with higher emotional intelligence can
cope better in relationships and may be less stressed than those with lower
emotional intelligence.
Although these personality traits seem theoretically justified as factors
that impact unwanted relationships, such a conclusion is premature with-
out empirical evidence. Levitt et al. (1996) examined personality traits
such as self-esteem and attachment style in relation to troublesome rela-
tionships and found that those traits “were generally associated more
strongly with modes of coping than with whether or not the individual had
had a difficult relationship” (p. 533). So, both theoretical and empirical ev-
idence must be examined before drawing conclusions about the impact of
120 -e=s HESS

personality traits on the likelihood of developing unwanted relationships


or the manner in which a person maintains them.
A third avenue of research that might provide useful information is an
examination of whether certain social behaviors can reduce
chances of finding themselves in undesired relationships. In their exami-
nation of coping, Pierce et al. (1996) asked why researchers seem to fo-
cus more on how people handle difficult situations than on why some
people find themselves in dire straights more often than others. Cer-
tainly, personality traits and bad luck are factors. But, Pierce et al. noted
that the own behaviors can also play an important role. For
example, if one person fears a depression and saves money whereas an-
other spends it freely, these people would face different situations in an
economic downturn. They noted that “coping researchers investigating
only those persons who have faced or are facing major economic hardship
would identify only the latter person, thus overlooking that the former
person avoided the problem by with the event prior to its occur-
rence” (p. 434). Analogously, some people might find themselves in more
undesired relationships than others in part due to social choices they
made prior to such relationships forming or becoming undesirable. Re-
searchers might be able to determine whether behaviors can
actually affect the number of undesirable relationships they face, and if
so, what behaviors those are.
One way that behaviors might affect their propensity to find
themselves in undesired relationships relates to satisfaction of needs.
Drigotas and (1992) argued that people stay in unsatisfying rela-
tionships to the extent that they depend on that relationship to meet cer-
tain needs (e.g., emotional involvement, sex, companionship). It may be
that some people invest too heavily in certain relationships (perhaps ignor-
ing warning signs that others would observe) and allow such relationships
to become the only channels for meeting those needs. Doing so could make
such relationships nonvoluntary to them because of their inability to meet
their needs without it. If the relationship later becomes undesired, the per-
son feels trapped. People could avoid the problem by cultivating additional
relationships that meet the same need, that is, by creating a “need satisfac-
tion redundancy” across relationships. Of course, while doing so can insu-
late a person from becoming trapped in certain unwanted relationships, it
risks reducing a ability to maintain extremely close relationships.
Making a relationship ordinary and replaceable as a way of keeping oneself
“safe” from becoming trapped makes the relationship less special because
uniqueness and irreplaceability are hallmarks of close relations. So, people
who wish to avoid becoming entrapped in a relationship that cannot be re-
placed must be careful that their strategies do not subvert their ability to
maintain close and meaningful relations.
5. MAINTAINING UNDESIRED RELATIONSHIPS =+- 121

Undesired relationships are, and always will be, one of the more diffi-
cult relationships that people encounter. Because they are an inevitable as-
pect of social interaction, everyone must face such relationships
throughout the course of their lives. It is for challenging relations such as
these that the relational research holds much promise. Learning how to
manage such relationships in productive ways provides benefits for theory
construction and for practical application.

Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The development of interpersonal
relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Attridge, M. (1994). Barriers to dissolution of romantic relationships. In D. J. Canary &
L. Stafford (Eds.), C ommunication and relational maintenance (pp. 141-164). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Axelrod, R. (1984). Th e evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.
Ayres, J. (1983). Strategies to maintain relationships: Their identification and per-
ceived usage. Communication Quarterly, 3 1, 62-67.
Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (1996). Relating: Dialogues and dialectics. New
York: Guilford.
Bell, R. A., Daly, J. A., & G onzalez, C. (1987). Affinity-maintenance in marriage and its
relationship to marital satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
49, 445-454.
Berger, C. R. (1997). PI anning strategic interaction.. Attaining goals through communi-
cative action. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Berger, C. R. (2000). Goal detection and efficiency: Neglected aspects of message pro-
duction. Communication Theory, IO, 156-l 66.
Berscheid, E. (1983). Emotion. In H. H. Kelley, E. Berscheid, A. Christensen, J. H.
Harvey, T L. Huston, G. Levinger, E. McClintock, L. A. Peplau, & D. R. Peterson
(Eds.), Close relationships (pp. 110-168). New York: W. H. Freeman.
Berscheid, E., Boye, D., & Walster, E. (1968). Retaliation as a means of restoring equity.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 370-376.
Berscheid, E., Snyder, M., & Omoto, A. (1989). The relationship closeness inventory:
Assessing the closeness of interpersonal relationships. Journal of Personality and So-
cial Psychology, 57, 792-807.
Bogdan, R. J. (1994). G rounds for cognition: How goal-guided behavior shapes the
mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bormann, E. G. (1990). Small g rou p communication: Theory and practice. New York:
Harper & Row.
Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1984). Th e f un d amental topoi of relational communica-
tion. Communication Monographs, 51, 193-2 14.
Brinkman, R., & Kirschner, R. (1994). Dealing with people you stand: How to bring
out the best in people at their worst. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Byrne, D., & Murnen, S. K. (1988). Maintaining loving relationships. In R. J. Sternberg
& M. L. Barnes (Eds.), The psychology of love (pp. 293-310). New Haven, CT Yale
University Press.
Cahn, D. D. (1996). Family violence from a communication perspective. In D. D.
Cahn & S. A. Lloyd (Eds.), Family violence from a communication perspective (pp.
I-l 9). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
122 w HESS

Clark, H. H., & Schaefer, E. F. (1987). C oncealing meaning from overhearers. JournaL
of Memory and Language, 26, 209-225.
Coleman, M., & Ganong, L. H. (1995). Family reconfiguring following divorce. In. S .
Duck &J. T. Wood (Eds.), Confronting relationship challenges (pp. 73-108). Thou-
sand Oaks, CA: Sage.
DeRidder, R., Schruijer, S. G., & Rijsman, J. B. (1999). Retaliation to personal attack.
Aggressive Behavior, 2.5, 91-96.
Dickens, W J., & Perlman, D. (1981). Friendship over the life-cycle. In S. Duck & R.
Gilmour (Eds.), Personal relationships: VOL. 2. Developing personal relationships
(pp. 91-122). New York: Academic Press.
Dillard, J. P, Segrin, C., & Harden, J. M. (1989). P rimary and secondary goals in the
production of interpersonal influence messages. Communication Monographs, 56,
19-38.
Drigotas, S. M., & Rusbult, C. E. (1992). Should I go or should I stay? A dependence
model of breakups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 62-87.
Duck, S. (1987). H ow to lose friends without influencing people. In M. E. Roloff & G.
R. Miller (Eds.), lnterpersonalprocesses: New directions in communication research
(pp. 278-298). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Duck, S. (1994a). Meaningful relationships: Talking, sense, and relating. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Duck, S. (1994b). Stratagems, spoils and a tooth: On the delights and dilem-
mas of personal relationships. In W R. Cupach & B. H. Spitzberg (Eds.), The dark
side of interpersonal communication (pp. 3-24). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Finello, K. (2000, November). Do you attract people rather repel? Self p. 134.
Fleming, J. H., & Darley, J. M. (1991). Mixed messages: The multiple audience prob-
lem and strategic communication. Social Cognition, 9, 25-46.
Fleming, J. H., Darley, J. M., Hilton, J. L., & Kojetin, B. A. (1990). Multiple audience
problem: A strategic communication perspective on social perception. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 593-609.
Fritz, J. M. H. (1997). Responses to unpleasant work relationships. Communication Re-
search Reports, 14, 302-311.
Fritz, J. M. H., & Omdahl, B. L. (1998, November). Effects of negative peer interactions
on organizational outcomes. Paper presented at the annual convention of
the National Communication Association, New York, NY.
Galvin, K. M., & Cooper, P J. (1990, June). Development of involuntary relationships:
The stepparent-stepchild relationship. Paper presented at the annual convention of
the International Communication Association, Dublin, Ireland.
Gottman, J. (1994). Why marriages fail. The Family Therapy Network, 41-48.
Greene, J. 0. (1984). Evaluating cognitive explanations of communication phenom-
ena. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 70, 241-254.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.
Hess, J. A. (2000). Maintaining nonvoluntary relationships with disliked partners: An
investigation into the use of distancing behaviors. Human Communication Research,
26, 458-488.
Hess, J. A. (in press). Distance regulation in personal relationships: The development
of a conceptual model and a test of representational validity. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships.
Hewitt, P L., & Flett, G. L. (1996). Personality traits and the coping process. In M.
Zeidner & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Handbook of coping: Theory, research, applications
(pp. 410-433). New York: Wiley.
5. MAINTAINING UNDESIRED RELATIONSHIPS +=a- 123

Holahan, C. J., Moos, R. F., & Schaefer, J. A. (1996). Coping, stress resistance, and
growth: Conceptualizing adaptive functioning. In M. Zeidner & N. S. Endler (Eds.),
Handbook of coping: Theory, research, applications (pp. 24-43). New York: Wiley.
Infante, D., & Wigley, III, C. J. (1986). Verbal aggressiveness: An interpersonal model
and measure. Communication Monographs, 53, 6 1-69.
Johnson, M. I? (1995). Patriarchal terrorism and common couple violence: Two forms
of violence against women. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 283-294.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psycholo-
gist, 39, 341-350.
Kelley, H. H., Berscheid, E., Christensen, A., Harvey, J. H., Huston, T L., Levinger, G.,
McClintock, E., Peplau, L. A., & Peterson, D. R. (1983). Analyzing close relation-
ships. In H. H. Kelley, E. Berscheid, A. Christensen, J. H. Harvey, T. L. Huston, G.
Levinger, E. McClintock, L. A. Peplau, & D. R. Peterson (Eds.), Close relationships
(pp. 20-67). N ew York: W. H. Freeman.
Kinney, T. A. (1998). The psychosomatic effects of negative interactions. Psychoso-
matic Medicine, 60, 114.
Krippendorff, K. (1989). The power of communication and the communication of
power: Toward an emancipatory theory of communication. Communication, 12,
175-196.
Lefcourt, H. M. (1991). L ecus of control. In J. F! Robinson, P R. Shaver, & L. S.
Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (Vol.
1, pp. 4 13-499). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Lempert, L. B. (1997). The line in the sand: Definitional dialogues in abusive relation-
ships. In A. Strauss &J. Corbin (Eds.), G rounded theory in practice (pp. 147-l 70).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Levinger, G. A. (1965). Marital cohesiveness and dissolution: An integrative review.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 2 7, 19-28.
Levinger, G. A. (1976). A social psychological perspective on marital dissolution. Jour-
nal of Social Issues, 32, 2 l-47.
Levitt, M. J., Silver, M. E., & France, N. (1996). Troublesome relationships: A part of
human experience. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 13, 523-536.
Messman, S. J., Canary, D. J., & Hause, K. W (2000). Motives to remain platonic, eq-
uity, and the use of maintenance strategies in opposite-sex friendships. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 67-94.
Newcomb, T. M. (1968). Interpersonal balance. In R. I? Abelson, E. Aronson, W. J.
McGuire, T M. Newcomb, M. J. Rosenberg, &I? H. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Theories of
cognitive consistency: A sourcebook (pp. 28-5 1). Chicago: Rand McNally.
Peterson, M. R. (1992). At personal risk: Boundary violations in professional-client re-
lationships. New York: W. W. Norton.
Pierce, G. R., Sarason, I. G., & Sarason, B. R. (1996). In M. Zeidner & N. S. Endler (Eds.),
Handbook of coping: Theory, research, applications (pp. 434-451). New York: Wiley.
Precker, M. (2000, September 19). You bug me! Dallas Morning News, pp. lC-2C.
Rotter, J. B. (1966). G eneralized expectancies for internal versus external control of re-
inforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80 (1, Whole No. 609). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Rubin, Z. (1973). Likingandl oving: An invitation to social psychology. New York: Holt,
Rinehart, & Winston.
Rusbult, C. E. (1987). R esp onses to dissatisfaction in close relationships: The
“exit-voice-loyalty-neglect” model. In D. Perlman & S. Duck (Eds.), Intimate rela-
tionships: Development, dynamics, and deterioration (pp. 209-237). Newbury Park,
CA: Sage.
124 -c=+ HESS

Rusbult, C. E., & Martz, J. M. (1995). R emaining in an abusive relationship: An invest-


ment model analysis of nonvoluntary dependence. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 21, 558-571.
Salovy, P, Bedell, B. T., Detweiler, J. B., & Mayer, J. D. (1999). Coping intelligently:
Emotional intelligence and the coping process. In C. R. Snyder (Ed.), Coping: The
psychology of what works (pp. 14 l-l 64). New York: Oxford University Press.
Schwartz, J. E., & Stone, A. A. (1993). Coping and daily work problems: Contributions
of problem content, appraisals and person factors. Work and Stress, 7, 47-62.
Strube, M. J., & Barbour, L. S. (1983). Th e d e&ion to leave an abusive relationship:
Economic dependence and psychological commitment. Journal ofMarriage and the
Family, 45, 785-793.
Thibaut, J. W, & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of groups. New York:
Wiley.
Thomsen, D. G., & Gilbert, D. G. (1998). Factors characterizing marital conflict states
and traits: Physiological, affective, behavioral and neurotic variable contributions to
marital conflict and satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 25,
833-855.
Tripp, T. M., & Bies, R. J. (1997). good about revenge? The perspec-
tive. In R. J. Lewicki, R. J. Bies, &B. H. Sheppard (Eds.), Research on negotiation in
organizations (Vol. 6, pp. 145-160). Greenwich, CT JAI Press.
Watzlawick, I?, Beavin, J., & Jackson, D. D. (1967). Pragmatics of human communica-
tion. New York: W W Norton.
West, J. T. (1995). Understanding how the dynamics of ideology influence violence be-
tween intimates. In. S. Duck & J. T. Wood (Eds.), Confronting relationship chal-
lenges (pp. 129-l 49). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Wiseman, J. P, & Duck, S. (1995). H aving and managing enemies: A very challenging
relationship. In. S. Duck & J. T. Wood (Eds.), Confronting relationship challenges
(pp. 43-72). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Wood, J. T., & Duck, S. (1995). Off the beaten path: New shores for relationship re-
search. In J. T. Wood & S. Duck (Eds.), Under-studied relationships: Off the beaten
track (pp. 1-21). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Wright, I? H., & Wright, K. D. (1995). Codependency: Personality syndrome or rela-
tional process? In. S. Duck & J. T. Wood (Eds.), Confronting relationship challenges
(pp. 109-128). Th ousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Contextual Variations
in Maintaining RelationstfiPs
M -+
This page intentionally left blank
+” * +“:,

*2

Maintaining -Distance
Relations a $35

Brooks A. Aylor
La Salle University

he majority of relational maintenance research has focused on behaviors


enacted to maintain geographically close relationships (GCRs); that is, rela-
tionships in which the partners are able to see each other, face-to-face, most
days. Long-distance relationships (LDRs), in which daily face-to-face con-
tact is not possible, are increasingly common in the United States, yet we
know relatively little about the processes used to maintain these relation-
ships. The study of LDRs holds the promise of extending previous relational
maintenance theory and research in this important relational context.
Reviewed in this chapter is the small but growing body of literature exam-
ining maintenance processes in LDRs. Although long-distance friendships
have received some attention in this literature (for a review, see Rohlfing,
1995), the focus here is on romantic LDRs; individuals who define their re-
lationships as casually dating, seriously dating, engagement or married.
There are many reasons to extend our study of relationships to distance
relationships. Among these are the growing number of distance relation-
ships, the disproportionate number of college students in LDRs, and the
unique characteristics of distance relationships relative to geographically
close relationships. Accordingly, this chapter first describes the growing
number of LDRs, unique characteristics of LDRs, and competing defini-

127
128 -e- AYLOR

tions of distance relationships. Further, work on relational characteristics


of satisfaction, commitment and trust in LDRs is reviewed. Taken as a
whole, this work suggests a paradox in LDRs, a picture of partners report-
ing comparative levels of relational satisfaction as their GCR counterparts,
yet simultaneously facing the very real challenges inherent in distance rela-
tionships. The final focus of the chapter is to help explain this paradox via
research on relational maintenance and communication modes in LDRs,
while offering future research directions.

PFXVALENCIZ Of= LDRs

Distance relationships have become increasingly common in this country.


Research suggests that as many as 1 million people annually report being in
a long-distance romantic relationship (Maines, 1994). Armour (1998)
noted that changes in technology and the workforce have led to record
numbers of commuter marriages and other types of distance relationships.
Armour presented a 1998 report from the Employee Relocation Council
showing that approximately 10% of all job relocations in the United States
resulted in LDRs, and 52% of employers in 1998 expected to see the num-
ber of job transfers increase.
Distance relationships are particularly prevalent among college stu-
dents. Nationally, 25% to 40% of college students reported being in a LDR
(Dellman-Jenkins, Bernard-Paolucci, & Rushing, 1993). Studies of
first-year college students suggest that LDRs are even more common
among these students, with as many as one half of first-year students in
LDRs (Knox, 1992). My ongoing work with Marianne Dainton further
supports the idea that distance relationships are prominent in college pop-
ulations. In our samples, we have consistently found that approximately
one third of dating college students consider their relationship to be long
distance, and about one half of first-year students report being in a roman-
tic distance relationship. This work is also consistent with Stafford, Daly,
and (1987) c 1aim that “as many as one third of premarital relation-
ships in university settings may be long-distance ones” (p. 274).

UNIQUE CHARACTWWiTICS Ol= LDRs

It is my contention that LDRs are qualitatively different from GCRs and


present partners with unique challenges to maintain such relationships. By
definition, partners in LDRs face geographic separation and lack of
face-to-face contact. Rohlfing (1995) reviewed research by Westefeld and
Liddell (1982) suggesting the following unique challenges for those in dis-
tance relationships:
6. LONG-DISTANCE RELATIONSHIPS +=a- 129

l Increased financial burdens to maintain distance relationships.


l Difficulty defining and negotiating “in-town” (i.e., geographically
close friendships) relationships while maintaining a long-distance ro-
mantic relationship.
l High expectations by partners for the quality of limited face-to-face
meetings afforded in the relationship.
l Difficulty assessing the degree and state of the relationship from a
distance.
l More extreme range of emotions experienced by partners in distance
relationships.

In addition, it should be noted that the financial burdens and geographic


separation in LDRs often lead to a greater reliance on mediated communi-
cation (Aylor & Dainton, 2002; Canary, Stafford, Hause, & Wallace, 1993;
Gerstel & Gross, 1984; Stephen, 1986). Further, Ficara and Mongeau
(2000) suggested that the physical absence of relational partner may
increase levels of general and relationship-specific uncertainty relative to
GCRs. Rohlfing (1995) correctly noted that married couples that are sepa-
rated geographically may face the unique challenge of cultural expecta-
tions to live together and sacrifice personal goals for the relationship.
Given these characteristics, it is important to examine the way research on
LDRs has been conducted and what information we currently have about
relational maintenance processes in distance relationships.

WHAT IS AN LDR?
Controversy exists concerning how to measure distance relationships.
Those studying LDRs have generally taken one of three approaches to de-
fining them. The first approach is to use the number of miles separated to
distinguish between distance and geographically close relationships. That
is, researchers have established a minimum number of miles necessary for
a relationship to be operationalized as a distance relationship.
For example Carpenter and Knox (1986) operationalized LDRs as part-
ners separated by more than 100 miles, but Schwebel, Dunn, Moss, and
Renner (1992) established a criterion of only 50 miles separated. Still oth-
ers (e.g., Stafford & Reske, 1990) reported the average number of miles
separating partners in LDRs, yet did not report how they distinguished
LDRs from GCRs.
Others have specified geographical boundaries (e.g., state lines) to de-
fine an LDR. Instead of the miles that separate residences, these research-
ers have focused on the city or state of residence as the criterion to
determine distance relationships. Helgeson (1994), in her examination of
relational dissolution in LDRs, defined an LDR as one in which one partner
130 -t++ AYLOR

lives outside the city limits of the other residence, whereas Ste-
phen (1986) re q uired that partners live in different states or different
parts of the same state. Canary et al. (1993) defined LDRs as relationships
in which the partners lived in separate towns.
A third school of thought has been to allow respondents to define if the
relationship is a distance relationship, regardless of the number of miles or
geographic boundaries that separate partners. Some studies (e.g., Dainton
and Aylor, 2001) h ave included a question similar to the following:

A geographically-close relationship is one in which partners are able to see each


other, if they choose, face-to-face, most days. A long-distance relationship is one
in which both partners are not able to see each other, face-to-face, most days.
Would you consider your relationship a distance relationship?

A version of this approach was used by Ficara and Mongeau (2000). They
asked respondents to indicate if they were not able to see each other “as
much as they would like primarily due to geographic separation.” Maguire
(1999) allowed respondents to indicate “if they were unable to see each
other on a regular basis (e.g., daily or weekly) due to time and/or distance
constraints.” Guldner and Swensen (1995) posed the statement, “my part-
ner lives far enough away from me that it would be very difficult or impossi-
ble for me to see him or her every day.”
Researchers such as Dellman-Jenkins et al. (1993) argued that allowing re-
spondents to define if their relationship is a distance relationship is more valid
than “miles separated” or “geographic boundary” standards because a
self-defined approach “is based on definitions, and their own
sense of reality in dating situations. To paraphrase W I. Thomas: If people de-
fine a situation as real, it becomes real in its consequences (p. 2 13) .” As was
previously noted, “miles separated” standards vary considerably across stud-
ies. Additionally, respondents often have difficulty accurately reporting the
number of miles separating themselves. Thus, a strict application of a “miles
separated” criterion may not accurately measure all distance relationships.

RELATIONAL CHAKACT~KISTICS IN LDRs

Because a common conceptualization of relational maintenance includes


efforts to continue a relationship in a particular state or condition
(Dindia & Canary, 1993), much research on relational maintenance has fo-
cused on perceptions of satisfaction with and commitment to the
relationship. The majority of research on geographically close relationships
has established clear relationships between relational maintenance behav-
iors and relational satisfaction, and between commitment and relational
stability (see e.g., Dainton, Stafford, & Canary, 1994; Lund, 1985;
Stafford & Canary, 1991).
6. LONG-DISTANCE RELATIONSHIPS +=a- 131

Although most of this research has focused on GCRs, some research has
examined these characteristics in LDRs. Taken together, these findings
suggest that, contrary to popular opinion, individuals in LDRs experience
the same or even greater levels of satisfaction and commitment relative to
their GCR counterparts.
For example, Guldner and Swensen (1995) found no differences be-
tween those in LDRs and GCRs on satisfaction or commitment. Similarly,
Govaerts and Dixon (1988), in their study of commuter marriages, found
no significant differences in satisfaction between the two groups. Stafford
and Reske (1990) reported that individuals in LDRs were more satisfied
with and committed to the relationship (defined as more in love) than their
counterparts in GCRs. They argued that this might be explained by the
tendency of those in LDRs to idealize their partners due to restricted
face-to-face communication.
Although the majority of studies seem to suggest that LDR partners ex-
perience the same or greater levels of satisfaction and commitment, an ex-
ception was the work of Holt and Stone (1988). They reported negative
relationships between both distance apart and satisfaction and time be-
tween visits and satisfaction. Additionally, in a longitudinal study of
long-distance and geographically close marriages, Rindfuss and Stephen
(1990) found that couples that were geographically separated at the time
of the study were significantly more likely to be divorced after 3 years. It
should be noted, however, that the generalizability of these findings has
been questioned because much of the sample consisted of military cou-
ples, a population that experiences higher divorce rates than the general
population (Guldner & Swensen, 1995; Rohlfing, 1995).
In addition to satisfaction and commitment, Dainton and Kilmer
(1999) argued that trust is an important relational characteristic, particu-
larly among partners who are geographically separated. Less attention has
been given to trust relative to satisfaction and commitment in geographi-
cally close relationships, but research does suggest that trust is critical to
relational quality (Canary & Cupach, 1988) and is positively related to re-
lational maintenance (Canary & Stafford, 1993). Interestingly, as Dainton
and Kilmer (1999) noted, trust has rarely been a focus of studies of LDRs.
Westefeld and Liddell (1982), in their qualitative analysis of coping strate-
gies of partners in LDRs, did imply that trust was critical for the long-term
success of LDRs. But this relationship has not been empirically tested. This
is particularly puzzling given the geographical separation and increased un-
certainty levels in distance relationships.
In summary, research on relational characteristics in distance relation-
ships suggests an interesting paradox. On the one hand, LDR partners face
the previously mentioned challenges and unique relational demands rela-
tive to their GCR counterparts based on the nature of a distance relation-
ship. On the basis of this research, one might conclude that distance
132 +==+ AYLOR

relationships often require more effort to successfully maintain relative to


geographically close relationships. On the other hand, however, most stud-
ies suggest few if any differences between partners in LDRs and GCRs on
satisfaction or commitment, and some research suggests greater levels of
satisfaction and commitment among those in LDRs. The rest of this chap-
ter is devoted to this paradox as research on maintenance processes, com-
munication channel use, and partner expectations is examined.

MAINTENANCE PROCl5SES IN LDRs

One of the first investigations of strategies used to successfully maintain


LDRs was the work of Westefeld and Liddell (1982). Their report was the
product of workshops conducted at Iowa State by the student YWCA and
counseling services program designed to explore the nature of romantic
distance relationships. Participants in the workshops were students cur-
rently involved in LDRs. From conversations with workshop participants,
Westefeld and Liddell reported nine maintenance strategies commonly
used to maintain successful LDRs. Rohlfing (1995) summarizes these:

Recognizing the prevalence of LDRs.


Developing support systems for partners who are separated.
Developing alternative ways to communicate, including sending
gifts, videotapes, and audiotapes.
Discussing relational expectations and relationship “ground rules”
prior to separation (e.g., will we date other people, how often will we
spend time with friends, how will we communicate with each other).
Using face-to-face time wisely by dealing with affection and other
needs.
Being open and honest with their partner.
Developing and maintaining trust.
Focusing on positive aspects of LDRs.

Some years later, Holt and Stone (1998) and Wilmot and Carbaugh
(1986) performed quantitative examinations of the effectiveness of cer-
tain behaviors in LDRs. Holt and Stone suggested that two strategies were
effective in maintaining LDRs, including frequent visits and visualizing
(i.e., daydreaming about the partner). They noted that visualizing posi-
tively affected relational satisfaction among partners with a “preference
for visual or verbal response modes of cognitive processing” (p. 137) but
that frequent visits benefited partners regardless of processing modes.
Although more empirical in nature than the typology offered by
Westefeld and Liddell (1982), the scope of this investigation was limited
to two behaviors. It can also be argued that visiting partner is a general
6. LONG-DISTANCE RELATIONSHIPS =a 133

behavior as opposed to specific maintenance activities that may or may not


be performed during those visits.
Wilmot and Carbaugh (1986) examined strategies used by partners in
LDRs to deal with physical separation. The coping behaviors reported are
typically distinguished from maintenance behaviors in that coping behav-
iors tend to focus on one partner, whereas maintenance behaviors are more
interactive in nature. These researchers identified self-development, inde-
pendence, adopting a religion, and high levels of self-disclosure as strate-
gies used to cope with physical separation.
Canary et al. (1993) conducted a study using subjects in romantic, platonic
and family relationships. Forty-two percent of their sample consisted of sub-
jects in LDRs, and the researchers analyzed open-ended responses concerning
strategies for maintaining various relationships. Subjects responded to the
question, “What are the communication behaviors that I use to maintain my
various relationships ?” They were required to address three different types of
relationships and to address both positive and negative behaviors used.
In all, 10 maintenance strategies were reported including positivity, open-
ness, assurances, sharing tasks, social networks, joint activities, cards/let-
ters/calls, avoidance, antisocial, and humor. This study did not, however,
distinguish between LDR and GCR partners in frequency of use of each
strategy. This inductive analysis supplemented previous work (Stafford SK
Canary, 1991) and f o 11owed an original flurry of work on relational mainte-
nance in geographically close relationships by communication scholars in the
1980s and early 1990s (see, e.g., Ayres, 1983; Bell, Daly, & Gonzalez, 1987;
Dindia & Baxter, 1987; Duck, 1988; Shea & Pearson, 1986).
More recently researchers have used the Canary and Stafford typology
of maintenance strategies to examine relational maintenance in LDRs. For
example, Ficara and Mongeau (2000) used an uncertainty reduction
framework in their examination of maintenance in LDRs. They focused on
the relationship between relational uncertainty and the use of three of Ca-
nary and maintenance behaviors, including positivity, assur-
ances, and openness. All three strategies were significantly and negatively
related to relational uncertainty in this study. Of the 170 subjects in this
study, however, only 55% were currently in an LDR. The remaining 45%
reported having terminated an LDR within the past 6 months.

CHANNEL USE AND MAINTENANCE IN LDRs

Most studies of LDR maintenance acknowledge that restricted communi-


cation and geographic separation lead partners to rely more on mediated
communication relative to their GCR counterparts. Stephen (1986) was
among the first to note that if either verbal or nonverbal communication is
restricted, relational maintenance should be primarily dependent on the
134 w AYLOR

unrestricted channel. In the case of distance relationships, the unrestricted


channel is verbal communication, and geographic separation dictates that
the majority of this must be mediated. Given the obvious importance of
channel use in any relationship, particularly distance relationships, re-
search on channel use in LDRs is reviewed separately here.
Generally, previous literature suggests that phone calls are the most
important mediated form of communication to partners in distance rela-
tionships (Carpenter & Knox, 1986; Gerstel & Gross, 1983; Stafford &
Reske, 1990). These studies also indicate the importance of cards, let-
ters, audiotapes, and videotapes. More recently, LDR scholars have ex-
amined the use of computer mediated communication to maintain
distance relationships (Aylor & Dainton, 2002; Maguire, 1999). Al-
though by definition distance relationships provide limited face-to-face
communication, the face-to-face contact that is afforded has emerged in
almost every examination of LDRs as a critical channel.
In my recent work with Marianne Dainton (Aylor & Dainton, 2002)
we found that face-to-face contact is not only a critical communication
channel in LDR maintenance, but can also differentiate between types
of distance relationships and can serve as a moderating variable for im-
portant relational outcomes. For example, in our study of jealousy expe-
rience, expression, in LDRs, we asked 114 individuals currently in
LDRs indicated how much face-to-face contact they had during a typi-
cal week. Thirty-three percent of participants reported no face-to-face
contact, whereas 67% reported periodic face-to-face contact with a
mean of 1 to 2 days.
This variable was instrumental in differentiating between distance re-
lationships with regard to jealousy experience, expression, and goals that
partners had in jealousy situations. When we compared LDRs as a whole
to GCRs, we noted few differences in jealousy experience, expression, or
goals. However, we found significant differences between GCRs, LDRs
with no periodic face-to-face contact, and LDRs with periodic
face-to-face contact. The pattern was for those in LDRs with no face-to-
face contact to experience more jealousy than those with periodic
face-to-face contact or those in GCRs.
Further, in our examination of the relationships between mediated
communication and maintenance behaviors in LDRs (Aylor & Dainton,
2002), individuals with periodic face-to-face contact used three of five
maintenance behaviors (shared tasks, positivity, and assurances) more fre-
quently than individuals with face-to-face contact or in geographically
close partners. Those without periodic face-to-face contact were more
likely to use the Internet to communicate with their partner, and the use of
computer-mediated communication was a significant predictor of trust for
these individuals but not for those with periodic face-to-face contact.
6. LONG-DISTANCE RELATIONSHIPS +==a 133

We also found the presence of periodic face-to-face contact to be an im-


portant factor in the satisfaction, commitment, and trust of LDR partners.
Those in LDRs who experienced periodic face-to-face contact reported
significantly higher levels of satisfaction, commitment, and trust than did
those with no face-to-face contact. We found no differences in satisfaction,
commitment, or trust when we compared distance relationships as a whole
with GCRs.
It seems then that the presence of some face-to-face interaction may be
one explanation for the paradox noted earlier; namely that individuals in
LDRs encounter more difficulties and challenges to relational survival, yet
report similar or higher levels of satisfaction and commitment as their
GCR counterparts. Distance alone should make LDRs less likely to suc-
ceed, as relationships that are more difficult to maintain are morelikely to
be terminated (Lloyd, Cate, & Henton, 1984). Yet previous research sug-
gests that LDRs are not terminated sooner than GCRs.
The previous studies did not distinguish between types of distance rela-
tionships. Distinguishing between those with face-to-face contact and
those without is consistent with (1999) argument that distance
relationship should not be viewed as homogenous relational types.
Another effort to explain this paradox comes from the work of Dainton
and Kilmer (1999). They posited that the differing expectations of rela-
tional partners in LDRs and GCRs might help explain the longevity of dis-
tance relationships. Their study of 485 subjects suggested that those in
LDRs and GCRs have similar expectations for the relationship and that hav-
ing these expectations met predicted satisfaction, commitment, and trust in
LDRs and GCRs. However, those in LDRs met or exceeded expectations
for relational maintenance to a greater extent than those in GCRs. The au-
thors noted that it is unclear if this finding suggests that LDR partners work
harder to meet expectations or if, as Stafford and Reske (1990) claimed, in-
dividuals in distance relationships tend to idealize their partners.

I=UTURE Rl3EAKC.H DIRKTIONS


Relational maintenance researchers have taken important first steps to-
ward a better understanding of the maintenance processes enacted in dis-
tance relationships. This chapter reviewed this work, including research on
the unique challenges of LDRs; relational characteristics of satisfaction,
commitment, and trust among individuals in LDRs; maintenance behav-
iors and channel use critical to maintaining LDRs; and partner expectations
for those in LDRs. Several avenues are worthy of future examination in an
attempt to further this growing body of research.
First, and most obviously, it is important that more maintenance re-
search focus on distance relationships. The increasing numbers and
types of distance relationships alone should encourage more research in
this context as we increasingly view knowledge of relational processes
in LDRs as an important extension of previous work on geographically
close relationships.
Second, it is important that researchers not consider distance relation-
ships as homogenous relational types. Research noted previously in this
chapter suggests that face-to-face contact is one variable that distinguishes
types of distance relationships. Others have argued that the reasons part-
ners separate (e.g., attending college, starting a new job, enlisting in the
military) make certain types of distance relationships qualitatively differ-
ent (Rohlfing, 1995; Sahlstein, 1999). Further, partners who have a defi-
nite idea of how long they will be geographically separated may differ in
maintenance processes relative to LDR partners who have no idea when, or
if, they will unite permanently. Clearly, distinctions do exist within dis-
tance relationships; all LDRs are not the same type of relationship. Many of
these differences seem significant and should be recognized in future re-
search if our examinations of maintenance processes are to be as produc-
tive as possible.
Third, more research needs to focus on communication channel use in
LDRs, particularly the use of computer-mediated communication, and the
relationships between channel use and maintenance behaviors in LDRs.
Many of the studies that have examined channel use predate the explosion
of the Internet. Today, more people have knowledge of, and access to, the
Internet and e-mail than ever before. As Clemente (1998) noted, more
than 50 million people use the Internet worldwide and 91% use the
Internet for personal reasons. Clemente also noted that 8 7% of users com-
municate via e-mail, and 44% have multiple e-mail accounts that are simul-
taneously active. Stafford, Kline, and Dimmick (1999) found that 61% of
home e-mail users reported using e-mail specifically for relationship main-
tenance purposes.
In addition to the use of conventional, asynchronous forms of com-
puter-mediated communication such as e-mail to maintain LDRs, it will
become increasingly important to examine geographically separated
use of synchronous computer-mediated communication (e.g.,
instant messaging, computer conferencing, real-time chat rooms). In-
creasingly individuals are able to use their computers in a way that pro-
vides instant feedback and more closely mirrors the dynamics of
face-to-face interaction.
Finally, not enough attention has focused on expectations of LDR part-
ners. The work of Dainton and Kilmer (1999) in this area is rare and to be
applauded. Relational maintenance researchers, particularly those guided
by a social exchange perspective, have long acknowledged the importance
of relational expectations in relational maintenance and development. One
might argue that partner expectations may be even more telling in distance
6. LONG-DISTANCE RELATIONSHIPS 3r, l-j7

relationships. Although distance relationships are gaining in popularity, the


majority of popular and academic information regarding relational mainte-
nance focuses on geographically close relationships. Because information
sources are less prevalent for those in LDRs, own experience in a dis-
tance relationship becomes increasingly important in explaining
maintenance activities and relational perceptions. Future examinations of
LDRs should examine a previous experiences with distance rela-
tionships, the outcomes of that relationship, and the expectations they
hold based partially on those past experiences.
In summary, as the number of distance relationships in this country
continues to increase, we understand more about these relationships
than ever before. Using a variety of theoretical frameworks, we have be-
gun to explore the unique nature and demands of LDRs; per-
ceptions of satisfaction, commitment and trust; maintenance behaviors
used to successfully maintain distance relationships; communication
channel use in LDRs; and relational expectations for those involved in
distance relationships. It is my hope that as the number and types of dis-
tance relationships increase, so too will serious empirical examinations of
the communication processes that facilitate the maintenance and devel-
opment of these relationships.
Indeed, it is hoped that rather than distance making the heart grow
fonder, in this case, distance will make the researcher work harder.

Armour, S. (1998, November 23). Married . . . with separation: More couples live apart
as careers put miles between them. USA Today, C2-C4.
Aylor, B., & Dainton, M. (2002). Patterns of communication channel use in the mainte-
nance of long-distance relationships. Communication Research Reports, 19,
118-129.
Ayres, J. (1983). Strategies to maintain relationships: Their identification and per-
ceived usage. Communication Quarterly, 31, 62-67.
Bell, R., Daly, J., & Gonzalez, M. (1987). Affinity-maintenance in marriage and its rela-
tionship to marital satisfaction. JournaZ of Marriage and the FumiZy, 49,
445-454.
Canary, D., & Cupach, W (1988). Relational and episodic characteristics associated
with conflict tactics. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 5, 305-325.
Canary, D., & Stafford, L. (1993). P reservation of relational characteristics: Mainte-
nance strategies, equity, and locus of control. In P J. Kalbfleisch (Ed.), Interpersonal
communication: Evolving interpersonal relationships (pp. 237-259). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Canary, D., Stafford, L., Hause, K., &Wallace, L. (1993). An inductive analysis of rela-
tional maintenance strategies: Comparisons among lovers, relatives, friends, and
others. Communication Research Reports, 10, 5-l 4.
Carpenter, D., & Knox, D. (1986). Relationship maintenance of college students sepa-
rated during courtship. College Student Journal, 28, 86-88.
Clemente, F?(1998). The state of the Net: The newfrontier. New York: McGraw-Hill.
138 +=+ AYLOR

Dainton, M., & Aylor, B. (2001). A relational uncertainty analysis of jealousy, trust, &
maintenance in long-distance versus geographically close relationships. Communi-
cation Quarterly, 49, 172-l 88.
Dainton, M., & Kilmer, H. (1999). Satisfaction, commitment, trust and expectations in
the maintenance of long-distance versus geographically-close relationships. Paper
presented at the annual conference of the National Communication Association;
Chicago.
Dainton, M, Stafford, L., & Canary, D. (1994). Maintenance strategies and physical af-
fection as predictors of love, liking, and satisfaction in marriage. Communication Re-
ports, 7, 88-98.
Dellmann-Jenkins, M., Bernard-Paolucci, T., & Rushing, B. (1993). Does distance
make the heart grow fonder? A comparison of college students in long-distance and
geographically-close dating relationships. College Student Journal, IO, 2 12-2 19.
Dindia, K., & Baxter, L. (1987). Strategies for maintaining and repairing marital rela-
tionships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 4, 143-158.
Dindia, K., & Canary, D. (1993). Definitions and theoretical perspectives on maintain-
ing relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 163-l 73.
Duck, S. (1988). Relating to others. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Ficara, L., & Mongeau, I? (2000, November). Relational uncertainty in long-distance
college student dating relationships. Paper presented at the annual conference of the
National Communication Association; Seattle, WA.
Gerstel, N., & Gross, H. (1984). Commuter marriages: A study of work and family.
New York: Guilford Press.
Govaerts, K., & Dixon, D. (1988). “. . . Until careers do us part”: Vocational and marital
satisfaction in the dual-career commuter marriage. international Journal for the Ad-
vancement of Counseling, 11, 265-28 1.
Guldner, G., & Swensen, C. (1995). T ime spent together and relationship quality:
Long-distance relationships as a test case. Journal of social and Personal Relation-
ships, 12, 313-320.
Helgeson, V (1994). Long-distance romantic relationships: Sex differences in adjust-
ment and breakup. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 254-265.
Holt, I?, & Stone, G. (1988). Needs, coping strategies, and coping outcomes associated
with long-distance relationships. Journal of College student Development, 29,
136-141.
Knox, D. (1992). Ch oices in relationships. St. Paul, MN: West Publishing.
Lloyd, S., Cate, R., & Henton, J. (1984). Predicting premarital relationship stability: A
methodological refinement. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 46, 71-76.
Lund, M. (1985). Th e d evelopment of investment and commitment scales for predict-
ing continuity of personal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
2, 3-23.
Maguire, K. (1999). Does distance make the heart work harder? A comparison of the
maintenance strategies of long-distance and proximal relationships. Paper presented
at the annual conference of the National Communication Association; Chicago.
Maines, J. (1994). Long-distance romances. American Demographics, IS, 47.
Rindfuss, R., & Stephen, E. (1990). Marital noncohabitation: Separation does not make
the heart grow fonder. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 259-270.
Rohlfing, M. (1995). anybody stay in one place anymore?” An exploration of
the under-studied phenomenon of long-distance relationships. In J. Wood & S.
Duck (Eds.), Under-studied relationships: Off the beaten track (pp. 173-196).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
6. LONG-DISTANCE RELATIONSHIPS +=a 139

Sahlstein, E. (1999, November). Presences, quasi-presences and absences: An investi-


gation of long-distance relational types. Paper presented at the annual conference of
the National Communication Association, Chicago.
Schwebel, A., Dunn, R., Moss, B., & Renner, M. (1992). Factors associated with rela-
tional stability in geographically separated couples. Journul of CoZZege Student Devel-
opment, 33, 222-230.
Shea, B., & Pearson, J. (1986). The effects of relationship type, partner intent, and gen-
der on the selection of maintenance strategies. Communication Monographs, 53,
334-364.
Stafford, L., & Canary, D. (1991). Maintenance strategies and romantic relationship
type, gender, and relational characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 8, 217-242.
Stafford, L., & Reske, J. (1990). Idealization and communication in long-distance pre-
marital relationships. Family Relations, 39, 274-279.
Stafford, L., Daley, J., & Reske, J. (1987, N ovember). The effects of romantic relation-
ships on friendship networks. Paper presented at the annual conference of the
Speech Communication Association, Boston.
Stephen, T (1986). C ommunication and interdependence in geographically separated
relationships. Human Communication Research, 2 3, 191-2 10.
Westefeld, J., & Liddell, D. (1982). Coping with long-distance relationships. Journal of
College Student Personnel, 23, 5 SO-5 5 1.
Wilmot, WI, & Carbaugh, D. (1986). Long-distance lovers: Predicting the dissolution of
relationships. Journal of Northwest Communication Association, 14, 43-59.
This page intentionally left blank
Compute Mediated
Communication IEFfects
on Relationship
I Formation
an d Maintenance

Michael K. Rabby
The University of Central Florida
Joseph B. Walther
Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute

ethnology affects lives in both predictable and unpredictable


ways. When one considers the influence that computers have (and will
have) on the way people engage in relationships, it is important to under-
stand that people are just starting to assimilate them into their daily lives.
People have only begun to scratch the surface of the extent to which com-
puters will affect their lives and the relationships within them.
Today, computer-mediated communication (CMC) has infiltrated a va-
riety of aspects of lives. People use it to communicate with each
other at work (Constant, Sproull, & Kiesler, 1997), in educational settings
(Brandon & Hollingshead, 1999), in friendships (Parks & Floyd, 1996),
and in romantic relationships (Merkle & Richardson, 2000). Negroponte
(1995) made perhaps the strongest claim for the importance of one type of
CMC, electronic mail (e-mail),
. < when he asserted that it will be the domi-
nant interpersonal telecommunications medium, approaching if not over-
142 -I+=+ RABBYAND WALTHER

shadowing voice within the next 15 years. Yet communication via e-mail
seems exotic to those just discovering it, banal to those who have used it for
a while, and acts as a lifeline to those who use it on a daily basis to commu-
nicate with others at work and home.
Although e-mail has not achieved the universality that the telephone
has, the signs point to a similar pattern of assimilation. For example, people
presumed both would have dehumanizing effects at first, but that has not
proven to be the case. As Mitchell (1995) noted, “Telephony did not re-
place face-to-face contact.. . . Rather, it created a new form of contact; it
extended and redefined the sphere of interaction and cohabitation” (p.
35). Scholars are only starting to construct the introduction in the tome of
research on CMC. As with the telephone, early theories about CMC, such
as the cues-filtered-out approaches (Culnan & Markus, 1987), assumed
that CMC would be less socially oriented and personal then face-to-face
communication. However, this has not proven true. In fact, communica-
tion via the Internet often reaches levels referred to as hyperpersond com-
munication: communication that is more intimate and sociable than that
found in equivalent, offline interactions (Walther, 1996).
With the movement toward increasing use, CMC has proven to be a valu-
able tool for many people to initiate, develop, and maintain relationships.
CMC offers people a venue through which they can meet new people with
whom they share similar interests (Rintel & Pittam, 1997). Often, people
meet and form relationships online that hold great importance in their lives
(Turkle, 1995). CMC h as also provided a new forum for people to maintain
previously established relationships with friends and family (Rabby, 1997).
This chapter focuses on the ways that CMC affects personal relation-
ships. We will review how CMC affords new ways to meet people to form
relationships, and affects the way they come to know one another. The
manner. in which relationships develop is subject to some similarities and
some differences than the trajectories of face-to-face dynamics, which
some research has described, although not in ways that entirely fit with a
focus on personal relations. Finally, this chapter examines how CMC may
be used in the maintenance of ongoing relationships-its role and
evaluation of its potency-and in social support online.

TYPES OF- CMC

Computer-mediated communication represents the wide spectrum of me-


dia that use some form of digital encoding, and in many ways, it is a misno-
mer to discuss CMC as a unifying concept. Most studies exploring CMC
focus on a single aspect or context, such as newsgroups (Parks & Floyd,
1996), Multi-User Dungeons, or MUDS (Curtis, 1997; Utz, 2000),
Multi-User Dungeons, Object-Oriented, or MOOS (Parks & Roberts,
7. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE VIA CMC ++ 14f,

1998), e-mail (Stafford, Kline, & Dimmick, 1999), and Internet Relay Chat
or IRC (Rintel & Pittam, 1997). One distinction that can be made among
these forms is that some are asynchronous and others are synchronous.
Asynchronous forms are those in which the message sender and message
reader are not online at the same time, where there is some kind of
store-and-forward capacity. The most common of these is e-mail, a comput-
erized letter delivered instantly to another person at the choosing,
but that is read by the receiver at his or her convenience. Usenet
Newsgroups are a second type of asynchronous channel. These consist of a
series of electronic bulletin boards that enable people to post messages on a
wide variety of topics, which can be read by as many as thousands of other
people. To communicate on Usenet, instead of communicating to specified
addressees, one posts a message to the topic, that is, for anybody who can ac-
cess it in a public forum. Other types of asynchronous bulletin boards are be-
coming common via Web sites, for facilitating group discussion.
Synchronous, or real-time CMC, includes chat rooms and MUDS/
MOOS. Like newsgroups, chatrooms remain open to anybody who can ac-
cess them, may be topically organized, and are text-based. Chatrooms ap-
pear in a variety of forms, such as IRC and in proprietary systems like
America Online. As a user types a message on the screen, and sends it to
the system, the message appears to all others connected to the same virtual
space. MUDS are game-oriented participatory chats, with rich text-based
descriptions of rooms and scenes. They feature programmable objects
(represented in text), and text-based descriptions each player selects as
users compete or socialize with one another. MOOS, similarly, feature de-
scriptions of architecture and decor, as well as other players, although
MOOS tend to be more oriented to socializing than game playing.
Instant messaging services represent a recent addition to the family of
synchronous CMC media. Popularized by the ICQ network and the AOL
Instant Messenger service, instant messaging combines features of e-mail
and chatrooms. Instant messaging is frequently used as a one-to-one me-
dium. Like a chatroom, communication occurs in real time if both parties
are present. Instant messaging lets users know instantly when messages ap-
pear for them, allowing them to respond to partners immediately. It also
lets one know who among friends are online at a given time.

DlSTlNGUlSHlNG I=EATURE Ol= CMC

Not all of these types of CMC are the same, although research has paid insuf-
ficient attention to the effects that the differences among them may have (see
Nass & Mason, 1990). One distinction mentioned earlier is the degree of syn-
chrony and asynchrony. Although more research is needed, one study has
found, insofar as relational communication is concerned, that synchronous
I++ += RABBYAND WALTHER

CMC and face-to-face discussion resemble one another, whereas asynchron-


ous CMC, and (asynchronous) paper-and-pencil exchange resemble each
other, in their sensitivity to relationship longevity and other factors (Walther,
2001). Another distinction exists in the audience of the message. In e-mail,
the user controls who will receive the message (at least at first), knowing who
will read the message. Other forms of CMC blur the line between interper-
sonal and mass media. Newsgroups allow the sender to post to a variety a
board that anyone who has access to it can read. The audience is limitless.
Finally, CMC forms differ from each other-and from Face-to-Face
(FtF) communication-in the extent to which they reveal the user. Most
CMC uses text-based messages, without the nonverbal signals and involun-
tary messages people emit while in the physical presence of another. The an-
onymity and simplicity of text-based messages hold a great appeal to many
people (Cutlip, Friedman, & Wolicki, 1996), and as discussed later, have im-
portant implications for the relational dynamics of online interaction. Other
forms of CMC, however, such as on the Activeworlds site, allow the user to
select an avatar, a graphic object that represents the user. Avatars can take
the form of people, animals, and robots. They move in a simulated
three-dimensional environment (on the computer screen) that people can
create and manipulate. This form gives users a sense of nonverbal space and
placement. Users can also manipulate their avatars to make nonverbal mo-
tions such as waving, kicking, dancing, and jumping. In all of these cases,
there is more or less of a digital barricade: The user is not shown as she or he
appears in real life, and the presentation of self online may be highly selec-
tive (if not, in some cases, downright deceptive).
Invariably, differences in the way people develop and maintain relation-
ships may exist between these technical characteristics, and the social con-
texts with which they are associated (Roberts, Smith, & Pollack, 1996).
With that in mind, the remainder of this chapter focuses on the primary
theories that speak directly to forming and maintaining relationships on
the Internet, unique characteristics of CMC, and future directions for
studying the impacts of CMC on ongoing relationships.

CMC AND VIRTUAL RELATIONSHIPS

The first role that CMC serves in relationships is that of a medium for ini-
tial exchange. Although many relationships that begin online migrate
offline, the majority of the research in this area has explored entirely pure
virtual relationships. Some scholars such as Wellman (1994) have explored
the possibilities of strong communities rising up in the wake of online inter-
action. Wellman et al. (1996) suggested that the Internet has replaced the
semiprivate meeting spaces of coffee shops, cafes, pubs, and parks. Now,
people interface with each other entirely in private. Entire communities
7. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE VIA CMC ++t- 143

have formed as a byproduct of the Internet and other types of CMC (e.g.
Baym, 1997; Curtis, 1997; Parks & Roberts, 1998; Sproull & Faraj, 1997).
Within these communities, personal relationships frequently emerge.

Meeting People is Easy

The Internet provides fertile ground for starting relationships. Anecdotal


accounts of spontaneous friendship development are noted in some of the
earliest, pre-Internet CMC studies (e.g., Johansen, DeGrasse, & Wilson,
1978). Because people often encounter each messages within
well-defined topical discussions, there is a well-founded presumption of
similarity. According to Rheingold (199 1) :

In a virtual community we can go directly to the place where our favorite sub-
jects are being discussed, then get acquainted with people who share our passions
. . . Your chances of making friends are magnified by orders of magnitude over the
old methods of finding a peer group. (p. 27)

Willie Nelson fans find other fans on alt.music.willienelson. Movie fans


can gossip about the latest news rumors in the forum pages on the
Web site. People having difficulties with depression can
gather in a variety of places to share and compare experiences. Within this
context, it is easy to take note of attitudes, style, wit, and experi-
ences in a manner that one finds attractive. As Baker (1998) reported from
her interviews with romantic couples who first met online, “What did they
like about each other right from the start? Sense of humor, response time,
interests, qualities described online, and writing style were prominent,
along with having ‘something in Thus, people meet in the same
manner that many people in FtF do-they initiate an interaction because
they have something in common.
However, unlike FtF first meetings, these commonalities and verbal im-
pressions are all that communicators have to go by. Rather than filter
through physically borne indicators of age, physical attractiveness, vocal
characteristics and behaviors, and other ephemera that are part and parcel
of FtF first impressions, currency online may be, as Rice (1987) sug-
gested, the quality of information and the wit with which one pres-
ents it. Some of these relationships are fleeting, and last the duration of a
conversation. Others develop into friendships and romantic relationships.
Everywhere on the Internet, people can meet and form relationships.
Research has documented the course of such relationship developments
in several online arenas. Parks and Floyd (1996), for instance, surveyed
528 users from 24 Usenet newsgroups, receiving 176 responses. They
found that 60.7% had formed a personal relationship with someone they
had met through a newsgroup. More women than men had formed a rela-
146 -es RABBYAND WALTHER

tionship in this manner (72.2% vs. 54.5%). The frequency of contributions


to the newsgroups helped predict the relationship formation, again indi-
cating the importance of familiarity with the medium. They further dis-
covered that prospective partners first note each postings in a
newsgroup, then send each other private e-mail; they often call one an-
other on the phone after that, and many then send photographs. A minor-
ity, but a significant proportion, go on to meet one another FtF. These
relationships, according to the research, were as intimate in several dimen-
sions as people commonly experienced in parallel FtF relationships.
Parks and Roberts (1998) extended this research in the context of
MOOS. In contrast to the static, asynchronous messages posted in
newsgroups, MOOS feature synchronous communication. Like the
newsgroup users, the MOO users migrated to other media after getting to
know the other person online. MOO users also reported longer duration of
their relationships. Nearly all of the MOO respondents (93.6%) had
formed an ongoing interpersonal relationship during their MOO interac-
tions. Furthermore, 90% of them had moved that relationship to another
channel in addition to the MOO. Parks and Roberts (1998) speculated that
MOOS are more dynamic and social in nature; unlike newsgroups, MOOS
do not have a set topic. Many MOOS seem to have meeting and socializing,
rather than information exchange, as their primary purpose. Utz (2000)
found, in contrast, that the likelihood of forming a relationship within a
MUD (game-playing) environment depended in part on the expec-
tation that such relationships could be supported by CMC, echoing Jacob-
(1999) findings that some people join MOOS for the technical
challenges, whereas others gravitate to the social challenges.
Another study focused on romantic couples who had met online and
successfully maintained their relationships offline (Baker, 1998). A major-
ity of these couples had first met in some topical discussion group or game
site, rather than in electronic venues devoted to finding partners. The sam-
ple in this study included only couples who did have online-to-offline rela-
tionships, so it is hard to know how critical a factor this is in virtual
relationship formation; as in offline relationships, there are a million rea-
sons not to like someone whom one meets under favorable circumstances.

Communication Dynamics in O-dine Relationships

The presence of online relationship formation is, on the one hand, no sur-
prise; Parks and Floyd (1996) concluded that the Internet provides just an-
other place to meet and talk, like so many venues in the offline world. On
the other hand, it is still a curious idea that people may develop relation-
ships in an environment in which they cannot see, hear, or verify the physi-
cal existence of their partners. Although not theoretically necessary for
7. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE VIA CMC 3t. 147

relationships to form (Lea & Spears, 1996), physical exposure and


co-presence are so common a precursor to conversation and relationship
initiation that making friends or falling in love without physically meeting
partner is conventionally unthinkable. The question arises, how do
people get to know one another online, and do the mechanisms by which
they do so affect their relationships in unusual ways?
One approach to this process has been articulated in Social Informa-
tion Processing theory (Walther, 1992). This theory assumes that peo-
desires to get to know one another are not thwarted by the lack of
nonverbal cues, but rather, should nonverbal cues be lacking, people
adapt to the medium they have in order to exchange the information they
need to develop impressions. The content, timing, and style of the verbal
message affects impressions, and should they be motivated to
escalate them, their relationships. However, when using text-based com-
munication-especially asynchronously-more time is needed for infor-
mation to exchange and attributions to form. Research has documented
that online interpersonal impressions are slower to form than FtF
groups, but given sufficient time, the depth of these impressions is no less
in CMC than among FtF partners in the end (Walther, 1993). Moreover,
levels of relational intimacy, trust, and informality also accrue similarly in
CMC as in FtF groups over time (Walther & Burgoon, 1992). Other re-
search has found that relational intimacy can start off quite high, when
partners anticipate that for some reason they are likely to continue their
interactions over time, rather than have a chance meeting, never to com-
municate again (Walther, 1994, 1997).
Detailed analyses of how CMC users get to know one another reveals
that the mechanisms they have at their disposal might predispose them
to intimate communication. Tidwell and Walther (2000) examined the
uncertainty reduction strategies available to CMC users and the ways
they were used. FtF partners, on first encounter, have a variety of means
to find out about one another and tend to use the most polite, or unob-
trusive, means to do so-passive observation, and seeing how prospec-
tive partners interact with the physical and social environment (Berger,
Gardner, Parks, Schulman, & Miller, 1976; Berger & Kellermann,
1983). Over e-mail, however, such passive means are unavailable, and
communicators must turn to more obtrusive methods-asking personal
questions, and self-disclosure. In a lab experiment, CMC and FtF pairs
engaged in first meetings, and their comments were recorded and ana-
lyzed. CMC partners exchanged proportionately more self-disclosures
and questions than did FtF partners. Moreover, the questions they
asked were about more personal topics than those that FtF partners ex-
changed. At the same time, the deeper the disclosures and questions
used by partners in CMC, the more effective they were rated by their
partners, in comparison to those who met in FtF discussions. CMC us-
148 -I++ RABBYAND WALTHER

ers have few alternatives when it comes to finding out about each other,
aside from such interrogatives and self-disclosures, although doing so
nevertheless involves a greater degree of intimacy than FtF strangers
have reason to develop. These initial, innocuous exchanges may be the
foundation for the intensity that some online relationships acquire.
Despite the differences in the how people initially meet, if the relational
partners feel close enough to each other they will use a variety of media and
even FtF contact to maintain their relationships. In fact, the development
of relationships online may simply be temporally retarded in comparison
to FtF relationship development. At this point, research on CMC as both a
primary means of communication, and as a supplement, starts to overlap.

SPK.ll=lC COMMUNICATION STKATlEGItES

Given the shifting dynamics of CMC, it is not surprising that the com-
munication strategies that people use in CMC also vary. The one consis-
tent feature of all types of CMC is the lack of formal rules. Some
informal rules do hold throughout a variety of CMC. For example, when
a person shouts (WRITING EVERYTHING IN ALL CAPITAL LET-
TERS), it is said to indicate anger. However, other communication be-
haviors do not carry such universal meanings, such as writing in all
lower-case letters. To some, this indicates laziness and powerlessness.
In other cases, such as between two close friends, it translates more pos-
itively. Likewise, the well-known group of emoticons, or typed-out,
sideways representations of facial expressions, are almost universally
described as functioning like nonverbal behavior and substituting for
the comparative lack of nonverbal cues that is part of CMC. Recent re-
search has found, however, that despite highly consensual recognition
among CMC users of the semantic meanings associated with several
emoticons, they have very little syntactic effect on message interpreta-
tion. That is, in combination with affectively valenced verbal messages,
they do not consistently add positive or negative meaning. Rather, a
negativity effect obtains: A negatively-valenced verbal message or a neg-
ative (frowning) emoticon skews message interpretation negatively,
whereas positive emoticons (smiles, winks) have no combinatorial ef-
fect (Walther & 2001). Other language and cue variations
that have potency in the offline world also resonate online. For instance,
Selfe and Meyer (199 1) d emonstrated that the linguistic patterns tradi-
tionally associated with power and status are also conveyed in
text-based CMC. Adkins and Brashers (1995) demonstrated that such
powerful versus powerless speech variations affect interpersonal im-
pressions online. Users of powerless language (operationalized as
7. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE VIA CMC +=a- 14?

hedges, qualifiers, and tag questions) were perceived as less credible, at-
tractive, and persuasive than users of powerful language.
Different politeness, or face-saving strategies are also present in online
discussions (Hiemstra, 1982). Witt, Wheeless, Reyna, and Swigger (2000)
found that variations in verbal immediacy corresponded to ratings of
conversational effectiveness. The time of day at which messages
are sent, and the speediness of replies interact with message content to af-
fect the perceived dominance/submissiveness and intimacy of e-mail ex-
changes walther & Tidwell, 1995). F or instance, an affectionate e-mail
message sent after normal business hours conveys greater intimacy than it
does as if sent during the day, whereas an e-mail task request sent at night
conveys more dominance than a daytime task request.
Perhaps the most distinctive differences in the ways that people com-
municate occur in asynchronous communication and synchronous com-
munication. In asynchronous types of CMC, the user has the
opportunity to carefully construct the message and can even edit and
change it at well before sending it. Thus, e-mail messages are frequently
well organized and contemplated. At other times, they have sentences
that would sound appropriate orally but not written. As one sender
wrote in an e-mail: “On Monday Bob and I are going to the Bush concert
then on Thursday I turn 21 but I really wait for the semester to be
over.” This reflects the conversational tone that many messages take.
Ferrara, Brunner, and Whittemore (1991) identified the tone of CMC
as featuring an emergent register- a hybrid form of language between
spoken and written prose.
In synchronous communication, the rules vary even more. People often
talk in fragments, and ignore punctuation except when they want to indi-
cate emphasis. Rintel and Pittam (1998) published several excerpts from
dialogues in a public IRC room, and discovered numerous instances of this.
In this excerpt, they show an IRC member exiting an interaction:

Big Bunny: well, i got to go . . .


Big Bunny: lots of work to do . . .
Reaper: big: email me someday or something
Big Bunny: email me first
Reaper: big: will do
Big Bunny: then respond to ya (p. 525)

As this example illustrates, the participants ignore conventional gram-


mar, and instead concentrate on firing off quick fragments of dialogue. Un-
like the monologues frequently featured in asynchronous interactions, this
interaction demonstrates the dynamic style frequented by users of syn-
chronous mediated communication.
120 -ts+ RABBYAND WALTHER

Other research has revealed that exaggerated intimacy can become part
of the fun of online interaction. Just as flaming may become a norm in some
online groups (Lea, Fung, & Spears, 1992), so may signals of affec-
tion. In the groups studied in Walther and (1992) research, it be-
came common for one group to sign each message with the and
signifying hugs and kisses; another group signed, “Love, Kara,” or whatever
their names were. Although clearly jocular in tone, no such jocular affec-
tion was exchanged in parallel FtF groups. The question arises as to what
effect, if any, these stylistic differences have on relationships. In other
words, how does this tendency to exaggerate emotional messages in CMC
influence the relationship? Although research still awaits, it seems reason-
able to expect some effect. The major theories that describe relational ef-
fects of CMC are consistent with this expectation.

~ociaI I&rrnation and Deindivuduation (SIDE) Theory

The SIDE theory of CMC (Lea & Spears, 1992; Spears & Lea, 1994) ex-
amines the development of relationships online not as interpersonal ones,
but as social relationships. This distinction, which is often overlooked in re-
lationships research (cf. Sanders, 1997), is a fundamental one in SIDE. Lea
and Spears defined interpersonal cues as those cues that distinguish one
person from another. Such cues are most apparent in FtF interaction visu-
ally; that is, when we see another person, it is immediately apparent that
the target person is individually different from oneself. Because CMC is, in
most cases, visually anonymous-that is, it does not present visual, identi-
fying cues-CMC users can become deindivuated online. The
deindividuation dynamic interacts with whatever identity may be most sa-
lient to a communicator. Whether that identity is role-based or based on
some salient social category (e.g., both students, both Dutch, etc.), if it is a
social rather than an individually oriented identity, we experience greater
attraction to others who share that identity. On the other hand, when the
salient identity is individualistic-one is aware of onesself as an individual
and is looking for individual differences in others online-the
deindividuation dynamic is muted, or even leads to dislike or disparage-
ment of the other (since we generally like similar and dislike dissimilar oth-
ers). Moreover, when a common social identity is active, CMC
participants more closely adhere to the norms of the group, and value those
who reciprocate those norms.
The SIDE approach has been used to explain how CMC participants,
especially in groups (and more especially where there is an outgroup as
well as an ingroup) become attracted to one another. It is important to
note, however, that this attraction is considered to be social in nature-so-
cial attraction-rather than interpersonal attraction: One is just as at-
tracted to any member of the group, and the members are essentially
7. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE VIA CMC =a 131

equivalent and substitutable. This attraction, however, may be (falsely)


perceived to be personal in nature-an illusory reflection of interpersonal
love-by the perceiver. Falsely, because it is based in social instead of in-
terpersonal cues. To detect interpersonal differences, even within an at-
traction cycle, would be to undermine and potentially dismantle the very
dynamic that led to the attraction. The theory has also been applied to neg-
ative affect in CMC-flaming (i.e., hostile comments, swearing, and in-
sults)-quite effectively. SIDE theory classifies flaming as a behavior that
can come to be valued in an online group, if that group is cohering along
SIDE principles. Should that behavior become normative, it should be re-
ciprocated by group members, and further valued. Thus a flame war may
become quite common in some CMC associations, yet most uncommon in
others, depending on the norms of the group.

H~perpersona~ CMC

Using salient social identities as a starting point that can lead to an elec-
tronic personal relationship, the Hyperpersonal Perspective (Walther,
1996) draws together several theories to explain how online relations
may become particularly intense and intimate. Acknowledging SIDE
theory, it is expected that users make overattributions about their online
partners, and when facilitating conditions are present (e.g., expected fu-
ture interaction, and some perceived similarity), users “fill in the blanks”
in desirable ways, interpreting messages favorably and constructing com-
mensurate impressions of online partners. When creating messages,
CMC users are posited to engage in selective self-presentation. A wired
variant of normal, offline impression management, the selectivity of
CMC affords communicators even greater leverage than FtF interaction
does. Online one can present oneself as one wishes, withholding or re-
vealing what they want, when they want. Moreover, users may refocus
their cognitive efforts to the task of writing, ignoring the ambient stimuli,
turn-taking, physical self-monitoring, and other tasks that accompany
FtF communication. The channel allows them to stop and choose
phrases, and to edit and rewrite in a way that FtF interaction does not.
Finally, the reciprocal influences of idealized perception and selective
presentation may create a self-confirming prophecy among sender and
receiver, leading to unexpected reward and intensity. This perspective
has received some confirmation in educational and group settings (e.g.,
Chester & Gwynne, 1998; Walther, 1997), and its approach suggests that
it should pertain in dyadic personal relations, albeit empirical verification
in the latter domain still awaits.
A recent test in the context of groups shows promise across domains,
however. Walther, Slovacek, and Tidwell(2001) examined whether CMC
152 +s+ RABBYAND WALTHER

partners sustained greater intimacy and attraction when they got to know
each other over time through electronic text alone, or whether photo-
graphs of their partners either helped or hindered their affinity. Reasoning
that short-term partners needed a head start but that long-term partners
would achieve hyperpersonality via text, half of some long-term groups
and short-term groups were shown photos of one another prior to an online
discussion, whereas the other half of the long-term and short-term groups
saw only text. Results revealed that the short-term partners achieved
greater intimacy and attraction with a photo but that the long-term part-
ners had less. Overall, the greatest affinity was achieved among those
long-term partners who never saw each other. The old aphorism that “a
picture is worth a thousand words” seems not to be the case when it comes
to relationships online.
Although CMC may be surprisingly useful for forming intimacy on-
line, CMC may not seem as useful to those for whom relationships origi-
nate offline. There appears to be a self-serving bias in the evaluation of
CMC as a method to get to know someone, based on where the relation-
ship began. In unpublished research by Dodds, Frost, Knudson, Smith,
and Thompson (1995), a questionnaire was posted for members of an
electronic discussion list for persons in long-distance relationships who
used CMC to keep in touch with their partners. Thirty participants re-
plied, 44% of whom were male, and 56%, female. Two-thirds of the par-
ticipants had first met their partners online, whereas the other third met
offline; all used e-mail as the primary method of communication with
their partners at the time of the study. The way in which the relationship
started had significant effects on evaluation of CMC and
their beliefs about relationships. Those who met their partners via the
Internet more strongly agreed with the statement, “on-line relationships
feel just as real as relationships I have had off-line,” than did those who
met offline. Likewise, they felt more strongly that “it is good that e-mail
offers the opportunity of getting to know character before any
physical involvement, ” than did those who met conventionally. Those
who met online were more likely to disagree with the statement, “you
cannot realistically say that you love someone who you have never met in
real life.” Ironically, the origination bias seemed to drop when asked
about the potential for misunderstanding: It was those who had met FtF
who more strongly agreed with the statement, “there is more opportu-
nity for misunderstanding in the physical presence of a loved one than
there is via E-mail.” These responses suggest that the hyperpersonal at-
traction potential of CMC may not be universal, but tightly bounded to
strictly or originally virtual relationships. To the extent that it brings
added dimensions to the maintenance of existing relationships is unclear.
At the same time, the message management aspects of CMC appeal to
those in a variety of relational contexts (see also Walther & Boyd, 2002).
7. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE VIA CMC -+ 153

CMC AS A SUPPLEMENT:
RW-ATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE

These theories demonstrate the various implications of CMC for forming


relationships. But CMC generally serves one of two capacities in a relation-
ship. In most cases, people use it as a supplement to FtF conversation. It
functions alongside phone calls, letters, and FtF interactions to keep the
relationship going. What does a relationship maintained using CMC look
like? Although relationships that begin online may feature different bases
of attraction or evolution than do FtF relationships, research is mixed with
respect to whether maintenance behaviors really differ much from the
standard repertory of maintenance behaviors discovered in other contexts.
CMC serves as a supplemental medium that allows relational partners
familiar to each other in a variety of contexts to stay in touch. As men-
tioned earlier, e-mail represents the most popular type of CMC. Friends,
family members, and romantic partners use e-mail as a means of staying in
touch with each other between face-to-face meetings and phone calls.
E-mail messages (and other types of CMC for that matter) facilitate rela-
tional maintenance on three different levels. First, the simple act of send-
ing a message helps keep the relationship in existence. It lets the other
relational partner know that he or she is in the other mind. Sec-
ond, each message can be considered as an attempt at openness. In an
e-mail message, the author tells the reader about something that is on his or
her mind. At a third level, one can look at the function of this openness, or
what the communication act appears to accomplish. In other words, the
purpose of each message (or the sentences and phrases therein) can be ana-
lyzed to determine its maintenance strategy.
E-mail messages often contain statements that hint at the importance of
nonvirtual contact. Indications of past contact (“oh, by the way, i just
wanted to tell you that i am really proud of how mature you were last night
when I told you about this guy . . . “) as well as future contact (“Maybe I can
even meet you at the train station. talk to you about it more tonight”)
abound (Rabby, 1997). Th e issue is not if CMC helps to maintain relation-
ships (it does). Instead, more pertinent questions reside in the types of be-
haviors that people exhibit there.
One glimpse into the maintenance communication partners exchange on-
line is found in a content analysis of e-mail messages among ongoing couples
(Rabby, 1997). The study employed Canary, Stafford, Hause, and
(1993) typology to explore relational maintenance strategies exhibited in
electronic mail messages. That typology included the categories positivity,
openness, assurances, shared tasks, social networks, joint activities, cards/let-
ters/calls, avoidance, antisocial, humor, and miscellaneous. A twelfth cate-
gory, narratives, was added to the typology to indicate the sharing of stories
124 ++ RABBYAND WALTHER

and objective descriptions of everyday life. Duck (1994) suggested that


these day-to-day, banal conversations bind relationships together.
With the addition of the category of narratives, 100% of the data could
be categorized as a specific relational maintenance strategy, with less than
0.1% coded as miscellaneous. Overwhelmingly, the messages contained
openness (38% of the total thought units) and narratives (22% of the total
thought units). In essence, both of these categories involved the sharing of
self, with openness covering the subjective observations and narra-
tives covering the objective observations. Openness includes subcategories
such as self-disclosure, metarelational communication, advice, and opinion
expression. Narratives includes generic mentions of topics such as school,
work, going to the store, and other routine activities.
Perhaps the most interesting results come from the maintenance strate-
gies not used. The messages contained only a small percentage of negative
thought units (avoidance had 1.7%, antisocial had O.O%, and negative hu-
mor had 0.5%). These low frequencies exist largely because of the nature
of the medium studied. To compose a message to another person contra-
dicts the intention of avoiding him or her or being antisocial. Although
these behaviors are negotiated both directly and indirectly in face-to-face
interactions, relational partners negotiate them indirectly via e-mail, usu-
ally by responding infrequently or not at all. In a few instances, individuals
used the nature of the technology as an excuse for avoidance. “I
checked my e-mail in a long time” and “I never received that message” have
become common excuses in our day and age. Another report of this behav-
ior can be seen in the following excerpt from the data sample:

[ G]uess what I got in the mail yesterday? it was an easter card from me to richard
in san francisco. it was return to sender - wrong address. that really sucks. i re-
cently asked him if his address was still the same and he told me it was. i try to
keep in touch and look what happens? i think i am going to write you any-
more either . . . just kidding?

Simply put, people avoid each other by not communicating, which then
signals avoidance and antisocialness.
This emphasis on the positive, proactive relational maintenance strate-
gies also comports with (1986) study, which suggests that peo-
ple tend to idealize their long-distance relationships. The potential for the
distanciation imposed by CMC has a similar effect, at least in personal rela-
tionships. When one communicates largely through e-mail he or she loses
the sense of that bad manners, slow speech, frequent cursing, and
other undesirable habits.
In essence, the content of these messages held no real surprises. In these
cases the messages used in CMC resembled the messages used in FtF inter-
actions. The relational partners know each other, they have already formed
7. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE VIA CMC -+t- 135

an impression of their relational partner, and they typically continue to see


that person in other contexts. Stafford et al. (1999), among others, made
the call to consider CMC as a part of interpersonal relationships, rather
than a false dichotomy between the mediated and the interpersonal. In-
deed, in these relationships CMC simply functions as a tool for relational
maintenance. At the same time, using taxonomies from traditional interac-
tion to analyze electronic exchanges may hide whether novel strategies or
new permutations of maintenance messages may evolve with the medium,
and future research must be alert to such changes. In any case, the differ-
ences in the way people maintain relationships emerges when CMC is the
primary point of contact for the participants.
One indication that CMC alters relationship maintenance is seen in the re-
sults of a recent large-scale survey by the Pew Charitable Trust (2000). Find-
ings indicate that many Americans-women particularly-use the Internet
quite regularly to stay in touch with family members and with friends. Among
the findings are that women feel they can be more honest and direct
with family members online than they would via FtF or telephone communi-
cation, no doubt due to the digital barricade, and the message management as-
pects of the hyperpersonal dynamics previously described.
A final perspective on the role of CMC in relational maintenance is seen
in research by Gunn and Gunn (2000), w h o compared the effects of using
CMC versus other forms of contact on existing long-distance relationships.
Their results indicated that those who used CMC to stay in touch with their
partners reported greater love and closeness to their partners, and less rela-
tionship insecurity than those who did not. Although Gunn and Gunn sug-
gested that communication frequency (a factor supported by CMC) is the
key variable underlying these dynamics (a factor supported by CMC), it
would not be surprising to find a hyperpersonal aspect within the findings as
well (i.e. an aspect by which online partnerships are more intimate than par-
allel offline relationships): CMC users also disclosed more to their online
partners. Gunn and Gunn reported that “people who were online preferred
their long-distance relationships to their local (unmediated) relationships,
whereas people who were not online preferred their local relationships to
their long-distance (letter or telephone based) relationships” (p. 2).

CONCLUSIONS
Although scholars have only begun to explore relationships that use CMC
as well as the influence it has on relational maintenance strategies, some in-
formation about the nature of CMC interactions and their contributions to
relational maintenance emerges.
First, from one perspective, despite some unique features, there is re-
ally nothing radical about CMC. It simply offers people another opportu-
156 -I+- RABBYAND WALTHER

nity to meet and communicate with others. Given that CMC can often
represent simply another context in which people can maintain their rela-
tionships, it is not too surprising that behaviors do not deviate much from
the behaviors exhibited in other contexts. CMC might be more notable for
its banality than anything else. Very often, the messages contain recount-
ing of daily life, such as in this example: “I am going to one of con-
certs tonight. He plays in the Blue Band and some concert band. Anyways,
the concert is outside and if it rains, which it is right now, we have to
go. Other than that, everything is same old-same old.”
Most CMC interactions in developed relationships tend to involve mi-
nor issues. The evidence thus far indicates that major relational events
(e.g., conflict) are usually reserved for other types of media and FtF inter-
actions.
Yet in other respects, CMC turns relationship processes upside down. It
is conventionally inconceivable to start a conversation with a stranger be-
fore having seen the person, at least without some telecommunication.
Well-known theories about impression and relationship formation such as
Uncertainty Reduction Theory (Berger & Calabrese, 1975) are premised
in face-to-face contact. The potency of interpersonal revelations and dis-
coveries -whether in new or existing relationships-may take greater
weight online without the usual nonverbal mechanisms to buffer them.
And such surprises may be just as likely constitute enticements as frustra-
tions, as seen in this e-mail message captured midway through a virtual stu-
dent project in Walther (1997):

Hey Mayte, I just wanted to thank you for taking the time to compile the reading list
for everyone. By the way, this may sound crazy, but you a guy? We tell
from your name; the “Oliver” part looks masculine, but the “bel” nickname could be
feminine. Sorry for such an offensive question. I guess that when you mentioned
naked, I just stand the suspense any longer. Erica (p. 365)

The adjustment that people make to these ambiguous situations will re-
main a phenomenon to track as researchers continue to unfurl the dynam-
ics of interpersonal relationships.
The specific roles that CMC play in relationships continue to increase
and change. The literature thus far has barely scratched the surface of the
specific role that e-mail and other forms of CMC play in relationships.
Stafford et al. (1999) reported that people use e-mail from home for four
primary reasons: interpersonal relationships, gratification opportunities,
personal gain (e.g., learning and information exchange), and business rea-
sons. Participants in (1997) study noted the advantages of using
e-mail over the telephone. They suggested that the low-cost of the me-
dium was the greatest advantage (52.9%). Other popular answers included
convenience (17.9%), the ease with which it allows one to keep in touch
7. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE VIA CMC 3L

with people (8.8%), and the ability to manipulate text (5.9%). It is not the
characteristics of the medium that people consider an advantage of using
the medium. Instead, the atheoretical and pragmatic reasons of low-cost
and convenience hold the appeal for most.
The precedent of these pragmatic reasons over preferential reasons is
also visible when researchers have investigated what people would
choose if price was no object. With all things being equal, people tend to
indicate they will choose as rich a medium as possible. Sellen (1995)
noted this when she compared satisfaction with long-distance meetings
conducted using audio and visual communication versus audio-only
equipment. She found that, although both were relatively equivalent in
terms of how people handled interactions when using them, participants
felt the video component was important for conversation, and given a
choice, would choose to include it.
Despite how people say they feel, actual usage may depart from such ide-
als. In other domains of CMC research, participants also consistently rate
face-to-face, or at least the telephone, superior to text-based messages for
interpersonally involving encounters. Yet such findings are consistent only
amongprojective tests, that is, questionnaire studies that ask respondents to
indicate what medium would be best to use, given a full range of choices for
every situation (Rice, 1993). Studies that actually observe media selection in
organizations almost never support what the projective tests suggest (e.g.
Fulk, Schmitz, & Ryu, 1995; Markus 1994a), and find instead that users se-
lect media opportunistically, or based on local social conventions that
emerge in specific relationships. Where a medium might be lean, they work
to make it richer (e.g. Markus, 1994b). M oreover, studies of personal media
such as the AT&T Picturephone (Noll, 1992) have concluded that, although
people like to see others, they do not like being seen, for routine communi-
cation. There seems to be a symbolic component to what media are best and
most personal-the more cues the better-which does not stand up to the
demands of the moment when people cannot be in each presence,
cannot be available to talk at the same time, or can not afford the gas, time,
airfare, or phone bill that would make such choices actionable.
Ironically, despite the avowed preference for higher-bandwidth media,
CMC may nevertheless contribute to more intimate and satisfying rela-
tionships than richer media or face-to-face conversations. Despite its
lower preference rating, the dynamics of CMC might improve relations,
perhaps unbeknownst to its users. It appears that people, aware of
shortcomings but less cognizant of its benefits, overaccommodate in stra-
tegic and highly coded ways the potential weaknesses of the medium.
Users, not systems, are what make a medium rich or lean.
The contemporary questions have to do with whether the unique prop-
erties of CMC enhance, diminish, or otherwise alter the dynamics of these
158 +a RABBYAND WALTHER

relationships through communication. Given the findings in this review,


the preliminary evidence of the role that CMC might play in relationships
indicates it to be a combination between the pragmatic and strategic. It re-
mains to be seen if e-mail becomes the dominant medium Negroponte
(1995) predicted, or simply remains a convenient option.

I=UTURE DIRKTIONS l=OR CMC


AND RIELATIONAL MAINTENANCE

Future research on relationship formation and relational maintenance in


CMC will reform not only the study of contemporary relationship dynam-
ics, but help to extend our understanding of CMC across a variety of do-
mains. Significant weaknesses exist in the dominant theories that describe
CMC relationships, especially insofar as relationship maintenance is con-
cerned. The SIDE and Hyperpersonal models both fit best when there is ex-
clusivity of channels. That is, each explains particularly well how people
form impressions of each other online and how users come to relate to each
other within the online environment. theoretical assumptions about
missing visual or personal cues, however, dictates that at the point where a
physical encounter occurs, the theory is no longer applicable. In the
Hyperpersonal model as well, reciprocation of flattering cues and relational
escalation are premised on advantageous exploitation of the selectivity that
text-only cues provides. Although purely online relationships may exist for a
time, many cross over into face-to-face encounters; and although many rela-
tionships begin off-line, they are maintained via the Internet.
These theories, although powerful within the boundaries of virtual rela-
tionships, do not explicitly address such movements across modalities.
This is not to say that they cannot pertain, but the extent to which their
theoretical dynamics may come into play at different stages deserves
greater attention. For instance, it may be that couples who have gone for
some time without physical exposure begin to idealize and hyper-
personalize; the results of Gunn and (2000) study of relational
maintenance online would seem to suggest this is so, as would
(1986) and Stafford and (1990) studies of geographically-
separated couples using conventional media. The applicability of these
dynamics as temporary states in ongoing relationships that are
sometimes mediated deserves further study. As such, the question of how
well these theories hold up when relational partners converse through both
mediated and FtF environments remains. Moreover, improvements in our
understanding of impacts in mixed modality romantic relation-
ships may also inform our study of use in other domains-educa-
tion, group work, and organizational relations-in which more and more
commonly people work together both online and offline.
7. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE VIA CMC * 159

We conclude with a cautionary note. We are often asked whether


CMC relationships, or whether the use of CMC in relationships, is help-
ful or harmful. In the extreme, we are asked whether CMC can really be
adequate for relating to one another, that is, is CMC rather than
face-to-face interaction a critical factor in the success or failure of rela-
tionships (i.e., for relationships to be successful, long lasting, or real). De-
spite the findings reviewed earlier, which suggest, at times, helpfulness or
harmfulness, we believe the process of maintaining relationships over
time is far too complex a process for CMC to shoulder too much of the
burden. We know too little about these media, not because, as some
would say, the media are changing so rapidly, but because the field is very
young and can barely keep up with the normative and innovative uses to
which millions of new users put them. More importantly, however, we
know too little about how people fall in and out of love, manage multiple
commitments, obligations and alternative attractions, and deal with their
relationships over time to predict the critical success factors for any rela-
tionship to succeed, mediated, face-to-face, or mixed. We caution read-
ers, before putting too much burden on CMC to make or break
relationships, to consider the baseline: Most relationships are temporary.
Rather than to look for the drastic changes media are unlikely to produce,
we urge the curious to look for the subtleties, the surprises, the delights,
and fluctuations promise to teach us little about media but a lot about
how humans pursue relationships, no matter how many miles of geogra-
phy or network connections lie between them.

Adkins, M., & Brashers, D. (1995). The power of language in computer-mediated


groups. Manugement Communication Quarterly, 8, 289-322.
Baker, A. (1998, July). Cyberspace couples finding romance online then meeting for
the first time in real life. CMC Magazine. Retrieved December 1, 2000, from:
http://www.december.com/cmc/mag 1998/jul/baker. html
Baym,-N. K. (1998). Interpreting soap operas and creating community: Inside an elec-
tronic fan culture. In S. Kiesler (Ed.), Culture of the Internet (pp. 103-l 20).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Berger, C. R., & Calabrese, R. J. (1975). S ome explorations in initial interaction and be-
yond: Toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication. Human
Communication Research, 1, 99-l 12.
Berger, C. R., Gardner, R. R., Parks, M. R., Schulman, L., &Miller, G. R. (1976). Inter-
personal epistemology and interpersonal communication. In G. R. Miller (Ed.), Ex-
plorations in interpersonal communication (pp. 149-l 7 1). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Berger, C. R., & Kellermann, K. A. (1983). T o ask or not to ask: Is that a question? In R. N.
Bostrom (Ed.), Communication Yearbook 7 (pp. 342-368). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Brandon, D. P, & Hollingshead, A. B. (1999). Collaborative learning and com-
puter-supported groups. Communication Education, 48, 109-l 26.
160 +a RABBYAND WALTHER

Canary, D. J., Stafford, L. S., Hause, K. S., &Wallace, L. A. (1993). An inductive analy-
sis of relational maintenance strategies: Comparisons among lovers, relatives,
friends, and others. Communication Research Reports, IO, 5-14.
Chester, A., & Gwynne, G. (1998). 0 n 1ine teaching: Encouraging collaboration through
anonymity. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 4(2). Retrieved Decem-
ber 1, 2000, from: http://www.ascusc.org/jcmc/vol4/issue2/chester.html
Constant, D., Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1997). Th e in d ness of strangers: On the use-
fulness of electronic weak ties for technical advice. In S. Kiesler (Ed.), Culture of the
Internet (pp. 303-322). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Culnan, M. J., & Markus, M. L. (1987). I n f ormation technologies. In F. M. Jablin, L. L.
Putnam, K. H. Roberts, & L. W Porter (Eds.),Handbook of organizational communi-
cation: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 420-443). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Curtis, P (1998). Mudding: S ocial phenomena in test-based virtual realities. In S.
Kiesler (Ed.), Culture of the Internet (pp. 121-142). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Cutlip, J ., Friedman, R., & Wolicki, J. S. (1996, J anuary 22). The joys and drawbacks of
e-mail. LegaZ Times, S32.
Dodds, D., Frost, K., Knudson, K., Smith, J., & Thompson, S. (1995). Investigating the
phenomenon of on-line relationships: A review and questionnaire study. Unpublished
paper, University of Manchester at Northwestern University.
Duck, S. (1994). Steady as (s) he goes: Relational maintenance as a shared meaning sys-
tem. In D. J. Canary & L. S. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational mainte-
nance (pp. 45-60). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Ferrara, K., Brunner, H., & Whittemore, G. (1991). Interactive written discourse as an
emergent register. Written Communication, 8, 8-34.
Fulk, J., Schmitz, J., & Ryu, D. (1995). Cognitive elements in the social construction of
technology. Management Communication Quarterly, 8, 259-288.
Gunn, D. O., & Gunn, C. W (2000, September). The quality of electronically main-
tained relationships. Paper presented at the annual conference of the Association of
Internet Researchers; Lawrence, KS.
Hiemstra, G. (1982). Teleconferencing, concern for face, and organizational culture. In M.
Burgoon (Ed.), Communication Yearbook 6 (pp. 874-904). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Jacobson, D. (1999). Impression formation in cyberspace: Online expectations and
offline experiences in text-based virtual communities. Journal of Com-
puter-Mediated Communication, .5(l). Retrieved January 3 1, 2001, from:
http://www.ascusc.org/jcmc/vol5/issuel/jacobson.html
Johansen, R., DeGrasse, R., & Wilson, T. (1978). Group communication through com-
puters: Vole. 5. Effects on workingpatterns. Menlo Park, CA: Institute for the Future.
Lea, M., & Spears, R. (1992). Paralanguage and social perception in computer-
mediated communication. Journal of Organizational Computing, 2, 32 l-34 1.
Lea, M., T., Fung, I?, & Spears, R. (1992). “Flaming” in computer-mediated
communication: Observations, explanations, implications. In M. Lea (Ed.), Contexts
of computer-mediated communication (pp. 89-112). London: Harvester-Wheatsheaf.
Markus, M. L. (1994a). El ec t ronic mail as the medium of managerial choice. Organiza-
tion Science, 5, 502-527.
Markus, M. L. (1994b). Finding a happy medium: Explaining the negative effects of
electronic communication on social life at work. ACM Transactions on Information
Systems, 12, 119-l 49.
Merkle, E. R., & Richardson, R. A. (2000). Digital dating and virtual relating: Concep-
tualizing computer mediated romantic relationships. Family Relations, 49.
187-192.
7. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE VIA CMC s=+ 161

Mitchell, VI! J. (1995). City of bits: Space, place, and the infobahn. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Nass, C., & Mason, L. (1990). On the study of technology and task: A variable-based
approach. In J. Fulk & C. Steinfield (Eds.), Organizations and communication tech-
nology (pp. 46-67). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Negroponte, N. (1995). Being digital. New York: Knopf.
Noll, A. M. (1992). Anatomy of a failure: Picturephone revisited. Telecommunications
Policy, 16, 307-316.
Parks, M. R., & Floyd, K. (1996). Making friends in cyberspace. Journal of Communica-
tion, 46, 80-97.
Parks, M. R., & Roberts, L. D. (1998). ‘Making The development of personal
relationships on-line and a comparison to their off-line counterparts. Journal of So-
cial and Personal ReLationships, 1.5, 517-537.
Pew Research Center. (2000). Tracking online life: How women use the Internet to culti-
vate relationships with family and friends. Retrieved May 10, 2000, from: http://
www.pewinternet.org/reports/toc.asp?Report= 11
Rabby, M. K. (1997, N ovember). Maintaining relationships via electronic mail. Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association, Chi-
cago, IL.
Rheingold, H. (1993). Th e virtual community: Homesteading on the electronic frontier.
Reading, MA: Addison Wesley.
Rice, R. E. (1987). New patterns of social structure in an information society. In J. R.
Schement & L. Lievrouw (Eds.), Competing visions, complex realities: Social aspects
of the information society (pp. 107-l 20). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Rice, R. E. (1993). Media appropriateness: Using social presence theory to compare
traditional and new organizational media. Human Communication Research, 19,
45 l-484.
Rintel, E. S., & Pittam, J. (1997). Strangers in a strange land: Interaction management
on Internet Relay Chat. Human Communication Research, 23, 507-534.
Roberts, L. D., Smith, L. M., & Pollack, C. (1996, September). A model of social inter-
action via computer-mediated communication in real-time text-based virtual envi-
ronments. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Australian Psychological
Society, Sydney, Australia.
Sanders, R. E. (1997). Find y our partner and do-G-do: The formation of personal rela-
tionships between social beings. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14,
387-415.
Selfe, C. L., & Meyer, P R. (1991). Testing claims for on-line conferences. Written
Communication, 8, 163-l 92.
Sellen, A. J. (1995). R emote conversations: The effects of mediating talk with technol-
ogy. Human-Computer Interaction, 10, 401-444.
Spears, R., & Lea, M. (1994). Panacea or Panopticon? The hidden power in computer-
mediated communication. Communication Research, 21, 427-459.
Sproull, L., & Faraj, S. (1997). Atheism, sex, and databases: The net as a social technol-
ogy. In S. Kiesler (Ed.), Culture ofthe Internet (pp. 35-51). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Stafford, L. S., Kline, S. L., Dimmick, J. (1999). H ome e-mail: Relational maintenance
and gratification opportunities. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 43,
659-669.
Stafford, L. S., & Reske, J. R. (1990). Idealization and communication in long-distance
relationships. Family Relations, 39, 274-279.
102 -ii=+ RABBYAND WALTHER

Stephen, T. (1986). C ommunication and interdependence in geographically separated


relationships. Human Communication Research, 13, 191-2 10.
Tidwell, L. C., & Walther, J. 8. (2000, July). Getting to know one another a bit at a time:
Computer-mediated communication effects on disclosure, impressions, and interper-
sonal evaluations. Paper presented at the International Conference on Language and
Social Psychology; Cardiff, Wales.
Turkle, S. (1995). L i f e on the screen: Identity in the age if the Internet. New York: Simon
& Schuster.
Utz, s. (2000). s ocial information processing in MUDS: The development of friend-
ships in virtual worlds. Journal of Online Behavior, 2 (1). Retrieved October 1,2000,
from: http://www. behavior.net/JOB/vlnl/utz.html
Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction: A rela-
tional perspective. Communication Research, 19, 52-90.
Walther, J. B. (1993). Impression development in computer-mediated interaction.
Western Journal of Communication, 57, 381-398.
Walther, J. B. (1994). Anticipated ongoing interaction versus channel effects on rela-
tional communication in computer-mediated interaction. Human Communication
Research, 20, 473-501.
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interper-
sonal, and hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23, 3-43.
Walther, J. B. (1997). Group and interpersonal effects in international com-
puter-mediated collaboration. Human Communication Research, 23, 342-369.
Walther, J. B. (2001). Synch ronicity, interactivity, and entrainment in computer-mediated,
oral, and written communication. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Walther, J. B., & Boyd, S. (2002). Attraction to computer-mediated social support. In
C. A. Lin & D. Atkin (Eds.), Communication technology and society: Audience adop-
tion and uses of the new media (pp. 153-l 88). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
Walther, J. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1992). R e 1a t ional communication in com-
puter-mediated interaction. Human Co&munication Research, 19, 50-88.
Walther, J. B., & K. I? (2001). The impacts of emoticons on message inter-
pretation in computer-mediated communication. Social Science Computer Review,
19, 323-345.
Walther, J. B., & Slovacek, C., & Tidwell, L. C. (2001). Is a picture worth a thousand
words? Photographic images in long term and short term virtual teams. Communica-
tion Research, 28, 105-134.
Witt, P. L., Wheeless, L. R., Reyna, J., & Swigger, K. (2000, November). An initial ex-
amination of observed verbal immediacy and opinions of communica-
tion effectiveness in on-line group interaction. Paper presented at the annual meeting
of the National Communication Association, Seattle, WA.
Wellman, B. (1994). I was a teen-age network analyst: The route from the Bronx to the
information highway. Connections, 17, 28-45.
Wellman, B., Salaff, J., Dimitrova, D., Garton, L., Gulia, M., & Haythronite, C.
(1996). Computer networks as social networks: Collaborative work, telework, and
virtual communitv. Annual Review of Sociolodt, 22, 213-238.
Relationshi I aintena rice
in 0 rganizational Setti “gs

Vincent R. Waldron
Arizona State University West

I was friends with my supervisor and had a pretty good relationship with this
other employee and his family. We had all worked together for several years,
through some challenging times, helping each other out, kind of like a team.
When he (the other employee) got a bad review and protested it, I was in the
middle and know what to do. Who should I stick-up for? What would the
other employees think if I defended him but not them? I had to be careful be-
cause the supervisor was my friend, but she was my boss too! The other em-
ployee ended up not talking with me anymore. He ended up leaving and partly
blamed me for a lack of loyalty to him. I lost my friendship with him and his wife.
They never even invited me to their house after that. It took a long time for things
to become “normal” again around the office.
--Wald?-on (2002)

his quote, from a state government employee, illustrates the complexi-


ties of relationship maintenance in organizational settings. At work, relation-
ship maintenance efforts must take into account differences in power, the
blending of work and personal relationships, task and role requirements, po-
tential career implications, and third-party perceptions, among other factors.
This chapter begins with a discussion of some of the unique characteris-
tics of organizational settings. Then, selected theoretical perspectives em-
ployed by researchers studying work relationships are briefly reviewed.
Next, research on relationship maintenance tactics and processes is de-
scribed. Both supervisory and peer relationships are considered in this sec-
tion. The fourth section emphasizes the individual, relational, and
I& -E==+ WALDRON

organizational consequences associated with relationship maintenance.


Upward mobility and perceptions of sexual harassment are among the con-
sequences addressed here. In a brief fifth section, conclusions are pre-
sented. Finally, directions for future research are discussed. The increasing
prevalence of temporary workers, telecommuting, and team-based orga-
nizing are among the themes discussed in this concluding section.

UNIQUE f”l?ATURE Ol= ORGANIZATIONAL


RIXATIONSHIPS

Work relationships are unique in a number of ways when compared to


purely personal relationships. To illustrate this point, several organizational
characteristics that influence relationships are discussed in this section:
power differences, multiple relationship forms, networks, task character-
istics, and procedural structure.

Power Differences

Power differences are commonplace in work relationships. Those enacting


leadership and management roles bring to the relationship a degree of for-
mally sanctioned position power. Status inequalities also result from differ-
ences in technical knowledge, differential access to key information, and
location in the social network, among other factors. Moreover,
members have a vested interest in the maintenance of relationships with
leaders and other powerful individuals. Most employees are aware that
poorly maintained relationships can eventually result in reduced promotion
opportunities, smaller salary increases, less rewarding work assignments,
and other unpleasant consequences. Even in presumably status-neutral
work teams, members can exert power through peer pressure (what Barker,
1993, called “concertive control”), if not raw coercion, and thus raise the
stakes of the relationship maintenance process. Accordingly, relationship
maintenance in work contexts, at least for the less powerful, may be more
mindful, strategic, and cautious than in the personal realm.

Work relationships take numerous forms. A hypothetical, but not atypical,


employee may tend to relationships with a supervisor, several subordi-
nates, members of a work team, formal and informal mentors, any number
of co-worker peers, customers or clients, suppliers, and so on. Organiza-
tions with complex structures add another level of difficulty to this chal-
lenge, requiring in matrix organizations, for example, that relationships be
maintained with multiple supervisors simultaneously. Of course, many
8. =+- 165

work relationships are personal-professional hybrids, complicating the re-


lationship maintenance picture even further (Sias & Cahill, 1998). The po-
tentially complicated intersections between the personal and the
professional become obvious in some familiar instances, as when an em-
ployee is suspected of gaining undeserved rewards from the boss through
excessive “schmoozing” or when an office romance develops.

Networks
Any given work relationship is nested within a complex system of vertical
and horizontal networks. The communication of co-worker peers is influ-
enced in part by their individual relationships with those in power and
their perception of the supervisory relationships their peers enjoy (Sias &
Jablin, 1995). Who is in favor? Who can I trust? Also, the rela-
tionship with his or her leader has consequences for the larger Workgroup
(Lee, 1998a, W aId ron & Hunt, 1992). Will positive or negative career con-
sequences flow from a close association with the boss? Lee (1997) demon-
strated the importance of this updated version of the Pelz effect on peer
communication patterns. Finally, the networked nature of organizational
communication means that relationship maintenance is not merely a
dyadic process; it extends to the maintenance of informal information net-
works and power-enhancing coalitions (Albecht & Hall, 199 1; Waldron,
1999). Employees must tend to a far-flung web of, sometimes, involuntary
relationships, the characteristics of which are determined in part by the
work they do.

?-askcharacteristics
Indeed, task characteristics enhance or constrain opportunities for un-
scripted communication (Waldron, 1994). Simple, well-defined, com-
partmentalized tasks (e.g., assembly work) may reduce
motivation and opportunity for relationship-sustaining interaction. Of
course, some manufacturing environments create opportunities for social
interaction as well, particularly if the task lends itself to small talk among
peers (Waldron, Foreman, & Miller, 1993). In contrast, complex, ambigu-
ous, or interdependent tasks can magnify the importance of relationships
with co-workers and supervisors (Thacker &Wayne, 1995). The quality of
maintenance communication, particularly the degree to which it can occur
opportunistically, is a product of work arrangement. In some workplaces
(e.g., credit card processing centers) the ratio of supervisors to workers can
be quite large, and informal relationship talk with supervisor is al-
most impossible. The relationship is instead based on formal reporting
(Waldron, 199 1). Yet, when work is organized around small creative teams
(i.e., advertising agencies), maintenance of relationships is largely infor-
mal, and absolutely critical to team success (Graen & Wakabayashi, 1994).

Procedural Structure

Another unique feature of work relationships concerns the role of proce-


dural structure (formal rules) in the maintenance process. The frequency,
content, and tone of some maintenance communication can be dictated by
formal guidelines (Waldron, 1999). For example, team members may be
required to meet regularly to discuss task coordination, address grievances,
and head-off simmering conflicts. Supervisors may request certain types of
information in weekly status reviews, including feedback about the social
climate of the Workgroup. Some of this required relationship talk overtly
problematizes issues related to relationship maintenance. (“So, how is ev-
eryone getting along this week?“).
Moreover, performance evaluations and 360 degree feedback sessions
involve the formal assessment of relationship management skills (Atwater,
Roush, & Fischthal, 1995). Through this process, employees can be ad-
vised to engage in relational communication that is more or less frequent,
directive, or formal. These assessments not only comment on relationship
maintenance efforts, but also constitute relational communication of a for-
mal kind. So, in work settings, aspects of relationship maintenance activity
are often regulated, compulsory, and subject to scrutiny. In this sense,
maintenance is not simply a dyadic phenomenon but instead reflects a
property of the larger collective. In keeping with structuration theory
(Giddens, 1994), maintenance communication is part expression and part
reinforcement of organizational structures that shape relationships.

THlEORrTTICAL ASSUMPTIONS AND KAMEWORKS

The existing research on relationship maintenance processes in the work-


place is fed by at least four theoretical streams. These approaches differ in
basic assumptions about the nature of communication. For example, some
approaches (e.g., leader-member exchange theory) construe communica-
tion primarily as a transaction of social resources. From this perspective,
the behavioral patterns that convey and preserve relational resources come
under scrutiny, as do the rewards accrued by relationship partners. A sec-
ond approach adopts the assumptions and language of systems theory, with
an attendant focus on how communication functions to restore or upset
equilibrium in relational systems. In contrast, a third perspective views re-
lationship maintenance as one of several strategic goals sought by individ-
ual communicators. The communicative plans and tactics used by
individuals to manage these goals are the subject of this research. A fourth
8. -?1- Id7

and final approach concerns the management of identity and social roles.
From this point of view, relationship maintenance is integrated with the
larger process of preserving work roles and avoiding threats to the
self-definitions offered by others. These four theoretical perspectives are
considered in more detail below.

Maintenance as Sociaf Exchange:


Leader-Member-Exchange Theory

Leader-member exchange theory, invoked in the literature for 30 years, is


perhaps the most pervasive of exchange-based approaches (for a review,
see Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995). Grounded in social exchange and
role-management perspectives, the theory holds, in part, that work rela-
tionships are defined by the quantity and quality of social resources in-
vested by the leader. Leaders are presumed to have at their disposal limited
amounts of time, effort, and communicative resources that must be appor-
tioned among multiple members. Typically, leader-member relations are
stabilized by the formal set of rules and procedures that govern workplace
interactions. For example, task responsibilities and communication rules
(e.g., reporting requirements) are standardized and followed with minimal
variation. Maintenance of such supervisory exchanges is relatively effi-
cient, requiring little negotiation and limited investment of social re-
sources.
However, more flexible and resource-intensive leadership exchanges are
developed with selected members, often those the leader regards as un-
usually proficient, loyal, likable, or promising. In these cases, the leader in-
vests considerably more informal communication, shares insider
information, and grants considerable autonomy to the member to shape his
or her role. This role-making process, in contrast to the role taking that
characterizes supervisory relationships, is maintained through more fre-
quent and informal communication between leader and member, discus-
sion of personal as well as task related information, the exercise of mutual
influence, and in general, a more extensive investment of social resources
by both parties.

Maintenance as a Systems Property


Lee and Jablin (1995) ex pl ained workplace relationship maintenance from
a systems perspective. For these authors, work relationships are subsys-
tems embedded within larger organizational systems. Communicative be-
haviors function to maintain the character of the relational system, that is,
to maintain a steady state, particularly during times of system disturbance
(entropic moments). Efforts by one party to accelerate or deaccelerate the
lo8 +== WALDRON

relationship represent moments of this kind, as when a member tries to be-


come more personal in his or her interactions with the supervisor. Invoking
the systems metaphor, Lee and Jablin drew on the idea of requisite variety
to explain how multiple forms of communication behavior can maintain
relationship equilibrium, depending on the nature of the disturbance.

Maintenance as an Interaction Goal

Researchers use the concept of interaction goals to explain the relation-


ship maintenance practices of members and leaders (Waldron, 199 1).
Organizational researchers too often assume that task objectives, like
obtaining influence or sharing information, are the primary objectives of
workplace communication. From this perspective, task objectives are
interdependent with, and sometimes subordinate to, the maintenance
of acceptably defined relationships and work identities. In cases where
status differences complicate the efforts of members to achieve task
goals, maintenance behavior is thought to be crucial in reducing (or
magnifying) relational threat. For example, members of a supervisory
dyad that is maintained through chronic avoidance will find open shar-
ing of negative performance feedback highly threatening. In contrast, a
history of more explicit talk about relational expectations might mini-
mize threat perceptions. This line of research continues to examine the
conceptual and empirical connections between maintenance behavior
and other forms of potentially threatening workplace communication,
including the expression of emotion and upward influence tactics
(Waldron, 1994, 1999, 2000).

Maintenance as the Management of Social Identity

Relationship maintenance processes are also implicated in a loosely con-


nected set of theories concerned with the management of work identities
through relational communication. In general, these approaches suggest
that relational upset can be managed by anticipating and avoiding messages
which threaten the identity of the self or the co-worker. For example,
Larson (1989) argued that employees take advantage of reluc-
tance to deliver negative performance feedback. Supervisors sometimes
fear the relational consequences of negative evaluation. By cautiously and
selectively seeking positive or vague feedback and by asking for advice
rather than inviting criticism, members reinforce positive relationship def-
initions. Over time it becomes increasingly inconsistent and face threaten-
ing for the supervisor to offer highly negative feedback within this
mutually constructed and apparently positive relationship. The mainte-
nance of the relational status quo is a side-effect of this identity manage-
8. WORKSETTINGS +a-

ment dilemma. According to Larson, apparently cordial relations


persevere under these conditions but so does poor performance. Em-
ployees are ultimately surprised when negative performance feedback can
be delayed no longer.

MAINTlZNANC.E PROCl3SES AND TACTICS

Research on maintenance communication traditionally has focused on ver-


tical (supervisory) relationships, with peer relationships becoming impor-
tant only in recent years. This section examines studies of both relationship
types. In addition, it seeks insight from research on forms of workplace
communication that have obvious relevance, but have yet to be conceptu-
alized as relationship maintenance by researchers.

Maintenance Communication in Supervisory ReIationships

In an early case study set in a communal setting, Kaplan (1978) conceived


of relationship maintenance as largely a matter of expressing or suppress-
ing feelings or perspectives. In subsequent studies, researchers have made
finer distinctions among tactics and the circumstances under which they
are used. Several authors, acknowledging the status differences inherent in
most leader-member relationships, have described the upward mainte-
nance tactics initiated by members (see Table 8.1). Waldron (1991, 1997;
Waldron & Hunt, 1992, Waldron, Hunt & Dsilva, 1993) inductively de-
rived a four-factor taxonomy from a survey of 5 18 working adults. Direct
tactics are communicative actions that overtly comment on relational ex-
pectations, question relationship injustices, and invite discussion of rela-
tionship status. Regulative tactics are defensive in nature. They involve
careful management of impressions, messages, feedback, and emotions.
Contacts with the leader are minimized and characterized generally by
avoidant, superficial communication.
Persond/InformaZ tactics appear to foster friendship ties with the super-
visor. Frequent informal chat, personal content, sharing of jokes and sto-
ries, compliments, and small talk define this relationship maintenance
approach. Finally, contractual communication involves rule following, clar-
ifying performance expectations, advice seeking and accepting, and abid-
ing by informal and formal agreements.
In the management literature, Tepper (1995) confirmed these four tac-
tic types and identified a fifth. An extension of contractual cate-
gory, these extracontractual tactics communicate the
willingness to exceed relationship and role expectations. They communi-
cate unexpected levels of accessibility, personal investment, and commit-
ment to the relationship.
TABLE 8.1
Categories of Maintenance T&tics with Behavioral ExampIes

Author [Date) Category/Subcategory Behavioral Examples

Waldron (199 1) Personal/Informal 1. We share small talk


2. I treat him/her like a friend

Contractual 1. I am careful to follow the rules he/she has established


2. I remain polite toward him/her

Regulative/Defensive 1. I sometimes stretch the truth to avoid problems with him/her


2. I make sure the supervisor is in a good mood before discussing important
work matters with him/her

Direct 1. I speak up when I feel I am treated unjustly


2. I frequently offer my opinions

Tepper (1995) Extra-contractual 1. I try to exceed what is required of me at work


2. I offer to relieve burdens on my boss

Escalating Situations

Lee & Jablin (1995) Avoidance of 1. I sit as far away as possible in meetings
Interaction 2. I Plan my schedule so as not to encounter him/her

Conversational 1. I try to focus my conversation with him/her on work-related issues


refocus: indirect 2. I sometimes act like I do not know what he/she is talking about
Conversational refocus: 1. 1 tell him/her that I do not want to discuss his/her personal life
direct 2. I tell him/her not to talk so much about me in public

Openness 1. I express my feelings in a forthrightly manner


2. I discuss what I perceive to be important aspects of my work

Procrastination 1. I tell him/her that I need more time to think about a matter
2. I tell him/her when it is not a good time for me to help him/her

Deteriorating
Situations

Direct/Open 1. In a nonthreatening manner, I let him/her know that there will be negative
consequences if things change
2. I speak up when I felt treated unjustly

Creating closeness 1. I talk to him/her as I would a good friend


2. I express my willingness to help him/her out

Deception/Distortion 1. I pretend that a dissatisfying work situation with him/her is over


2. I withhold communicating undesirable information to him/her

Circumspectiveness 1. I avoid saying anything that may embarrass him/her


2. I avoid the expression of extreme negative emotions toward him/her

Self-Promotion 1. I make sure that he/she knows how hard I am working


2. I let him/her know about my past successes on the job

(Continued on next page)


TABLE 8.1 (Continued)
Categories of Maintenance Tactics with Behavioral BarnpIes

Author (Date) Category/Subcategory Behavioral Examples

Routine Situations

Avoidance 1. I avoid delivering bad news to him/her


2. I try to look busy when he/she is around

Supportiveness 1. I encourage him/her to discuss problems he/she is experiencing


2. I inquire about his/her progress on the job

Positive regard 1. I remain polite toward him/her


2. I am honest in what I say to him/her

Restrained expression 1. I do not interrupt when he/she is engaged in conversation


2. I avoid appearing too anxious when we talk

Small talk 1. I engage in small talk with him/her


2. I ask him/her how things are going
8. WORKSETTINGS =+- r,q

Lee (1997,1998a, 1998b; Lee & Jablin, 1995) examined tactics used in
deteriorating, escalating, and stable supervisory relationships. As can be
seen in Table 8.1, these two independent lines of research yield fairly con-
sistent tactic categories. However, list of tactics is more extensive
and reported at a finer level of granularity. Such tactics as conversational
refocusing, distortion, and circumspection (Lee & Jablin, 1995) add be-
havioral detail to the Regulative category proposed by Waldron (199 1). As
is common in taxonomic research, differences in grouping terms and level
of analysis account for much of the variance in maintenance behaviors re-
ported in these studies. For example, Small Tulle tactic resembles one
of the behaviors associated with PersonaZ/Informal category.
work makes clear that the occurrence of certain maintenance tactics
depends on the state of the supervisory relationship and the re-
lational intentions. For example, in escalating situations, in which a super-
visor sought to personalize the relationship beyond the
comfort level, a distinct set of tactics emerged. In such cases, the members
forestalled escalation through procrastination and conversational refocus-
ing. These tactics were less evident when the relationship was perceived to
be stable or deescalating.

Maintenance in Peer ReIationships

Relationship maintenance in peer relationships has received less explicit


attention from researchers, although the importance to workers of peer re-
lationships is often noted in the literature (Kram & Isabella, 1985; Odden
& Sias, 1997). Using a revised version of a survey reported by Ayres
(1983), Shea and Pearson (1986) examined maintenance tactics used by
370 working adults in their relationships with friends, acquaintances, and
co-workers. Ayres originally suggested three types of maintenance tactics
among friends. Avoidance tactics attempted to control behavior that might
harm the relationship. Balance tactics compensated for relational prob-
lems. Direct tactics explicitly commented on relationship expectations. A
factor analysis by Shea and Pearson confirmed the three-factor structure.
The study found no difference between the relationship types. However,
due to methodological limitations, these groundbreaking studies left room
for considerable refinement in methodology and conceptualization. The
original Ayres taxonomy was not developed inductively. Students rather
than workers were the participants. As Shea and Pearson noted, their re-
sults were influenced by the potentially overlapping relational status of
friends and co-workers.
Recent studies of workplace friendship have made important contribu-
tions, although relationship maintenance behavior has not been the pri-
mary objective of interest. Fine (1986) observed that friendships in the
17-f -e- WALDRON

workplace are characterized by shared humor, extra-organizational activi-


ties, and the equitable sharing of tasks. More recently, Sias and Cahill
(I 998) examined the development of peer relationships in the workplace,
comparing co-workers, friends, close friends, and “almost best” friends.
They stressed that communication with workplace friends was an impor-
tant source of social support and mentoring. Sias and Cahill found that
shared socializing and close proximity were features of workplace friend-
ship, whereas closer relationships were constituted by activities like talk-
ing through important life events (unrelated to work) and sharing major
work challenges. Cox (1999) took a different approach, examining the role
of peer communication in worker decisions to maintain or terminate their
association with the group. Such tactics as encouraging self-evaluation,
criticism, and the reduction of interpersonal support were among the be-
haviors reported by peers.

Related Types OF Relational Communication

Although not explicitly concerned with the process of relationship mainte-


nance, organizational researchers have studied extensively other forms of
interpersonal behavior with obvious relevance. For example, the vast liter-
ature on workplace compliance-gaining tactics (for extensive reviews see
Barry & Watson, 1996) has demonstrated that workers establish condi-
tions of friendliness, liking, and similarity as a relational precondition for
seeking compliance, particularly when the target is a more-powerful per-
son. These behaviors are typically classified as a form of ingratiation
(Gordon, 1996; Thacker & Wayne, 1995). Similar to tactics associated
with personal relationships (e.g., Canary & Stafford, 1994), exchange tac-
tics involve compromise, worksharing, and equitable contributions. These
behaviors may sustain work relationships by creating a sense of mutual ob-
ligation. Consultation tactics, although typically conceived of as a form of
compliance gaining, may also foster commitment to the relationship by ac-
knowledging the potential contributions of the partner and mitigating re-
sentments stemming from status inequality. In each case, these tactics
make salient and reinforce particular relational qualities, typically as a pre-
condition to future requests for behavioral change.
The regulation of emotion has become increasingly important in re-
search on work relationships (e.g., Fineman, 2000). Rules governing emo-
tional display serve in part to stabilize professional relations among
co-workers and with customers. For example, workers sometimes report
editing positive emotions during the performance appraisal process
(Wayne & Kacmar, 199 1). An employee might suppress the joy of an unex-
pected raise in pay to minimize the envy of less fortunate co-workers.
Maintaining good relations with more powerful supervisors may require
8. WORKSETTINGS w 175

the suppression of anger or the false production of enthusiasm (Lee &


Jablin, 1995; Waldron, 1991). Further, team players express or repress
their irritation with other members to sustain group commitment and har-
mony (Barker, 1993). Th e management of emotion is critical in maintain-
ing personal relationships as well. But, as these examples illustrate, it is
complicated at work by emotional strictures built into work relationships,
organizational cultures, and group norms valdron, 1994).

OUTCOME Ol= RIELATIONSHIP MAINTIENANCE

The extent to which employees succeed in relationship maintenance ef-


forts may have personal and career consequences. Several of these are ad-
dressed below.

Career Advancement

At least in the popular imagination of employees, relationship maintenance


behavior is closely linked to career advancement. Above and beyond meri-
torious performance, remaining on good terms with the boss is believed to
be a prerequisite for workplace success. However, the complexity of the
relationship maintenance task is illustrated by the negative slang terms
used to characterize those who sacrifice task performance or personal in-
tegrity in the process. Those accused of “brownnosing” or “sleeping your
way to the top” among other denigrating labels, are thought to have taken a
strictly relational route to career success. Avoiding these negative percep-
tions may require sophisticated maintenance behavior, as employees find
the right mix of tactics. In doing so, they may rely on the behaviors typi-
cally used in supervisory, friendship, and even romantic relationships,
while managing third-party perceptions. This balancing act was noted in
(1991) study of approximately 500 full-time workers. Some
employees appeared to combine informal/personal maintenance tactics
(used to maintain friendship ties with supervisors) with contractual tac-
tics, which legitimized the members access to the boss. These tactics call
attention to the conformity to performance expectations and
role requirements. In this way, employees take advantage of friendship ties
but also minimize perceptions of favoritism. Promotions and other positive
recognition can always be explained to skeptical or jealous colleagues by
referencing the history of meeting expectations and being a
“good soldier” rather than his or her friendship with the boss.
Specific relationship maintenance tactics have in fact been linked to
indicators of upward mobility. For example, behaviors like sharing confi-
dences, expressions of support and similarity, and flattery have been sta-
tistically associated with supervisor evaluations and promotability
176 -c== WALDRON

judgments (Gordon, 1996; Wayne & Kacmar, 1991). Relationally ori-


ented influence behaviors affect performance ratings indirectly over
time, by first establishing negative or positive affective responses in the
target. These subjective reactions subsequently shape evaluations of per-
formance. In addition, the frequent use of exchange tactics in upward in-
fluence situations, which have conceptual similarities to the task sharing
used to maintain personal relationships (Canary & Stafford, 1994), has
been linked with salary attainment in at least one study (Dreher,
Dougherty, & Whitely, 1989).
A skill in maintaining relationships with powerful people may
determine the extent to which he or she will receive opportunities to cus-
tomize the work role, access to resources that facilitate individual or team
success, and presumably, advancement in his or her career (Graen &
Wakabayashi, 1994). M oreover, the benefits associated with the leader-
ship-type relationship are apparently enhanced as the relationship matures
and is maintained over time (Graen & Uhl-bien, 1995). Variation in main-
tenance behavior has been documented not just in formal supervisory rela-
tionships, but also in mentoring relationships, which in turn have been
linked to career success (Tepper, 1995).

Sexual Harassment

The workplace creates opportunities for maintenance behavior to be per-


ceived as intrusive, inappropriate, and possibly illegal. This is most obvi-
ous in cases where apparently inappropriate relational communication
has lead to charges of sexual harassment. One such instance, still part of
the collective memory of many Americans, gained national attention in
controversial nationally televised hearings conducted by the Judiciary
Committee of the United States Senate to confirm the Supreme Court
appointment of Clarence Thomas (see Waldron, Foreman, & Miller,
1993). Senators dissected the relational behavior of Thomas and former
employee Anita Hill, who had charged him with sexual harassment. A
host of third-party witnesses were questioned about the relationship
maintenance practices of Thomas and Hill. Witnesses speculated about
such matters as whether or not Ms. “expectation of access” and
“expressions of admiration” were evidence of proprietary rather than a
professional interest in her boss. relational behavior was subject
to similar scrutiny, and gave rise to questions: Did he raise sexual topics
that were inappropriate in a supervisory relationship? Did he act in other
ways that implied a sexual interest? In essence these questions concern
whether behavior was intended to maintain or alter his profes-
sional relationship with Hill. Because women are more often the victim
of harassment and gender bias, they may work harder to maintain accept-
8. WORK SETTINGS -+I- 177

ably defined relationships, using relationship maintenance communica-


tion more frequently and employing more varied tactics. Waldron et al.
(1993) found that women reported using contractual tactics more fre-
quently than males, apparently as a means of reducing relational ambigu-
ity and the potential for biased perception. Men were particularly careful
not to engage in personal communication when the supervisor was a fe-
male. The result of these patterns of behaviors may be a reduction in rela-
tional risk and generally more cautious communicative climate.

well-Beingand.satisFaction
Maintenance communication at work may be associated with personal
well-being, work satisfaction, and a favorable psychological climate
(Odden & Sias, 1997). Stable, supportive relationships with co-workers
can provide a protective buffer when employees are faced with stressful
tasks, personal trauma, burn out, or difficult co-workers (cf., Sias &
Cahill, 1998). The potentially turbulent processes of accepting new work
roles and workplace changes may be eased for individuals who have main-
tained supportive work relationships (K-am & Isabella, 1985; Kramer,
1995, 1996). Indeed, f or some, the maintenance of friendships with
co-workers may be among the most important motives for remaining in a
particular job.

Organizational Outcomes

In addition to personal outcomes, the successful maintenance of work rela-


tionships can have organization-wide effects. For example, when supervi-
sors maintain relationships with subordinates differently, perception of
inequity and dissatisfaction can spread across the Workgroup (Sias &
Jablin, 1995). Th e extent to which personal relationships are maintained
appears to affect the flow of innovative information across an organization
(Albrecht & Hall, 199 1). Employee turnover may be reduced when work-
personal relationship networks are strong. Employees who maintain
regular, informal communication with supervisors are more likely to com-
municate negative information up the chain of command (Waldron, 199 1)
so the organization can react quickly to problems.

CONCLUSIONS
The literature reviewed thus far lends itself to several conclusions regard-
ing relationship maintenance tactics and outcomes. After discussing sev-
eral of these conclusions here, directions are presented for new research in
the next sclctinn.
$78 +== WALDRON

Several trends are obvious in the research on relationship maintenance


behavior. First, at least some of the behaviors used to maintain work rela-
tionships, such as (199 1) contractual tactics, vary from those re-
ported in the literature on personal relationships. This is in part due to the
unique features of the workplace, including power differences and formal
role requirements. Perhaps most important, relationship maintenance at
work is conducted in front of an audience of interested peers. Tactics that
manage the perceptions of co-workers (e.g., avoiding perceptions of favor-
itism) become an important part of the maintenance equation. This sur-
veillance factor, when coupled with significant power differences, makes
relationship maintenance at work a mindful and strategic process.
A second conclusion is that relationship maintenance in the workplace
is not entirely a dyadic process. Relationships are maintained in part
through reporting structures, role requirements, and procedures that
compel communication, add stability, and sometimes provide feedback
to employees about the success of their relationship maintenance efforts.
Interestingly, these macrovariables are embedded in the taken-for-
granted business practices and cultural norms that form the backdrop of
organizational life. Employees may not consciously question or even re-
flect on the fact that they are required by organizational protocol to com-
municate with their supervisor in a particular format, using a respectful
form of address, at particular times of the day or week. So, despite the
previously mentioned claim that relationship maintenance at work is po-
tentially strategic and mindful, it may be true as well that the stabilizing
constraints on work relationships remain unnegotiated and predeter-
mined by forces beyond the control of the partners.
A third conclusion is that this area of research is rife with conceptual
overlap. Behaviors and tactics associated with impression management,
upward influence, and emotional regulation, among other processes are
similar to those reported in relationship maintenance taxonomies. Con-
ceptual integration of tactic taxonomies is needed. More important, theo-
retical frameworks are needed to explain how these seemingly related
communication activities function together as work relationships evolve
and stabilize.
A fourth conclusion from this research is that relationship maintenance
behavior has truly important consequences at work, for individuals and for
organizations. The research confirms what many workers know intu-
itively-that tending to their network of relationships is an integral part of
doing the work. The tactics used for this purpose are linked reliably to rela-
tional outcomes and at least indirectly to such factors as performance eval-
uation and upward mobility. Information transfer, the development of
innovation, and workplace effectiveness may be enhanced when work-
place relationships are suitably maintained. The research confirming these
trends provides a most compelling rationale for continued research on rela-
8. WORKSETTINGS -+t- 17~

tionship maintenance at work: It can help improve the quality of


lives and the effectiveness of their organizations.

l=UTURlE DIRlECTlONS

The pace of research on work relationships is accelerating with the rapidly


changing nature of work in the U.S. and elsewhere. In the future, research-
ers must explain how workers manage relationships that are increasingly
complex, diverse, and fluid. In addition, theoretical perspectives must be
modified to more fully explain the contribution of macro-forces like orga-
nizational structure, economics, and technological trends in maintaining
associations among workers.

TheChangingSocial Contract Between


Organizationand Employee

Work relationships are becoming harder to maintain. Organizations have


been relentless in recent years in their efforts to streamline, outsource, and
otherwise downsize their workforces. At least hypothetically, these steps
allow organizations to respond more rapidly to changes in the external en-
vironment. Of course these moves have significant implications for work-
ers who lose their jobs and for those who remain. Some of these are
economic (loss of income), some physical (longer work hours), and some
psychological (job stress). Of interest here are relational effects.
Consider, for example, outsourcing of work traditionally performed by
in-house employees. Outsourcing increases an reliance on
temporary workers and creates an expanding class of impermanent work
relationships. What are the psychological and behavioral characteristics of
relationship maintenance under these conditions? One effect may be that
temporary employees and those who work with them are increasingly cau-
tious about investing resources in work relationships in which the future is
short-term or unpredictable. Another could be a reduction in the valuable
social support that comes from long-term relationships with co-workers
(cf., Sias & Cahill, 1998). H ow do workers compensate for these relation-
ship-threatening trends? Do they invest relatively more resources in main-
taining familial relationships and personal friendships? Do they manage to
sustain relationships with former co-workers, perhaps through profes-
sional associations or informal professional networks? The communication
patterns of temporary workers have only recently begun to receive re-
search attention (Sias, Kramer, & Jenkins, 1997).
In general, it would appear that the reduced commitment of employers
to life-long employment and the reciprocal reduction in worker loyalty
would dramatically alter and (perhaps) negatively affect maintenance
180 -I+=+ WALDRON

communication. But, well-maintained work relationships may provide


protective effects in stressful and uncertain times. Researchers have barely
begun to address how the changing social contract between employers and
employees affects relationship maintenance behavior. Clearly this is a re-
search area of critical importance for workers and society at large.

The ~oIe OF Communication Technology

In yet another trend, communication technology continues to create im-


proved opportunities for physically isolated workers to maintain relation-
ships with individual co-workers and networks (see Rabby & Walther,
chap. 7, this volume). Indeed, the rise of telecommuting and the advent of
virtual organizations threaten to reinvent the relationship maintenance
process. For example, the potency of dyadic maintenance behaviors like
small talk can be magnified exponentially when shared across electronic
networks. The force of destructive relational behaviors, like the unautho-
rized sharing of confidences, can be magnified in a similar manner.
The capacity to access relational partners is altered in the virtual world
of work. On the plus side, choosing not to read your e-mail from the home
office may be an effective way of avoiding nosy co-workers (especially
when the alternative is an open cubicle in a crowded work area). On the
down side, pagers, e-mail, and cell phones more frequently tether employ-
ees to their work, making it more difficult to maintain social distance be-
tween the individual and co-workers. Relationship maintenance may
necessarily become a more deliberate and mindful process when one is un-
able simply to “drop in” to a physical workspace. Even so,
some of the benefits accrued from opportunistic maintenance may be lost
when face-to-face interaction is minimized. Are informal, unplanned op-
portunities for relationship maintenance, such as small talk around the of-
fice coffee pot, critical in keeping work relationships vital? Is face-to-face
maintenance a better option if one hopes to increase visibility, foster trust,
convey assurances, and ultimately gain the cooperation of the supervisors
and peers who influence career outcomes? These are questions researchers
have yet to address fully.

Increasingly Diverse Relationship Types

The complexity and blended nature of work relationships hinted at in


this chapter will only increase in coming years. The almost exclusive em-
phasis by researchers on maintenance of supervisory relationships must
be altered to acknowledge this reality. Several researchers have examined
the interpersonal communication patterns that characterize peer based
(&am & Isabella, 1985) and hierarchical mentor@ relationships
8. -+ 181

(Tepper, 1995). It is clear even from preliminary studies reported ear-


lier, that peer relationships and the social support they provide are crucial
in work and life satisfaction (Odden & Sias, 1997). This will become in-
creasingly evident as organizations continue the retreat from traditional
hierarchies to decentralized structures based on self-directed work
teams and loosely organized networks. In this environment, strategies for
long-term maintenance of team relationships should become a focus of
communication researchers.
As workplace peers are increasingly of different cultures, ethnicities,
and genders, researchers should also examine maintenance practices in di-
verse relationships (Graen & Wakabayashi, 1994). Moreover, the blurring
boundaries of work and personal life create new opportunities for relation-
ship maintenance failures in one realm to have cross-over effects. The an-
ecdote that introduced this chapter provides an example of just this
problem, as a strained work relationship apparently spread to relationships
with family members.

Theory Development

Waldron (1999) recently urged management researchers to expand their


theoretical perspectives regarding communication in work relationships.
The incorporation of developmental and dialectical perspectives has been
encouraged, in part because these approaches draw attention to
long-lasting patterns of work relationships rather than the less frequent
conflict management and compliance-gaining episodes that have con-
sumed attention to date. As has long been noted by social the-
orists like Giddens (1991), work relationships are embedded within and
reproduce larger social structures. But until recently maintenance re-
searchers have tended to isolate the work relationship as a
microphenomenon onto itself. Theory development in this area needs to
expand beyond the dyad to consider existing research on networks, coali-
tions, and interdependent systems. Network approaches characterize rela-
tionships as nodes in larger social structures or systems designed to
propagate or process information about values, tasks, personal relation-
ships, and innovations. Relationship maintenance can be conceptualized as
a process of stabilizing and reinforcing properties (like interdependence)
of communication systems (see e.g., Harrison, 1994). During times of
change, increased dialogue and connectivity across a network of
co-workers might help reduce uncertainty and increase predictability. Ul-
timately, this may allow the system to sustain itself over time. At the level
of individuals, maintaining central positions in this web of relationships be-
comes critical. Networking is the means by which power, information, new
ideas, and other benefits are sustained.
182 -c=+ WALDRON

Organizational researchers have studied workplace relationships for


many years. But at a time when such relationships are becoming more di-
verse, maintenance researchers can bring a new vitality to this work. As this
chapter makes clear, traditional assumptions about the nature of work rela-
tionships are under scrutiny. Also clear at this juncture is this: Traditional
perspectives on interpersonal communication as strictly a micro-
phenomenon will not transfer well to the study of work relationships. Main-
tenance researchers must take into account the structural, economic, social,
and political forces that affect the character of relations among co-workers.
At the same time, we must acknowledge that the boundary between per-
sonal and work life is in part illusory. Finally, researchers should approach
this area of research with a sense of both excitement and urgency. Arguably,
there has been no time in industrial history when the maintenance of work
relationships has been more challenging, more complex, and more conse-
quential for workers and their organizations.

Albrecht, T. A., & Hall, B. (1991). Facilitating talk about new ideas: The role of personal
relationships in organizational innovation. Communication Monographs, 58,
273-289.
Atwater, L., Roush, I?, & Fischthal, A. (1995). The influence of upward feedback on self
and follower ratings of leadership. Personnel Psychology, 48, 35-59.
Ayers, J. (1983). Strategies to maintain relationships: Their identification and per-
ceived usage. Communication Quarterly, 3 1, 62-67.
Barker, J. B. (1993). Tightening the iron cage: Concertive control in self-managing
teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 408-437.
Barry, B. &Watson, M. R. (1996). C ommunication aspects of dyadic social influence in
organizations: A review and integration of conceptual and empirical developments.
Communication Yearbook 19, 269-3 17.
Canary, D., & Stafford, L. (1994). C ommunication and relationship maintenance. New
York: Academic Press.
Cox, S. (1999). Group communication and employee turnover: How co-workers en-
courage peers to voluntarily exit. Southern Communication Journal, 64, 18 l-l 92.
Dreher, G., Dougherty, T., & Whitely, W (1989). I n fl uence tactics and salary attain-
ment: A gender-specific analysis. Sex Roles, 20, 535-550.
Fine, G. A. (1986). F riendships in the workplace. In V I. Derlaga & B. A. Winstead
(Eds.), Friendship and social interaction (pp. 185-206). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Fineman, S. (2000). Emotion in organizations (2nd ed.). London: Sage.
Giddens, A. (1984). The constitution of society. Berkeley: University of California
Press.
Gordon, R. A. (1996). Impact of ingratiation on judgments and evaluations: A meta-
analytic investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 54-70.
Graen, G., & Uhl-Bien, M. (1995). Relationship-based approach to leadership: Devei-
opment of leader-member exchange (LMX) theory over 25 years: Applying a
multi-level, multi-domain perspective. Leadership Quarterly, 6, 2 19-247.
Graen, G., & Wakabayashi, M. (1994). Cross-cultural leadership-making: Bridging
American and Japanese diversity for team advantage. In H. C. Triandis, M. D.
8. WORKSETTINGS -+- 183

Dunnette, & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psy-


chology (Vol. 4, pp. 4 15-446). N ew York: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Harrison, T. (1994). C ommunication and interdependence in democratic organiza-
tions. In S. Deetz (Ed.), Communication Yearbook I7 (pp. 247-274). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kaplan, R. E. (1978). Maintaining relationships openly: A case study of “total open-
ness” in a communal setting. Human Relal;~ns, 31, 375-393.
Kram, K. E., & Isabella, L. A. (1985). Mentoring alternatives: The role of peer relation-
ships in career development. Academy of Management Journal, 28, 110-l 32.
Kramer, M. (1995). A longitudinal study of superior-subordinate communication dur-
ing job transfers. Human Communication Research, 22, 39-64.
Kramer, M. (1996). A longitudinal study of peer communication during job transfers:
The impact of frequency, quality, and network multiplicity on adjustment. Human
Communication Research, 23, 59-86.
Larson, J. R. (1989). The dynamic interplay between feedback seeking
strategies and delivery of bad news. Academy ofManagement Review,
14, 409-422.
Lee, J. (1997). Leader-member exchange, the “Pelz Effect,” and cooperative commu-
nication between group members. Management Communication Quarterly, 1 I,
266-287.
Lee, J. (1998a). Effective maintenance communication in superior-subordinate rela-
tionships. Western Journal of Communication, 62, 181-208.
Lee, J. (1998b). Maintenance communication in superior-subordinate relationships:
An exploratory investigation of group social context and the “Pelz Effect.” Southern
Communication Journal, 63, 144- 159.
Lee, J., & Jablin, F. (1995). Maintenance communication in superior-subordinate work
relationships. Human Communication Research, 22, 220-257.
Odden, C., & Sias, P (1997). Peer communication relationships and psychological cli-
mate. Communication Quarterly, 45, 152-l 66.
Shea, B. C., & Pearson, J. C. (1986). The effects of relationship type, partner intent,
and gender on the selection of relationship maintenance strategies. Communication
Monographs, 53, 352-364.
Sias, I? M., & Cahill, D. (1998). F rom co-workers to friends: The development of peer
relationships in the workplace. Western Journal of Communication, 62, 273-299.
Sias, P M., & Jablin, F. M. (1995). D i ff erential superior-subordinate relations, percep-
tions of fairness, and co-worker communication. Human Communication Research,
22, 5-38.
Sias, I? M., Kramer, M. W, & Jenkins, E. (1997). A comparison of the communication
behaviors of temporary employees and new hires. Communication Research, 24,
731-754.
Tepper, B. (1995). Up ward maintenance tactics in supervisory mentoring and
nonmentoring relationships. Academy of Management Journal, 38(4),
1191-1205.
Thacker, R. A., &Wayne, S. J. (1995). An examination of the relationship between up-
ward influence tactics and assessments of promotability. Journal of Management,
21, 739-756.
Waldron, V (199 1). Achieving communication goals in superior-subordinate relation-
ships: The multi-functionality of upward maintenance tactics. Communication
Monographs, 58, 289-306.
Waldron, V (1994). Once more, with feeling: Reconsidering the role of emotion in
work. In S. A. Deetz (Ed.), Communication Yearbook 17, 388-416.
I&I- -E=+ WALDRON

Waldron, V. (1999). C ommunication practices of followers, members, and proteges:


The case of upward influence tactics. In M. Roloff (Ed.). Communication Yearbook
22 (pp. 251-299). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Waldron, V (2000). Relational experiences and emotion at work. In S. Fineman (Ed).
Emotion in Organizations (2nd ed., pp. 64-82). London: Sage.
Waldron, VI (2002). [S urvey responses regarding communication, emotion and rela-
tionships] Unpublished raw data.
Waldron, V., Foreman, C., & Miller, R. (1993). Managing gender conflicts in the super-
visory relationship: Relationship definition strategies used by women and men. In
G. Crepes (Ed.), Sexual harassment: Communication implications (pp. 234-256).
Cresskill, NJ: Hammond Press.
Waldron, V, & Hunt, M. H. (1992). H ierarchical level, length, and quality of supervi-
sory relationship as predictors of use of maintenance tactics. Commu-
nication Reports, 5, 82-89.
Waldron, H., Hunt, M., & Dsilva, M. (1993). Towards a threat management model of
upward communication. A study of influence and maintenance tactics in the
leader-member dyad. Communication Studies, 44, 254-272.
Wayne, S. J., & Kacmar, K. M. (199 1). The effects of impression management on the
performance appraisal process. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-
cesses, 48, 70-88.
Whenpa rs Falter:
Repair After a Transgression

Tara M. Emmers-Sommer
University of Arizona

MAINTENANCE AND RH?AIR

e might wonder why a chapter addressing relational repair exists


in a book on maintenance in close relationships. Indeed, a close relation-
ship with an intimate partner is something that many people strive for as it
brings fulfillment to their lives (Duck, 1988). Similarly, many theories and
models of relational development address closeness as a desired product in
our relationships (e.g., Altman &Taylor, 1973). In a word, individuals value
and cherish their close, personal relationships. It seems to follow, then,
that examining the maintenance of such valued relationships is in order.
And, it is necessary to examine relational repair within those very same
close, personal relationships. In fact, it might be somewhat more necessary
to examine repair in close relationships, in part because people tend to
treat close relational partners more poorly than they treat complete strang-
ers (e.g., Birchler, Weiss, & Vincent, 1975). Thus, a seeming paradox ex-

The author wishes to thank Rachel Rainwater McClure for her assistance with this chapter and
Dan Canary and Marianne Dainton for their very helpful suggestions on an earlier draft of this
chapter.
186 -ii=+ EMMERS-SOMMER

ists: If we value and cherish our close relationships, why would we threaten
their maintenance?
According to Miller (1997), several sources of ammunition exist that in-
fluence engagement in undesirable, adverse behavior toward
their relational partners. These sources of ammunition, if you will, include
use of intimate information against the partner, learning undesirable infor-
mation about the partner, the erosion of illusion about the partner, the loss
of novelty, reduction of maintenance strategies in the relationship, interde-
pendence, loss of gains from the relational developmental period, and ex-
clusion. Miller (1997) furthered that additional elements can fuel negative
behavior in close relationships. For example, the interjection of culture
might affect behavior adversely if partners hail from different cultures
with different cultural values and goals. If opposing goals exist, conflict is
likely to ensue. Similarly, individual differences such as differences in per-
sonality reflected through varied levels of assertiveness, aggressiveness, or
self-esteem (to name a few) between the partners can affect behaviors
negatively. Given that partners do not always treat one another well, the
focus of this chapter is the processes of relational maintenance and repair
in close, personal relationships.
As the truism states, “if it broke, fix it.” Indeed, within on-
going close relationships, the process of maintaining is often not so clearly
recognized as when partners are not maintaining, when aspects of the re-
lationship are broken, distressed, challenged, or the like than when the
relationship is stable. This truism, however, implies that relationships are
self-sustaining and effortless, and that effort is not necessary until a prob-
lem arises. This assumption is problematic, as lack of effort to maintain a
relationship will inevitably result in the need for repair at some point in
time (Duck, 1988; Guerrero, Eloy, & Wabnik, 1993). For this reason, it is
nearly impossible to think of maintenance without considering repair. Al-
though both maintenance and repair are separate constructs, they never-
theless exist within the context of the other. Relational repair can, in fact,
be conceptualized as a type of relational maintenance. For instance,
Dindia (1994) labeled relational repair as “corrective maintenance” (p.
100). The coexistence notion of maintenance and repair is further elabo-
rated later in the chapter.
The purpose of this chapter is to examine: (a) the notions of relationship
maintenance and repair and how the constructs are brought together by the
presence of a relational transgression; (b) assumptions regarding relational
repair in close relationships; (c) communication strategies used to repair
close relationships; (d) conclusions drawn from the extant literature; and (e)
directions for future research. To begin, the constructs of relational mainte-
nance and repair are addressed within a definitional framework. This defini-
tional framework is to be considered for the remainder of this chapter as
issues regarding relational transgressions and repair are addressed.
9. REPAIR AFTER TRANSGRESSION -=a- 187

A variety of definitions of relational maintenance and relational repair


exists. Whereas relational maintenance is often conceptualized as at-
tempts to preserve the relationship in its current state (e.g., Baxter, 1994;
Canary & Stafford, 1992; Dindia & Baxter, 1987; Stafford & Canary,
1991), reZationa2 repair involves partners engaging in behaviors to restore
the relationship to its former condition, which assumes that something has
disrupted the relationship (e.g., Aune, Metts, & Ebesu Hubbard, 1998;
Davis, 1973; Dindia & Baxter, 1987). Repair has been approached in the
literature from both individual and dyadic standpoints (e.g., Duck, 1984;
Roloff & Cloven, 1994) as well as from a position of social networks and
the role they play in enabling the couple repair their relationship (Duck,
1984). For the purposes of this chapter, relational maintenance and repair
is later addressed with a focus on the relational partners. This focus does
not imply that social networks do not and cannot play a salient role in the
repair of a relationship. Indeed, they can and do. However, this
chapter conceptualizes the partners comprising the couple as the primary
unit and the social network as a source of support secondary to that pri-
mary relationship. Given this focus, Roloff and (1994) definition
of relational maintenance (and repair) is most appropriate for the direction
of this chapter. Specifically, this chapter focuses on the partners in the
close, personal relationship and their efforts, both individually or
dyadically, to maintain or repair their close relationship. Second, and as ar-
gued earlier, this chapter conceptualizes maintenance and repair as coex-
isting entities. That is, it is necessary to consider repair within the context
of maintenance and vice versa. Roloff and definition of relational
maintenance best suits the direction of this chapter because it melds the
constructs of relational maintenance and repair, and it recognizes and ad-
dresses the constructs similarly. Further, Roloff and definition of
maintenance focuses on individuals in intimate relationships and identifies
maintenance as an individual or joint activity. Second, Roloff and
definition of maintenance acknowledges the role of relational transgres-
sions as disrupting relational maintenance and recognizes the actions an in-
timate partner or partners take in an effort to repair a close relationship
that has been threatened by a transgression or series of transgressions. Spe-
cifically, Roloff and Cloven defined relational maintenance as, “the individ-
ual or joint approaches intimates take to limit the relational harm that may
result from prior or future conflicts and transgressions” (p. 27). Their con-
ceptualization of relational maintenance is adopted because it involves ef-
forts to correct past problems as well as engage in preventative measures to
keep the relationship going as smoothly as possible. This framing of rela-
tional maintenance closely aligns with Dindia and (1987) research
on relational maintenance and repair, which crafted relational mainte-
nance strategies as composing of corrective strategies (i.e., repair) and pre-
ventative strategies. In essence, preventative maintenance strategies are
188 -e=+ EMMERS-SOMMER

affected by prior relational repair episodes. With this framework in mind,


the following section examines a phenomenon that can conjoin the con-
structs of maintenance and repair, relational transgressions.

TRANSGRi3SlONS

This section of the chapter addresses three aspects of relational transgres-


sions. First, a higher order discussion of transgressions is presented. It is ar-
gued that transgressions can be incidental or incremental. That is, a
transgression can comprise a single incident or episode or reflect a cumula-
tive process. Second, specific types of transgressions are presented. Third,
responses to transgressions are addressed. To begin, the various forms of
transgressions are presented.
Transgressions can take the form of a social transgression or a relational
transgression (Metts, 1994). Social transgressions involve the violation of
some socially accepted rule, convention, or practice. For example, not ac-
cepting hand to shake when it is extended in a greeting repre-
sents a social transgression. Relational transgressions, on the other hand,
involve the violation of relational rules and expectancies (e.g., Emmers &
Canary, 1996; Metts, 1994; Roloff & Cloven, 1994). Examples of these
rules and expectancies might be explicit or implicit (Metts, 1994). Al-
though certain rules exist that generalize to most close, romantic relation-
ships (e.g., monogamy), other rules can be negotiated between relational
partners that are relationship specific. For example, many couples in close
relationships accept and practice monogamy as rule in their relationships.
Beyond those implicit rules and expectancies, partners can negotiate rules
and expectancies that are specific to their relationship. Examples of such
rules and understandings might include not discussing past partners or re-
lationships, not discussing in-laws, not going to bed angry, or not discussing
relational problems with friends or co-workers. Interestingly, what can be
drawn from the definitions of what constitutes a relational transgression is
that an act or series of acts that might qualify as a transgression need not
necessarily be negative. Indeed, a positive act or event might nonetheless
violate a relational rule and represent a transgression. Research by Afifi and
Metts (1998), f or example, examined relational expectancy violations and
responses to such violations. The authors argued and found that relational
expectancy violations can be positive or negative in valence. They created a
nine-category typology of violations that included both positive and nega-
tive expectancy violations. One category of Afifi and typology in-
volved relational transgressions. The authors noted that their
conceptualization of relational transgressions involved “behaviors that in-
volve a clear violation of taken-for-granted relational rules” (p. 377). Yet,
this author contends that a positive expectancy violation can constitute a
9. REPAIR AFTER TRANSGRESSION ++t- 18:

relational transgression. An illustration of this argument might involve a


couple that has negotiated a relational rule to keep things “out in the open”
and to “not have any surprises.” Yet, one partner organizes a surprise party
for the other to celebrate a landmark birthday. Whereas many would per-
ceive such a gesture as kind and generous, the partner on the receiving end
of this gesture might interpret the surprise party as violating the
no-surprises rule. This notion is further addressed in a latter section on
transgression types.

R&tiot-d Transgressions as Incident or Increment

As previously identified, transgressions can take on a variety of forms. It


must also be recognized that transgressions can take the form of a single in-
cident or represent a cumulative process. Duck (1994), for instance, ar-
gued that events can represent ongoing relational processes constituted in
everyday talk. Emmers (1995) f ound that both positive and negative
events in romantic relationships took the form of either an isolated inci-
dent or a process that evolved over time (see Table 9.1 for a listing of
events). Thus, the violation of a relational rule could occur in the form on
an instantaneous behavior (e.g., an act of infidelity). Alternately, a rela-
tional rule could slowly be chipped away over time. For example, assume
that a couple has negotiated the rule to be open and honest with one an-
other. Over time, this rule slowly erodes as disclosures become more infre-
quent and less detailed; this erosion would constitute a relational
transgression. Similarly, research on responses to troubled relationships
also suggests that behavior can be instantaneous or cumulative.
(1984, 1985) research on termination strategies, for example, illustrates
that termination behaviors can be long in the coming (e.g., cost escalation)
or instantaneous (e.g., fait accompli). Cost escalation involves an individ-
ual making the relationship costly over time for the partner. For example,
the individual might become increasingly distant or difficult toward the
partner. Fait accompli, on the other hand, involves ending the relationship
in an abrupt manner. In sum, what can be concluded from the literature on
what instantly affects relationships adversely or what slowly erodes at rela-
tional maintenance is that transgressions can be framed as either an inci-
dent or as an incremental process.
In addition to the violation of relational rules, it is important to note
that the culmination of everyday interaction in close relationships can
also be trying because the interactions are often adverse in some respect
(e.g., Miller, 1997). Similarly, Afifi and Metts (1998) concurred that re-
lational expectancy violations need not necessarily constitute a single
event. Miller contended that the everyday interaction between inti-
mates, although not intentional, can be fraught with unexpected “hassles,
TABLE 9.1
supraordinate and Subordinate Categories of Positive and Negative
Events and Processes

Positive Events

1. Commitment

Cohabiting

Future together

Loss of virginity

Commit to each other

Propose/plan marriage

2. Physical Separation

Can manage

Partner is “Mr./MS. Right”

Managed dating others

Balanced relationship, work,


school

Abstinence

Managed third parties


3. Realize Relationship
is Temporary

Partner is not “Mr./MS. Right”

4. Expression of Feelings

Expressed feelings

Expressed love

5. Acceptance from Network

Family/friends were accepting

6. Break Up/Trial Separation


Broke up

7. Trial separation

Unfaithfulness/Infidelity

Unfaithfulness

Infidelity
--
Negative Events

1. Substance Abuse

Illicit drugs

Alcohol

2. Deceptive Practices

Unfaithfulness

Infidelity

Lying

Flirting

3. Distance

Physical separation

Psychological separation
(avoidance, ignoring, break up)

Psychological separation
(fear of intimacy)

4. Deviant Behavior

Sexual practices

Personal past

5. Inhibiting emotions

Jealousy

Suspicion/lack of trust
(continued on next page)
191
I,$?. -is+ EMMERS-SOMMER

TABLE 9.1 (continued)

Worries

Stress

J,ack of motivation

Possessiveness

6. Aggression

Violence

Attitude

7, Third Party

Third party

Others

8. Miscellaneous

Rekindling

No identity

Pornography

Rape

Boredom

Stealing

Pet died

frustrations, nuisances, and disappointments that relational partners im-


pose on one another” (p. 14). 0 ver time, these oversights and uninten-
tional behaviors can challenge the preservation of a close relationship.
Indeed, it could be argued that everyday irritants and hassles provide
strong fodder for a transgression to erupt. Similarly, everyday nuisances
could evolve into a transgression.
9. REPAIR AFTER TRANSGRESSION +=s- 193

Types 06 Relational Transgressions

Within the context of personal relationships, a variety of relational trans-


gression types have been identified. Transgressions have been described us-
ing an abundance of terminologies, including uncertainty arousing events
(e.g., Emmers & Canary, 1996; Planalp & Honeycutt, 1985; Planalp,
Rutherford & Honeycutt, 1988), negative events (e.g., Emmers-Sommer,
1999), negative relational turning points (e.g., Baxter & Bullis, 1986), be-
trayals (Jones, Moore, Schratter, & Negel, ZOOO), face threats (e.g., Metts,
1997) relational expectancy violations (Afifi & Metts, 1998), and prob-
lematic events (e.g., Samp & Solomon, 1998), to name a few. Although
various forms of relational transgressions exist, the literature is consistent
in identifying infidelity and unfaithfulness as the most frequently reported
relational transgressions in close, romantic relationships (Metts, 1994).
Indeed, a variety of messages conveyed in our close relationships are
perceived as hurtful (Vangelisti, 1994). In particular, individuals experi-
ence hurt when a person close to the communicates a message that reflects
a devaluation of the relationship (Leary, Springer, Negel, Ansell, & Evans,
1998). Both intentional and unintentional messages hurt, but intentional
messages have more of a distancing effect on the relationship (Vangelisti &
Young, 2000). Research examining intentional versus unintentional trans-
gressions indicates that intentional transgressions are perceived more nega-
tively (e.g., Manstead & Semin, 198 1). The fact that offenders experience
less guilt when their transgression is intentional (vs. accidental) likely adds
to the violated sting (McGraw, 1987). Overall, such behavior
contributes to relational breakdown and the need for reparation.

Responses to Relational Transgressions

Duck (1984) offers two key questions regarding the situations that corre-
spond with relational breakdown and the processes by which individuals
rebuild the relationship: “What breaks down when a relationship breaks
down? How does the answer to that question help to define the corre-
sponding goals of repair interventions?” (p. 163). These issues are impor-
tant when considering responses to a transgression. Specifically, who or
what broke down the relationship in some respect affects goals re-
garding repair interventions (Samp & Solomon, 1998). Similarly, emo-
tional response to the transgression might affect how reparation might be
approached.
As noted earlier, transgressions can take a social form or a relational
form (Metts, 1994). Emotional responses to the transgression also vary,
depending on whether the transgression was social or relational in nature.
Specifically, in the event of a transgression, an individual could feel embar-
I$-?4 +- EMMERS-SOMMER

rassed, could experience guilt or shame, or not really care at all. It is likely
that an emotional experience in light of a transgression will re-
late to how he or she responds to it in terms of reparation.
In a phenomenological examination of guilt and shame, Tangney (1998)
observed the two emotions to be distinct such that shame involved a focus
on the self, and guilt resulted in a focus on particular behaviors. Tangney
also found that motivations in interpersonal relationships differed due to
experiencing either of these emotions, with guilt leading to more adapta-
tion in response to transgressions. In an empirical study, Tangney (1992)
found that guilt was typically aroused by moral transgressions whereas
shame was aroused by both moral (e.g., engaging in deception) and
nonmoral transgressions (e.g., personal failure in a performance situation).
Although both shame and guilt aroused concern about how this
might affect the partner, only shame was related to concern about the part-
evaluation of the offender. This conclusion makes sense given that
someone who committed a moral transgression was not being sensitive to
the feelings in the first place, thus the offender is likely not con-
cerned with the evaluation of him or her. On the other hand, if an
individual commits a nonmoral transgression (e.g., being late in attending
an important occasion for the partner) the individual is likely ashamed for
his or her tardiness and is concerned that the partner will think less of him
or her for the lack of consideration.
In an empirical investigation examining embarrassment, guilt, and
shame, Keltner and Buswell (1996) f ound results similar to
(1992). Specifically, the authors found that embarrassment was most of-
ten associated with transgressions involving social rules and conventions
that guide public interaction. Guilt most often occurred when the trans-
gression involved behaviors that violated responsibilities or behaviors that
harmed others. Similarly, Jones, Kugler, and Adams (1995) found that
guilt was associated with relational transgressions but not nonrelational
transgressions. Finally, shame resulted when the transgression involved a
failure to meet salient personal standards (e.g., being reliable, being
prompt; Keltner & Buswell, 1996). Other research also suggests that asso-
ciating shame with personal failure is consistent across individualistic (i.e.,
values individual goals over group goals) and collectivistic (i.e., values
group goals over individual goals) cultures (Stipek, 1998).
Overall, the emotions experienced by the offender could affect the re-
pair strategies enacted. It appears that experience of shame or embarrass-
ment most often results in the repair of the self. That is, shame results from
a personal failure and embarrassment results from the failure to adhere to a
social convention. Accordingly, personal adjustments must be made so as
not to embarrass or shame oneself. The experience of guilt, however, re-
fleets a situation whereas reparation the partner and repair of the rela-
tionship are in order as guilt is typica experienced due to harm infl icted
9. REPAIR AFTER TRANSGRESSION 31, y/5

on others, also, guilt feelings are abated when the transgression is inten-
tional (McGraw, 1987).
Overall, of the three emotions aroused due a transgression, guilt is
most tied to a relational transgression, although various emotions are ex-
perienced depending on the type of transgression committed. Below, var-
ious repair strategies enacted in response to a relational transgression are
reviewed.

Ri3-ATIONAL RIYAIR STKATEGIEi

This section focuses on several aspects of repair. First, a variety of relational


repair strategies will be offered. Second, the relationship between the
form of relational transgression committed and repair strategy chosen to
manage the transgression is addressed. Finally, the efficacy of relational re-
pair strategies enacted is reviewed.
Research suggests that if there was ever a relationship type in which
partners need to possess a vast repertoire of repair strategies, close roman-
tic relationships certainly qualify. Specifically, the research is clear that
close relational partners do not treat each other well (Birchler et al., 1975;
Miller, 199 1, 1997). Specifically, close relational partners often engage in
behaviors toward one another that they would make a conscious effort not
to engage in when in the company of nonintimates (e.g., being impolite, in-
sensitive, irresponsible, unreliable). And, despite the obvious need for re-
lational repair strategies, the research indicates that partners possess a
greater array of relational maintenance than relational repair strategies
(Dindia & Baxter, 1987).

Types 06 Relational Repair Strategies

Little exists on relational repair in terms of typologies of relational repair


(Dindia, 1994). D in d ia argued that her and work (Dindia &
Baxter, 1987) as well as (1973) work examined maintenance and re-
pair in the same breath. “Both Davis (1973) and Dindia and Baxter (1987)
defined rekztiond maintenance to include both strategies to maintain the
relationship (preventative maintenance) and strategies to repair the rela-
tionship (corrective maintenance)” (Dindia, 1994, p. 100). Dindia and
(1987) work rendered 11 supraordinate categories of mainte-
nance and repair strategies: changing the external environment, communi-
cation, metacommunication (e.g., relational talk), avoid
metacommunication, antisocial strategies, prosocial strategies, ceremo-
nies, antirituals/spontaneity, seeking/allowing autonomy, and seeking out-
side help. Of these strategy types, prosocial, ceremonial, communication
and togetherness strategies were used most. However, metacom-
136 -I+-+ EMMERS-SOMMER

munication strategies were used more when partners wanted to repair the
relationship, whereas spontaneity was more prevalent when the
desire is to maintain the relationship.
Although not labeled relational repair strategies per se, (e.g.,
1980a, 1980b) work examined responses to periodic episodes of decline in
close relationships. Inspired by interdependence theory, Rusbult argued in
her investment model of responses to relational decline that partners
choose responses depending on the levels of investment and satisfaction in
their close relationships as well as quality of alternatives to their close rela-
tionship. Collectively, Rusbult argued that these indicators affect an indi-
level of commitment to their partner and relationship. In turn,
level of commitment affects an response to periodic relational
decline. Specifically, in the event of relational decline, a partner can choose
to: (a) voice his or her dissatisfaction, (b) remain loyal to the partner and
relationship, (c) approach the partner and relationship in a neglectful man-
ner, or (d) engage in exit behaviors, which involve actually leaving the part-
ner and relationship or threatening to do so. Rusbult argued that each
response falls onto a constructive-destructive axis and a passive-active axis
at they relate to the preservation of the relationship. In a word, voice in-
volves an active response that is constructive to the preservation of the re-
lationship. Loyalty entails a passive response that is also constructive to the
preservation of the relationship. On the other hand, neglect reflects a pas-
sive response that is destructive to the preservation of the relationship and
exit involves an active response that is destructive to the preservation of
the relationship. It is important to note that the notions of constructive and
destructive within the context of model refer only to the preser-
vation of a relationship. Indeed, in the event of a dysfunctional relation-
ship, exiting might be the most constructive behavior one could enact in
terms of personal well-being. Nevertheless, the action of exiting is destruc-
tive to the preservation of the relationship.

Relational Repair Strateg Selection

A variety of issues affect response choices when a relational transgression


occurs. For example, attributions for the transgression and severity of the
offense are considered when examining response options to a transgression
(Metts, 1994). Similarly, goals after a transgression vary and
such goals affect response (e.g., Samp & Solomon, 1998). Specifically,
Samp and Solomon found that responses to a problematic event in close re-
lationships included maintaining the relationship, accepting fault for the
event, managing positive face, addressing the event, managing the conver-
sation, managing emotion, and restoring negative face. Accepting fault for
the event was the most frequent goal in both friendships and dating rela-
9. REPAIR AFTER TRANSGRESSION ==+ l,?;l

tionships. The authors also found that the goal to accept fault for the event
was intense and frequent, whereas the goal to avoid addressing the event
was not frequent.
As mentioned earlier, Rusbult (e.g., 1980a, 1980b, 1983) and Rusbult
and others (e.g., Rusbult, Drigotas, &Verette, 1994; Rusbult, Johnson, &
Morrow, 1986a; Rusbult & Verette, 199 1) clearly demonstrated that fac-
tors such as relational commitment, satisfaction, and alternatives to the
relationship affect choices partners make in response to a transgression.
Specifically, Rusbult (1987) indicated that partners who experience low
satisfaction, low investment, and a high quality of alternatives are in-
clined to respond to dissatisfaction with the response of exit. Partners
who experience low satisfaction, low investment, but a poor quality of al-
ternatives are inclined to respond with neglect. Conversely, partners who
experience high satisfaction, high investment, and a poor quality of alter-
natives are likely to respond to dissatisfaction with loyalty. Finally, part-
ners who experience high satisfaction with their close relationship, high
investment, and high quality of alternatives are likely to respond to rela-
tional dissatisfaction with voice. It is important to note, however, that the
relationship between quality of alternatives and the responses of voice or
neglect are weak at best (Rusbult, 1987).
Finally, aspects of an personality, such as levels of
self-esteem, affect repair strategies (Rusbult, 1987). For example, asser-
tive individuals are more likely than responsive individuals to assume con-
trol and exercise optimistic strategies when trying to repair a relationship.
Assertive partners were less likely to use sensitivity strategies, whereas re-
sponsive partners were more likely to engage in listening strategies
(Patterson & Beckett, 1995).
Strategies for managing relational problems can also vary by relationship
type (e.g., Canary & Stafford, 1994). For example, Emmers-Sommer
(1999) found that individuals were most likely to use integrative strate-
gies, as opposed to distributive or avoidance (i.e., passive and indirect)
strategies, when their goal was to repair their closest relationship after a
negative event. Integrative strategies involve partners discussing the mat-
ter in a constructive manner, not seeking concessions, and offering a neu-
tral evaluation of the partner. Distributive strategies involve engaging in
destructive behaviors that do seek concessions from the partner and can in-
volve behaviors such as negative attributions or threats. Finally, avoidance
strategies involve not discussing the issue. Sillars (1980a, 1980b), how-
ever, found that individuals in less close relationships (i.e., college room-
mates) were more inclined to use avoidance or distributive strategies than
integrative strategies in response to conflict. Thus, the type of relationship
one is engaged in as well as the importance of that relationship affect rela-
tional repair choices. Specifically, one can choose to begin dissolving the re-
lationship (e.g., Duck, 1984; Rusbult, 1983), to break off the relationship
108 +=s EMMERS-SOMMER

(e.g., Baxter, 1984, 1985), or to repair the relationship (e.g., Dindia &
Baxter, 1987; Duck, 1984).
Aune et al. (1998) examined a variety of relationship types varying in
closeness and found that repair strategies exercised varied by closeness.
Specifically, in a study of responses to the transgression of deception, Aune
et al. found that close partners (e.g., marrieds) were more likely to engage
in behaviors that communicated the positive aspects of their relationship
in an attempt to repair than were less close relational partners (e.g., co-
workers). Other research also demonstrates that deception is managed dif-
ferently according to relationship type (e.g., Metts, 1989).

Relational Repair Strateg Efficacy

Research on relational repair strategies indicates that partners do not per-


ceive all strategies equally and that some strategies are more effective than
others in remedying the ill brought about by a relational transgression. For
example, research indicates that voicing feelings in a constructive manner
to be beneficial to the preservation of close relationships (e.g., Gottman,
1994). For example, Gottman clearly demonstrated that partners who re-
spond to relational dissatisfaction and conflict in a constructive manner ex-
perience more satisfactory relationships. Constructive conflict
management behaviors include refraining from the use of behaviors such as
defensiveness, criticism, contempt, avoiding the issue, mindreading, or
making negative attributions toward the partner. Instead, Gottman argued
the benefits of engaging in voicing dissatisfaction without blaming, para-
phrasing a feelings to ensure accurate understanding of the part-
perspective, and focusing on behaviors rather than the individual
(e.g., avoiding negative personal attacks).
Other research findings concur that discussing the issues and the rela-
tionship in a constructive manner are beneficial to the preservation of the
relationship. Guided by an uncertainty reduction perspective, Emmers
and Canary (1996) examined what uncertainty reduction strategies (pas-
sive, active, and interactive) were enacted by couples in an effort to repair
their relationship. Interactive strategies involve directly talking to the part-
ner; active strategies involve seeking information from the partner from a
knowing third party or manipulating the environment to observe how the
partner reacts; finally, passive strategies involve observing the partner. The
authors added a fourth strategy category, assumed acceptance, for those
individuals who seemingly accepted the uncertainty arousing event and
made no efforts to reduce uncertainty. The authors found that romantic
couples most often engaged in the interactive communication strategy of
relational talk when the goal was to repair the relationship after
experiencing a negative event. This finding is similar to Dindia and
9. REPAIR AFTER TRANSGRESSION ~“77 199

(1987) finding that couples most often engage in relational talk strategies
when their goal is to repair the relationship. Similarly, Guerrero,
Andersen, Jorgensen, Spitzberg, and Eloy (1995) found that
use of integrative strategies to communicate jealousy resulted in more sat-
isfying relationships. Finally, Courtright, Millar, Rogers, and Bagarozzi
(1990) examined eight couples undergoing counseling due to their dis-
tressed marriages. Following the 6-week counseling sessions and three
taped marital discussions, these researchers found that the spouses who
engaged in direct communication and negotiation behaviors repaired their
marriage. However, the couples that engaged in avoidant, indirect, and de-
creased involvement behaviors terminated their marriages.
use of apologies, excuses, or justifications used in response to a
transgression has also been examined in the literature. Apologies entail the
offender admitting fault and expressing regret for the wrongdoing
(Hunter, 1984). E xcuses involve the offender admitting that the offense
occurred, but not accepting responsibility for the offense. Finally, justifica-
tions involve the offender admitting responsibility for the act, but denying
that the act was an offense (Hunter, 1984; Scott & Lymon, 1968). Hupka,
Jung, and Silverthorn (1987), f or example, found that apologies (e.g., “I
am sorry I was insensitive”) were the preferred response to a transgression,
regardless of intent. Excuses were perceived as weak accounts to a trans-
gression (e.g., been under a lot of stress”). Interestingly, justifications
(e.g., “Everyone loses their temper sometimes and is insensitive, no
different”) were rated the most negatively when the intent was to maintain
the relationship. However, justifications were rated more highly than ex-
cuses when the intent was to terminate the relationship. Transgressors ap-
praised justifications and apologies higher than the violated partners.
Hupka et al.‘s study only examined intent to maintain or terminate the re-
lationship, however, and did not examine when the intent was to repair the
relationship.
Overall, the prescription appears simple: Be nice to your partner to
maintain your relationship, and if you transgress, engage in prosocial, com-
municative behaviors to repair the relationship. Indeed, the research evi-
dence overwhelmingly suggests that engaging in some type of prosocial
behavior (e.g., being positive, talking about the relationship positively) and
engaging in direct, metacommunicative behavior strongly affects close re-
lationship repair (and maintenance) positively (e.g., Aune et al., 1998; Ca-
nary & Stafford, 1992; Dindia, 1989; Dindia & Baxter, 1997;
Emmers-Sommer, 1999; Emmers & Canary, 1996; Samp & Solomon,
1998; Stafford & Canary, 1991). Yet, we are well aware that relationships
are complex and are constantly evolving. Thus, a simple elixir to relational
problems is nonexistent. Nevertheless, the aforementioned findings do
suggest that certain reparations are more effective than others.
LOO -I+=+ EMMERS-SOMMER

CONCLUSION

The purpose of this chapter is to examine aspects of relational transgres-


sions and relational repair strategies. In doing so, evidence from the extant
research was presented and some extending arguments were offered. The
purpose of this section is to briefly offer conclusions from what was re-
viewed and presented in this chapter.
First, both relational transgressions and reparation strategies are
prevalent in close, personal relationships. As argued in this chapter, it is
necessary to perceive maintenance and repair in a coexistent fashion as
repair is corrective maintenance and maintenance strategies represent
preventative strategies such that the need for repair is lessened. Despite
many quest to find a close, intimate partner, they nonethe-
less often treat that close partner adversely or insensitively. This nega-
tive treatment often involves one or both engagement in
relational transgressions. Second, what constitutes a relational trans-
gression can be implicit or explicit in nature. That is, a transgression can
involve the violation of an implicit relational rule or expectancy such as
monogamy or an explicit, negotiated relational rule or expectancy (e.g.,
to not keep secrets from one another, to not go to bed angry). Third, and
related, a transgression can constitute a single incident or an incremen-
tal process. That is, a single event might (e.g., an act of infidelity) repre-
sent a transgression. On the other hand, a transgression might represent
the cumulative or incremental process of actions and interactions. For
example, the negotiated rule and expectancy to remain open and honest
with one another erodes over time. Fourth, a transgression can be posi-
tive or negative in valence. That is, an act that might be perceived as pos-
itive in nature might nevertheless violate a relational rule or expectancy
and thus constitute a transgression. Fifth, varied emotional reactions to
the transgressions can be experienced depending on the nature of the
transgression. Specifically, embarrassment is often experienced due to a
social transgression, whereas shame is often experienced due to a rela-
tional transgression due to a personal incompetence and guilt is often
experienced when a relational transgression affects the partner ad-
versely. Sixth, perceptions of the transgression within the context of
the importance of the relationship affect repair strategies enacted. That
is, individuals in close relationships who value and want to preserve
their relationships often engage in constructive, prosocial,
metacommunicative strategies when relational reparation is the goal.
Finally, and related, the efficacy of relational repair strategies varies.
Specifically, admission of fault and willingness to be open in
discussing the problem and the relationship is more efficacious than not
taking responsibility for a transgression and avoiding discussing the situ-
9. REPAIR AFTER TRANSGRESSION + 201

ation. In sum, it is evident that communication plays a central role in the


maintenance and repair processes of close relationships.

l=UTUR!E DIRKLTIONS

Dindia (1994) argued that numerous relational strategies are multiphasic


in nature. That is, certain strategies (e.g., relational talk) are useful during
various stages of relational development (e.g., initiation, development,
maintenance, repair). This contention assumes a phase or stage approach
to relationships. Yet, because relationships do not occur in a vacuum, there
is also movement within stages. Within the context of relational transgres-
sions in close relationships, one future direction of research might want to
examine how relational rules are negotiated and renegotiated over the
course of a relationship. Specifically, this chapter addresses how transgres-
sions occur when a relational rule or expectancy is violated by one or both
relational partners. Given the evolution of relationships, what might have
qualified as a transgression during one phase of a close relationship might
not qualify at a later phase of the relationship. For example, partners might
have negotiated “not talking about past partners” as a relational rule early in
their relationship. However, as the relationship develops and strengthens,
a topic that might have been perceived as a threat is no longer perceived in
that fashion. In fact, partners might eventually refer to past relationships
openly in their present relationship. Future research should examine the
negotiation and renegotiation of relational rules and how that process af-
fects perceptions of transgressions.
Another area worthy of consideration involves the burgeoning area of
new technologies and the onset of online relationships (see Rabby &
Walther, chap. 7, this volume). This new area in the interpersonal litera-
ture provides fertile ground for researchers to test existing interpersonal
theories, models, and typologies in an online environment. Several possible
questions exist that are worthy of exploration. For example, how do online
relational partners negotiate relational rules? How do online partners iden-
tify and manage a transgression ? Also, how do individuals in primary,
face-to-face relationships perceive a “involvement” with some-
one online? Research suggests that whereas some individuals perceive on-
line relationships as real (e.g., Parks & Roberts, 1998), others perceive
them as nonreal (e.g., Walther, 1996). Depending on the nature of the
face-to-face relationship, then, the perception of whether or not a trans-
gression has been committed in the primary relationship due to a
actions in the online relationship will vary. Overall, the development,
maintenance, and repair of online relationships are fast-growing aspects of
society and provide a new environment (i.e., the online domain) in which
to examine relational processes.
202 w EMMERS-SOMMER

Afifi, W A., & Metts, S. (1998). Characteristics and consequences of expectation viola-
tions in close relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 1.5,
365-392.
Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration. New York: Holt, Rinehart, &
Winston.
Aune, R. K., Metts, S ., & Ebesu Hubbard, A. S. (1998). Managing outcomes of discov-
ered deception. The lournul of Social Psychology, 138, 677-689.
Baxter, L. A. (1984). Trajectories of relationship disengagement. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 1, 29-48.
Baxter, L. A. (1985). A ccomplishing relationship disengagement. In S. Duck & D.
Perlman (Eds.), Understanding personal relationships: An interdisciplinary ap-
proach (pp. 243-265). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Baxter, L. A. (1986). Gender differences in the heterosexual relationship rules embed-
ded in breakup accounts, Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 3,289-306.
Baxter, L. A. (1994). A dialogic approach to relational maintenance. In D. J. Canary &
L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational maintenance (pp. 233-254). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Baxter, L. A., & Bullis, C. (1986). T urning points in developing romantic relationships.
Human Communication Research, 12, 469-493.
Birchler, G. R., Weiss, R. L., &Vincent, J. I? (1975). M u It imethod analysis of social re-
inforcement exchange between martially distressed and nondistressed spouse and
stranger dyads. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 349-360.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1992). Relational maintenance strategies and equity in
marriage. Communication Monographs, 59, 239-267.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1994). M am f t aining relationships through strategic and
routine interaction. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and rela-
tional maintenance (pp. 3-22). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Courtright, J. A., Millar, F. E., Rogers, L. E., & Bagarozzi, D. (1990). Interaction dynam-
ics of relational negotiation: Reconciliation versus termination of distressed rela-
tionships. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 54, 429-453.
Davis, M. S. (1973). Intimate relations. New York: The Free Press.
Dindia, K. (1994). A multiphasic view of relationship maintenance strategies. In D. J.
Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational maintenance (pp.
99-l 12). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Dindia, K., & Baxter, L. A. (1987). Strategies for maintaining and repairing marital rela-
tionships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 4, 143-158.
Duck, S. W (1994). Meaningful relationships: Talking, sense, and relating. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Duck, S. W (1984). A perspective on the repair of personal relationships: Repair of
what, when? In S. W. Duck (Ed.), Personal relationships 5: Repairingpersonal rela-
tionships (pp. 163-l 84). London: Academic Press.
Duck, S. W (1988). Relating to others. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Emmers, T. M. (1995). Th e p revalence of uncertainty in romantic relationships: Exam-
ining instrumentality in the uncertainty reduction process. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Ohio University.
Emmers, T. M., &Canary, D. J. (1996). Th e e ff ec t o f uncertainty reducing strategies on
young relational repair and intimacy. Communication Quarterly, 44,
166-182.
9. REPAIR AFTER TRANSGRESSION ++i- 203

Emmers-Sommer, T M. (1999). Negative relational events and event responses across


relationship-type: Examining and comparing the impact of conflict strategy-use on
intimacy in same-sex friendships, opposite-sex friendships, and romantic relation-
ships. Communication Research Reports, 16, 286-295.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Guerrero, L. K., Eloy, S. V, & Wabnik, A. L. (1993). Linking maintenance strategies to
relationship development and disengagement: A reconceptualization. Journal of So-
cial and Personal Relationships, 10, 273-283.
Guerrero, L. K., Andersen, I? A., Jorgensen, P F., Spitzberg, B. H., & Eloy, S. V: (1995).
Coping with the green-eyed monster: Conceptualizing and measuring communica-
tive responses to romantic jealousy. Western Journal of Communication, 59,
270-304.
Hunter, C. H. (1984). Aligning actions: Types and social distribution. Symbolic Interac-
tion, 7, 155-174.
Hupka, R. B., Jung, J., & Silverthorn, K. (1987). P erceived acceptability of apologies,
excuses, and justifications in jealousy predicaments. Journ.& ofBehavior and Person-
ality, 2, 303-313.
Jones, W H., Kugler, K., & Adams, I? (1995). Y ou always hurt the ones you love: Guilt
and transgressions against relational partners. In J. I? Tangney & K. W Fischer (Eds.),
Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride
(pp. 301-321). N ew York: Guilford.
Jones, W H., Moore, D. S., Schratter, A., & Negel, L. A. (2001). Interpersonal trans-
gressions and betrayals. In R. M. Kowalski (Ed.), Behaving badly: Aversive behaviors
in interpersonal relationships (pp. 233-256). Washington, DC: American Psycho-
logical Association.
Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1996). E vi d ence for the distinctiveness of embarrass-
ment, shame, and guilt: A study of recalled antecedents and facial expressions of
emotion. Cognition &: Emotion, 10, 155-l 71.
Leary, M. R., Springer, C., Negel, L., Ansell, E., & Evans, K. (1998). The causes, phe-
nomenology, and consequences of hurt feelings. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74, 1225-1237.
Manstead, A. S ., & Semin, G. R. (198 1). Social transgressions, social perspectives, and
social emotionality. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 249-261.
McGraw, K. M. (1987). Guilt following transgression: An attribution of responsibility
approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 247-256.
Metts, S. (1989). An exploratory investigation of deception in close relationships. Jour-
nal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6, 159-l 79.
Metts, S. (1994). Relational transgressions. In W R. Cupach & B. H. Spitzberg (Eds.),
The dark side of interpersonal communication (pp. 217-239). Hillsdale, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum Associates.
Metts, S. (1997). Face and facework: Implications for the study of personal relation-
ships. In S. W Duck (Ed.), Handbook ofpersonal relationships: Theory, research and
interventions (2nd ed., pp. 373-390). Chichester, England: Wiley.
Miller, R. S. (1991). 0 n d ecorum in close relationships: why we polite to those
we love? Contemporary Social Psychology, 13, 74-76.
Miller, R. S. (1997). We always hurt the ones we love: Aversive interactions in close re-
lationships. In R. W. Kowalski (Ed.), Aversive interpersonal behaviors (pp. 1 l-29).
New York: Plenum Press.
Parks, M. R., & Roberts, L. D. (1998). “Making MOOsic”: The development of per-
sonal relationships on line and a comparison to their off-line counterparts. lournal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 1.5, 5 17-537.
LOS w EMMERS-SOMMER

Patterson, B. R., & Beckett, C. S. (1995). A re - examination of relational repair and rec-
onciliation: Impact of socio-communicative style on strategy selection. Communi-
cation Research Reports, 12, 235-240.
Planalp, S., & Honeycutt, J. M. (1985). E vents that increase uncertainty in personal re-
lationships. Human Communication Research, 11, 593-604.
Planalp, S., Rutherford, D. K., & Honeycutt, J. M. (1988). Events that increase uncer-
tainty in personal relationships II: Replication and extension. Human Communica-
tion Research, 14, 5 16-547.
Roloff, M. E., & Cloven, D. H. (1994). When partners transgress: Maintaining violated
relationships. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational
maintenance (pp. 23-43). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Rusbult, C. E. (1980a). Commitment and satisfaction in romantic associations: A test
of the investment model.JournaZofExperimentaZSociaZPsychoZogy 2 6, 172-l 86.
Rusbult, C. E. (1980b). Satisfaction and commitment in friendships. Representative
Research in Social Psychology, 11, 96-l 05.
Rusbult, C. E. (1983). A longitudinal test of the investment model: The development
(and deterioration) of satisfaction and commitment in heterosexual involvements.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 10 l-l 17.
Rusbult, C. E. (1987). Responses to dissatisfaction in close relationships: The
exit-voice-loyalty-neglect model. In D. Perlman & S. Duck (Eds.), Intimate rela-
tionships: Development, dynamics, and deterioration (pp. 209-237). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Rusbult, C. E., Drigotas, S. M., & Verette, J. (1994). The investment model: An inter-
dependence analysis of commitment processes and relationship maintenance phe-
nomena. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational
maintenance (pp. 115-l 39). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Rusbult, C. E., Johnson, D. J., & Morrow, G. D. (1986). Impact of couple patterns of
problem solving on distress and nondistress in dating relationships. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 50, 744-753.
Rusbult, C. E., & Verette, J. (199 1). An interdependence analysis of accommodation
processes in close relationships. Representative Research in Social Psychology, 19,
3-33.
Samp, J. A., & Solomon, D. H. (1998). C ommunicative responses to problematic
events in close relationships I: The variety and facets of goals. Communication Re-
search, 25, 66-95.
Scott, M. B., & Lymon, S. M. (1968). Accounts. American Sociological Review, 33,
46-62.
Sillars, A. L. (1980a). The sequential and distributional structure of conflict interac-
tions as a function of attributions concerning the locus of responsibility and stability
of conflicts. In D. Nimmo (Ed.), C ommunication Yearbook 4 (pp. 217-235). New
Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books.
Sillars, A. L. (1980b). Attributions and communication in roommate conflicts. Com-
munication Monographs, 4 7, 180-200.
Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1991). Maintenance strategies and romantic relationship
type, gender and relational characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 8, 217-242.
Stipek, D. (1998). D i ff erences between Americans and Chinese in the circumstances
of evoking pride, shame, and guilt. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29,
616-629.
Tangney, J. P (1992). Situational determinants of shame and guilt in young adulthood.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 199-206.
9. REPAIR AFTER TRANSGRESSION +=s 205

Tangney, J. l? (1998). How does guilt and shame differ? In J. Bybee (Ed.), Guilt and
children (pp. l-l 7). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Vangelisti, A. L. (1994). Messages that hurt. In W. R. Cupach & B. H. Spitzberg (Eds.),
The dark side of interpersonal communication (pp. 53-82). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Vangelisti, A. L, & Young, S. L. (2000). Wh en words hurt: The effects of perceived
intentionality on interpersonal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relu-
tionships, 17, 393-424.
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interper-
sonal, and hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23, 3-43.
This page intentionally left blank
Cultural Variations
in Maintaining ReIationships
+t++ w
This page intentionally left blank
Stephen M. Haas
University of Cincinnati

n the past 15 years, communication scholars have begun to explore the


strategies and behaviors that couples use to maintain romantic relationships
(see Canary & Stafford, 1992, 1994; Dainton & Stafford, 1993; Dindia &
Baxter, 1987; Stafford & Canary, 1991; Stafford, Dainton, & Haas, 2000).
Relationship maintenance in romantic relationships can be understood as
employing communicative strategies and behaviors that prevent relation-
ship dissolution through efforts to sustain a dynamic equilibrium in
their relationship definition and satisfaction levels as they cope with the ebb
and flow of everyday relating” (Baxter & Dindia, 1990, p. 188). The existing
research has done much to increase our understanding of romantic relation-
ship maintenance, but primarily in one demographic group-American,
White, middle class, heterosexual married or dating couples. Many ques-
tions remain concerning the generalizability of this research to couples that
differ by class, race, ethnicity, or sexual orientation.
Gay and lesbian relationships have been recognized as being understud-
ied by researchers across academic disciplines (Huston & Schwartz, 1995;
Ossana, 2000). Clark and Serovich (1997), for example, highlighted that
studies of gay, lesbian, and bisexual issues make up roughly 0.006% of the
family therapy literature. A primary reason for this lack of research is that

20.9
gay and lesbian relationships have been viewed as deviant in U.S. society.
As a result, lesbian and gay male couples experience social stigma and dis-
crimination throughout their lives (Goffman, 1963; McWhirter &
Mattison, 1984). Also Huston and Schwartz (1995) observed that:

The lack of institutional recognition for homosexual couples plays a very power-
ful role in their stability. Heterosexual unions are sanctioned by the church and
the state through the marriage ceremony. The state rewards such unions with
family health insurance, property rights when breakups occur, and institutional
prerogatives such as untaxed inheritance and the right to distribute property af-
ter a death. (p. 114)

Four forms of stigma most profoundly affect gay and lesbian relation-
ships: (a) ignorance-a lack of knowledge of gay and lesbian lifestyles, (b)
homophobia-a persistent fear of homosexuals based in ignorance, (c) prej-
udice-forming negative attitudes towards gays and lesbians as a group,
and (d) oppression-legal and social actions that deny equal treatment and
rights to gays and lesbians (McWhirter & Mattison, 1982). All of these stig-
matizing attitudes and behaviors can have a serious impact on the lives of
gay and lesbian couples, such as loss of employment or housing; rejection
from family, friends, and co-workers; as well as, verbal or physical assault.
What can be particularly damaging is when stigma is internalized by lesbi-
ans and gay men (Lynch, 1987; McWhirter & Mattison, 1982).
Self-oppression (learned and internalized antigay prejudice) can result in
devastating emotional effects for gays and lesbians (e.g., low self-esteem,
embarrassment, social isolation, unwillingness to self-disclose, and even a
lack of comfort in their intimate relationships; Laird, 1993; Ossana, 2000).
These forms of stigma may create internal barriers to establishing and main-
taining successful, long-term, same-sex relationships. Also, lacking the legal
and social validation that binds married couples forces same-sex relation-
ships to rely largely on emotional commitment to maintain them.
Moreover, gay and lesbian individuals may refrain from disclosing their sex-
ual orientation to others for fear of rejection. A lack of openness (popularly
referred to as being “in the closet”) can cause particular problems for
same-sex couples in the form of added relational stress and isolation (Berger,
1990; Haas, 2002; Patterson & Schwartz, 1994). Thus, the absence of legal
and social barriers that help prevent relationship termination in marital cou-
ples make relationship maintenance all the more challenging in same-sex re-
lationships (Attridge, 1994; Patterson & Schwartz, 1994).
Because research on relationship maintenance in same-sex couples has
been sparse to date, this chapter first focuses on research that has explored
characteristics of gay and lesbian relationships that impact maintenance
(e.g., relational quality and satisfaction, sex-role ideology, power dynam-
ics, etc.,). In addition, study findings that address societal assumptions
concerning differences between heterosexual and same-sex relationships
10. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE IN SAME-SEX COUPLES -+I- 211

are discussed. Furthermore, a discussion of communication-based rela-


tionship maintenance research is addressed; highlighting three recent
studies, in particular, that have explored the communicative strategies and
behaviors utilized in maintaining same-sex relationships. Finally, sugges-
tions for future research on relationship maintenance in same-sex couples
will be proposed.

REXARCH ON GAY AND LESBIAN RELATIONSHIPS

Early studies viewed gay men and lesbian women as “perverts” and “devi-
ants” within society. It was not until 1973, when the American Psychiatric
Association removed homosexuality from its list of pathological illnesses,
that researchers began to rethink the study of gays and lesbians. In the late
197Os, researchers began to shift their focus away from “studying homo-
sexuality exclusively from the perspective of toward studying
homosexuality as part of work on ‘alternative or ‘sex
(Peplau, 1982, p. 3).
Despite this shift, much of the early research was grounded in
heterosexist assumptions regarding gay and lesbian lifestyles. One assump-
tion was that gays and lesbians were more sexually promiscuous than het-
erosexuals and were unable to establish and maintain meaningful,
long-term, intimate relationships. The language used in early studies to de-
scribe ongoing same-sex relationships was indicative of this underlying as-
sumption. For example, in studies by Saghir and Robins (1973) and Bell
and Weinberg (1978), ongoing same-sex relationships were referred to
merely as affairs. In their survey of 4,639 gay men and women, Bell and
Weinberg (1978) ex pl ained that “virtually all of the male respondents had
been involved in at least one affair (defined as a ‘relatively steady relation-
with another man) [italics added] during the course of their lives” (p.
86). These researchers also considered only gays and lesbians who lived to-
gether to be “coupled,” and even then, they described these subjects as
“roommates” (p. 9 1). In yet another study, Weinberg and Williams (1974)
interviewed 1,057 gay men about their sexual activity but failed to ask if
any of the men considered their relationships to be long-term. Despite the
fact that most gay men and lesbian women in these early studies reported
wanting to establish a relationship (Bell & Weinberg, 1978; Harry, 1982;
Jay & Young, 1977), researchers operated under the assumption that
long-term gay and lesbian relationships were rare.
By the 198Os, researchers began to realize that gays and lesbians do estab-
lish long-term relationships (Blumstein & Schwartz, 1983; Kurdek &
Schmitt, 1986b; Mendola, 1980). For instance, in a sample of 405 gay men
and lesbian women, Mendola (1980) f ound that 63% of the men and 70% of
the women reported being in a committed, “marriage-like” relationship.
Peplau and Cochran (1981) and Lewis, Kozac, Milardo, and Grosnick
(198 1) found that gays and lesbians seek out long-term relationships for the
same reasons as heterosexuals: love, commitment, and companionship. Fur-
thermore, Dailey (1979) investigated the heterosexist assumption “that ho-
mosexuals may love each other, but the love expressed is and
really not love at all” (p. 155). Using the Caring Relationship Inventory
(CRI), Dailey f ound no difference between same-sex and heterosexual as-
sessments of love within these relationships. Furthermore, Dailey found
that married heterosexual couples showed greater discrepancy in dyadic co-
hesion than the same-sex couples. Similarly, Peplau and Cochran (198 1)
found that in comparing a sample of 50 lesbians, 50 gay men, 50 heterosex-
ual women, and 50 heterosexual men, there were no differences in feelings
of love or relationship satisfaction. Also, in comparing relationship adjust-
ment and degree of love and liking, again they found no differences (al-
though lesbians and gay men reported higher degrees of positive feelings for
their partners than heterosexuals). In addition, Peplau (1991) found that
gay, lesbian, and heterosexual relationship likes and dislikes were very simi-
lar. In fact, a panel of judges blinded to the sexual orientation of respondents
were unable to differentiate between open-ended responses in this study.
In general, gay men, lesbian women, and heterosexuals have reported fairly
equivalent levels of relational satisfaction on standardized measures such as
Dyadic Adjustment Scale (see Dailey, 1979; Duffy & Rusbult,
1986; Kurdek & Schmitt, 1985-1986, 1986a, 1986b, 1987). Kurdek and
Schmitt (1986b) also found that relationship quality for same-sex and hetero-
sexual couples revolved around similar dimensions: a high level of dyadic at-
tachment, few relationship alternatives, shared decision making, and holding
few beliefs that disagreements are destructive to the relationship. For gay and
lesbian couples, Kurdek (1988, 1989) found that the most important predic-
tors of relationship quality were a focus on trust, similarity, and intrinsic moti-
vation. Emotional expressiveness and equality of power were particularly
important for relationship quality in lesbian couples, but overall, no significant
differences in relationship commitment or quality were found between gay
male and lesbian couples (Kurdek, 1988, 1989).

Sexual PXClUSi”itLJ
The societal assumption that gays and lesbians are more sexually promiscuous
than heterosexuals has been fueled by findings in several studies that some gay
and lesbian relationships negotiate open sexual agreements (Blumstein &
Schwartz, 1983; Kurdek & Schmitt, 1985-1986; Wagner, Remien, &
Carballo-Dieguez, 1998). Sexual exclusivity, or monogamy, has long been ap-
plied as the model for heterosexual relationships by church and state. Despite
this ideal, studies of heterosexual sexual behavior indicate that rates of marital
10. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE IN SAME-SEX COUPLES +=+I- 213

infidelity range from 26% to 70% for women and from 33% to 75% for men
(Buss, 1994; Fisher, 1987; Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948; Kinsey,
Pomeroy, Martin, & Gebhard, 1953; Shackelford & Buss, 1997).
Like heterosexuals, research has found that many same-sex couples
strive to maintain monogamous relationships as a model (Berger, 1990;
Bell & Weinberg, 1978; Blumstein & Schwartz, 1983; Kurdek, 1986b;
Mendola, 1980; Peplau, 1991). For example, Mendola (1980) found that
83% of the lesbian women in her study reported being monogamous. Simi-
larly, in their study, Fitzpatrick, Jandt, Myrick, and Edgar (1994) found
that “70 percent of gay males and 80 percent of lesbians had never broken
their monogamy agreement” (p. 273). According to Tuller (1978), the
question of whether to maintain a monogamous relationship:

is more likely to come up in a homosexual relationship than in a heterosexual one


because homosexuals have no model to imitate as do heterosexuals. Further, the
socialization process does not teach what a good gay marriage should be like as it
does with heterosexual marriages. Some gay people use the heterosexual model
and remain monogamous; some are influenced by religious doctrines and remain
monogamous; and others simply do whatever works best for their particular rela-
tionships. (p. 336)

Berger (1990) f ound that gay male monogamy may have been increased
by fear of HIV infection since the onset of the AIDS epidemic. Berger
(1990) reported that “Of 83 couples who responded . . . 96.4% described
their relationships as monogamous. This differs from pre-AIDS surveys
which showed that only a minority of gay couples were strictly monoga-
mous” (p. 44). The AIDS crisis has less directly affected monogamy among
lesbian women because they are at lower risk for HIV infection within
their relationships (Carl, 1986; Fitzpatrick et al., 1994). However, AIDS
has lead to the loss of friends and relatives for many lesbian women, and as
a result, AIDS-related causes have become a human rights issue for many
politically active lesbians and gay men in general.
Nonmonogamy has been found to be more prevalent in gay male couples
than lesbian relationships (Green, Bettinger, & Zachs, 1996). In a recent
study of 75 gay male couples in New York City in which one partner was
HIV positive, Wagner et al. (1998) f ound that 50 of the couples (67%) re-
ported engaging in at least one sexual encounter outside of the primary re-
lationship in the last year. Mendola (1980) found that 49% of the gay males
in her study admitted to having an occasional sexual experience outside
their primary relationship, which is quite similar to the 43% of heterosex-
ual married men in Pietropinto and (1979) study who admit-
ted infidelity. Regardless of sexual orientation, studies have found that
some men do not view outside sexual activity as being tied to emotional
commitment to their primary relationship (Blumstein & Schwartz, 1983;
Glass & Wright, 1985; Ossana, 2000; Thompson, 1984; Wagner et al.,
1998). Lee (199 1) pointed out that the ability of same-sex couples to ne-
gotiate open or closed sexual agreements may increase levels of trust and
relational satisfaction, as well as helping to reduce feelings of betrayal often
experienced with marital infidelity.
Overall, research is suggesting that the predominant focus by research-
ers on the dichotomy of open versus closed relationships may in fact be of
less significance in understanding same-sex relationships. For instance,
studies on relationship quality have indicated no difference between sexu-
ally-open versus monogamous gay male couples (Blasband & Peplau, 1985;
Blumstein & Schwartz, 1983; Kurdek & Schmitt, 1985-l 986). Spe-
cifically, Kurdek and Schmitt (1985-l 986) found that sexually open and
closed gay male couples were actually more similar than different in psy-
chological adjustment, dyadic attachment, a positive belief in partner
changability, and relationship quality.
Similarly, some studies have shown no difference in relationship satisfac-
tion or stability between sexually open versus closed heterosexual marriages
(Knapp, 1976; Knapp & Whitehurst, 1977; Ramey, 1975; Watson, 198 1). In
investigations of heterosexual couples who “swing” (an open agreement), no
evidence was found that swinging was harmful to marital or family stability
(Cole & Spanier, 1974; Gilmartin, 1972; Paulson & Paulson, 1971). In fact,
when Rubin (1982) sampled 130 sexually open and 130 sexually exclusive
married and divorced individuals, he found that those who were in sexually
open marriages were no more poorly adjusted than those that were not. For
the divorced couples, those who had been in sexually open marriages were
no more unhappy after the divorce than those in monogamous relationships.
Rubin (1982) concluded that “nothing in this data argues for the view that
sexual openness or exclusivity, in and of themselves, make a difference in the
overall adjustment of a married couple” (p. 107).
In general, the research on both same-sex and heterosexual couples pro-
vides evidence that when relational expectations for sexual behavior are
shared by partners, relationship quality and satisfaction are fairly equiva-
lent across sexually open and sexually exclusive couples. Despite societal
assumptions to the contrary, it appears that nonmonogamy becomes prob-
lematic in relationships only when that value is not shared by partners.

Another common assumption pertaining to gay and lesbian relationships is


the belief that stereotypical, heterosexual “hutch-femme” role-playing oc-
curs. The assumption that same-sex couples model themselves after het-
erosexual relationships so that one partner assumes a masculine role and
the other a feminine role has not been supported by research (Kurdek,
1987, 1993; Kurdek & Schmitt, 1985-1986, 1986a; Lynch & Reilly,
10. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE IN SAME-SEX COUPLES 3f 215

19851986). I n reviewing several studies, Peplau (1982) asserted that


“most contemporary gay and lesbian relationships do not conform to tradi-
tional (masculine and roles; instead role flexibility and
turn-taking are more common patterns” (p. 4). Across the studies, Peplau
found that, “traditional heterosexual marriage is not the predominant
model or script for current homosexual couples” (p. 4). In an early study,
Tuller (1978) f ound “all of the couples claimed that they did not have any
hutch-femme roles in their relationships-that, in fact, they shared house-
hold tasks and definitely did not sexually imitate conventional heterosex-
ual roles” (p. 340). In addition, Dailey (1979) found no evidence of
increased cross-gender endorsement in gay male or lesbian couples.
findings revealed that in instances when cross-gender roles were
found, they existed equally across same-sex and heterosexual couples. In
yet another study, Marecek, Finn, and Cardell (1983) found that when
cross-gender roles were present in same-sex couples, the frequency was
lower than within the heterosexual couples in the study.
In general, masculine and feminine sex roles have been found to be
blended in gay and lesbian relationships toward more androgynous roles
(Schullo & Alperson, 1984). In a direct comparison study, Kurdek (1987)
found that lesbian women tended to be more instrumental (task oriented)
than heterosexual women in their sex-role orientation; whereas gay and
heterosexual men were equivalent. Gay men were more expressive than
heterosexual men, but heterosexual and lesbian women were equivalent.
Also, Kurdek and Schmitt (1986a) f ound a fairly random and equal distri-
bution of pairings of gay, lesbian, and heterosexual partners across
sex-role inventory categories of masculine, feminine, undifferentiated,
and androgynous orientations. According to (1974) measure, mas-
culine individuals are more task oriented, hostile, dominant, egocentric,
temperamental, and low in nurturance. Feminine persons are more sub-
missive, dependent, and nurturing. Undifferentiated individuals tend to
be self-centered, withdrawn, depressed, possess underdeveloped social
skills, and are lacking in intimacy. Androgynous persons are more extro-
verted, empathic and flexible in their social skills, and high in
self-disclosure. In addition, Kurdek and Schmitt (1986a) found that an-
drogynous and feminine partners reported the highest levels of relation-
ship functioning in same-sex couples. Couples in which one or both
partners were undifferentiated or masculine reported the lowest relation-
ship quality (Kurdek & Schmitt, 1986a), and Reece and Segrist (1981)
found that more androgynous same-sex couples remained together longer
than those scoring low on cooperation.
Overall, the studies indicate that gay men and lesbian women tend to be
more androgynous in their gender-role orientation, and as such, the
hutch-femme myth in gay and lesbian relationships has not been sup-
ported. Marecek, Finn, and Cardell (1983) asserted that gender roles (e.g.,
instrumental vs. expressive) are likely not linked to biological sex, but
rather lie on a continuum much like personality traits. In a similar vein,
Schullo and Alperson (1984) suggested the need to relabel the poles of this
continuum from sex-related terms (i.e., masculine-feminine) to more ac-
curately reflect the instrumental and expressive qualities being assessed.

Power Dyna rrlics

Gay male and lesbian relationships seem to be based in relational expecta-


tions of equality and reciprocity (Blumstein & Schwartz, 1983; Kurdek &
Schmitt, 1986a; Lynch & Reilly, 1985-1986; McWhirter & Mattison,
1984; Peplau, Padesky, & Hamilton, 1982; Reilly & Lynch, 1990). Because
gay and lesbian relationships tend not adhere to traditional sex roles, issues
surrounding “power imbalances manifested in unequal influence in deci-
sion making, unfair division of household labor, or biased allotment of
rights, resources, and privileges” (Huston & Schwartz, 1995, p. 108) must
be negotiated in same-sex couples. In heterosexual couples, adherence to
sex-based role assignments often serve as relational scripts regarding
power and decision making (e.g., males assume the role of primary finan-
cial supporter and females adopt primary responsibility for household du-
ties). In gay and lesbian relationships, however, roles and relational duties
often are negotiated based on individual desires, skills, and schedule con-
straints (Kurdek, 1993).
Gay male and lesbian couples also have expressed equity in power dy-
namics as a relational ideal (Kurdek & Schmitt, 1986b; Peplau, Padesky, &
Hamilton, 1982; Reilly & Lynch, 1993). Th e means of establishing equity,
however, may differ slightly in gay and lesbian couples. For example,
Blumstein and Schwartz (1983) f ound that mutual dependency and egali-
tarian power sharing were important for relationship quality;
whereas, gay male couples reported that having similar education levels
and values and sharing financial responsibilities were important to their re-
lationship quality. Like heterosexual couples, Blumstein and Schwartz
(1983) found that gay male couples tended to use financial contribution to
the relationship as a guide for establishing decision-making power. They
found that male partners who earned more took on a more dominant role
in the relationship; although gay male couples were not satisfied if earnings
and power dynamics were greatly imbalanced. Additionally, in couples
where one partner was at least 5 years older or more, the older partner
tended to assume more power in couple decision making (Harry, 1982).
In lesbian couples, money apparently does not play as important a role in
determining decision making and power dynamics (Lynch & Reilly,
1985-l 986; Reilly & Lynch, 1990). Lesbian couples have been found to go
out of their way not to construct their relationships based on financial is-
10. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE IN SAME-SEX COUPLES ++t- 217

sues. Instead, lesbian couples describe more of a focus on emotional close-


ness (McCandlish, 1982), interdependence of lives, and relational equity
(Lynch & Reilly, 1985-l 986). For example, Reilly and Lynch (1990)
found that power tended to be shared equally by lesbian partners, and in
instances when power imbalances did occur, lesbian couples worked hard
to resolve the issue and reinstate equality.
Regarding issues of power in the distribution of household labor, both gay
men and lesbian women tend to share in household tasks (Kurdek, 1993).
For gay men, McWhirter and Mattison (1984) indicated that couples
tended to distribute household duties based on skill, interest, and work
schedules. Kurdek (1993) f ound that same-sex couples worked to balance
household tasks, but that each partner did not perform every task equally.
Lesbian couples, however, more often were found to attempt to share in
household tasks equally. This emphasis on sharing in all tasks may be related
to the strong emphasis on interdependence in lesbian couples. In addition,
lesbians reported slightly stronger degrees of expressiveness and equality of
power in their relationships than gay males (Kurdek, 1989). Overall, though,
household tasks were negotiated by both gay and lesbian couples on an indi-
vidual basis rather than relying on role-based assignments.

Kelationship Stages

Only one longitudinal study has explored relationship stages in same-sex


couples. Over the course of a 5year period, McWhirter and Mattison
(1982, 1984) d eve 1op e d a stage model from counseling long-term, gay
male relationships lasting between 1 and 3 7 years (average length was 8.7
years). The stage model consists of six stages: (1) Blending, (2) Nesting,
(3) Maintaining, (4) Building, (5) Releasing, and (6) Renewing. According
to McWhirter and Mattison, the stages are dynamic and couples may move
quickly through one, several, or even skip stages. Their stage model may
serve as a useful framework for understanding relationship maintenance in
same-sex couples, therefore, each stage is discussed briefly here.
In the first stage, Blending (first year), partners experience a very in-
tense sense of togetherness. Their similarities draw them together and
they tend to ignore their differences. They spend a large amount of time
together, almost to the exclusion of others. Feelings of falling in love are
very intense in this stage, but vary by individual. Equality is preferred in fi-
nancial concerns and household chores. And sexual activity is frequent,
and almost always sexually exclusive.
In the second stage, Nesting (2-3 years), gay couples turn their atten-
tion increasingly to their surroundings. The desire to establish a home to-
gether becomes a goal. Couples begin to notice each short comings
and discover ways to cope with them, or complement each other to in-
crease compatibility. This stage also involves a gradual decline in the in-
tense feelings of love felt in the Blending stage. McWhirter and Mattison
(1982, 1984) o b served that the combination of searching for compatibili-
ties, and a lessening of intense feelings of love, often create what they
termed ambivalence.
Maintaining (4-5 years) involves couples learning to manage relational di-
alectics of togetherness versus independence. After the intense togetherness
of the blending stage, partners in this stage begin to try to re-establish their
own sense of personal identity within the relationship. Risk-taking begins to
occur-sometimes through outside sexual encounters, more time apart, in-
creased self-disclosure concerning the relationship, and new separate
friends. This increased risk taking often results in high conflict that must be
negotiated and resolved. The quality that seems to maintain the relationship
in this stage is the sense of both time and emotional investment, and the feel-
ing that the relationship had taken on a life of its own. Additionally, it was
found that outside recognition of the relationship by family and friends on
average did not occur until after 3 years together.
During Building (6-l 0 years), gay couples tend to enter a stage of coop-
eration. By this time, the couple has developed a sense of security in the re-
lationship. However, this stage may also bring feelings of boredom and
entrapment. But at this point in the relationship couples have usually de-
veloped coping mechanisms for dealing with these types of relational
threats. Complementarity is successfully managed, and the individualiza-
tion of the maintaining stage is strengthened by partner support.
In stage five, Releasing (1 l-20 years), the couple has established mutual
trust and conviction to the relationship as time has strengthened positive
mutual regard for each other. By this time, partners have merged their fi-
nancial assets and possessions completely. But gay men in this stage express
less caring and concern for both self and other, which sometimes leads to
increased isolation. Also, by this stage, both partners may become guilty of
taking the relationship for granted.
The final stage, Renewing (20 years and beyond), marked a special time
for couples around or beyond their twentieth anniversary. Couples in this
stage experienced a renewal of their relationship. Partners focused on en-
joying each company more. Also, establishing future financial secu-
rity and professional achievements became a focus. These couples
assumed they would be together until death. However, partners did worry
about health, financial security, fear of loneliness, and death of partners or
themselves.
Overall, gay male couples who worked with McWhirter and Mattison
(1984) were quite satisfied and capable of maintaining long-lasting rela-
tionships. McWhirter and Mattison observed, however, that gay relation-
ships do lack role models and often have expectations based on
heterosexual couples that may cause distress when the relationship does
10. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE IN SAME-SEX COUPLES +e 219

not match those expectations. There was common curiosity and worry
concerning how other gay couples functioned in day-to-day life and deal
with finances, family, outside relationships, etcetera. Scholarly research-
ers also have become interested in exploring the everyday communication
behaviors and strategies that gay and lesbian couples use to maintain their
relationships. The area of relationship maintenance, as well as, the limited
research on maintaining same-sex relationships, is the focus of the remain-
der of the chapter.

RWATIONSHIP MAlNTlZNANCE REXARCH

As previously defined, relationship maintenance refers to employing com-


municative strategies and behaviors to prevent relationship dissolution
through efforts to sustain a dynamic equilibrium in their relation-
ship definition and satisfaction levels as they cope with the ebb and flow of
everyday relating” (Baxter & Dindia, 1990, p. 188). Interest in relationship
maintenance in romantic relationships has increased among communica-
tion researchers over the past 15 years. Romantic relationships have been
found to provide particular relational benefits such as love, affection, sex-
ual activity, emotional intimacy, advice, encouragement, as well as social
support (Coyne & Delongis, 1986; Cutrona & Suhr, 1994; Prager, 1995;
Stafford, Dainton, & Haas, 2000). In fact, Coyne and Delongis (1986)
found that other confiding relationships (e.g., parent, sibling, or friend)
could not compensate for the confiding intimacy of a romantic partner.
Social exchange theory has formed the basis of much of the research on
relationship maintenance. According to social exchange theory, a relation-
ship consists of resources that are exchanged between two persons
(Homans, 1950; Thibaut & Kelly, 1959). Preferred resources are consid-
ered to be rewards and lost resources are costs. In its basic form, the theory
proposes that as long as there are excess rewards, or profit, for both part-
ners after incurring relational costs, they will seek to maintain the relation-
ship. In applying social exchange theory, Stafford and Canary (1991;
Canary & Stafford, 1992, 1994) d eve 1oped a typology of strategic behav-
iors that both married and dating couples perceive exchanging in maintain-
ing their intimate relationships. Stafford and Canary proposed five
primary relationship maintenance strategies (defined as purposive mainte-
nance behaviors) resulting from their analysis: (a) positivity (e.g., cheerful-
ness and being positive); (b) openness (e.g., self-disclosure and
meta-relational communication); (c) assurances (e.g., expressions of love
and comfort); (d) shared tasks (e.g., household duties and relationship re-
sponsibilities); and (e) social networks (e.g., seeking mutual friendships
and kinship ties). Two additional behaviors-Advice (e.g., helping a part-
ner problem solve) and Conflict Management (e.g., using empathy and val-
idation behaviors)- recently were added by Stafford, Dainton, and Haas
(2000) in expanding Canary and original typology to include
seven behaviors.
Canary and Stafford (1992) a1so f ound that equity is an important rela-
tional characteristic underlying these behaviors. Equity is relational ex-
change where the overall ratio of each costs to rewards is roughly
equal. Equity was found to be a salient predictor of both personal use and
perception of relationship maintenance behaviors for married cou-
ples. In addition, self-reported and perceived maintenance strat-
egies combined to predict several other relational characteristics.
Positivity was the primary predictor of both control mutuality (i.e.,
amount of agreement on relationship control of partners) and liking of
partner. Perceptions of maintenance behaviors, in general, were
especially predictive of liking, whereas, sharing tasks and social networks
were correlated with commitment to the relationship.
Dainton and Stafford (1993) extended this work by examining behaviors
partners report enacting that may be routine in addition to being strategi-
cally planned. Seven primary routine behaviors (defined as intentional or un-
intentional habitualized maintenance behaviors) emerged: (a) joint activities
(e.g., spending time together and rituals); (b) small talk (e.g., discussing
daily events); (c) affection (e.g., touching, kissing, and sexual intimacy); (d)
avoidance (e.g., avoiding topics and conflict); (e) antisocial (e.g., acting jeal-
ous); (f) focus on self (e.g., watching weight and furthering career); and (g)
mediated communication (e.g., leaving notes or phone calls).
In addition to a social exchange approach, Baxter and colleagues
(Baxter 1994; Baxter & Simon, 1993) have examined relationship main-
tenance from a dialectical perspective. Relational dialectics are tensions
between polar opposites, which are central and necessary within all rela-
tionships. Three primary relational dialectics that occur internally and
externally in relationships have emerged. The internal dialectic of inte-
gration-separation (connection-autonomy) is the tension individuals
feel to establish inclusion in relationships, and at the same time, maintain
a sense of self-identity. The external dialectic of integration-separation is
the conflicting pull a couple feels to maintain outside relationships that
connect them with society, and yet, isolate themselves to increase rela-
tional intimacy. The internal dialectic of stability-change (predictabil-
ity-novelty) reflects the conflicting needs to feel secure in a predictable
relationship, but also, avoid boredom through seeking out novelty.
Externally, the dialectic of stability-change represents a struggle
to mange enactment of cultural norms, and also, maintain a unique rela-
tional identity. The dialectic of expressiveness-privacy (open-
ness-closedness) manifests itself internally as conflicting needs for open
self-disclosure of self, and yet protection of self and other from being
hurt. Externally, in the dialectic of expressiveness-privacy a couple must
10. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE IN SAME-SEX COUPLES =a 221

manage their privacy from family and members of society, and also, con-
nect with others revealing their life as a couple. Baxter and colleagues ap-
plied a dialectical perspective in analyzing how relational partners
communicatively manage these relational tensions in order to maintain
their relationship over time.

Relationship Maintenance in Same-Sex Couples

Research exploring the communication strategies and behaviors used to main-


tain gay and lesbian relationships has only recently been undertaken. Three
studies specifically in this area will be discussed here. In the first study, Haas
and Stafford (1998) replicated the open-ended questionnaire format of
Dainton and Stafford (1993) to explore the relationship maintenance behav-
iors and strategies of gay and lesbian couples. In a small sample of 15 gay men
and 15 lesbian women, Haas and Stafford found very similar maintenance be-
haviors as those in the Canary and Stafford (1992) and Dainton and Stafford
(1993) studies. Specifically, the behaviors of positivity, openness, assurances,
shared tasks, and social networks were reported as basic maintenance behav-
iors within the gay and lesbian relationships.
In addition, two unique maintenance behaviors emerged in the Haas
and Stafford (1998) study as means for same-sex couples to deal with so-
cial stigma and the lack of wide spread acceptance in society. These behav-
iors included: being “out” as a couple and seeking gay and lesbian
supportive environments. Being “out” as a couple utilizes social network
support for validation and support of the couple relationship. Seeking gay
and lesbian supportive environments was described as seeking social envi-
ronments that are accepting and supportive of gays and lesbians and their
relationships (e.g., gay and lesbian bars, gay and lesbian vacation guest
houses, gay-pride events, and the like). Because same-sex couples cur-
rently cannot obtain legal validation of their relationships (except in the
state of Vermont, but the national ramifications remain unclear at this
time), being open or “out” to the networks and seeking supportive
social environments were described as serving important relationship
maintenance functions in these relationships.
In a second follow-up study, Haas and Stafford (1997) performed a direct
matched-sample comparison between the 30 lesbians and gay men in Study
I and 30 heterosexual married individuals who were part of a larger mainte-
nance study. Participants were matched according to age and length of rela-
tionship (within 2 months). Open-ended responses describing maintenance
strategies and behaviors were compared for similarity and frequency re-
ported. Results indicated that maintenance strategies and behaviors were
very similar across gay, lesbian, and heterosexual participants. This direct
comparison design goes beyond proposing similarities, as in previous studies,
and offers more direct support for the use of basic maintenance behaviors
across relationship types.
A second goal of this follow-up study was to examine the frequency
with which maintenance behaviors were mentioned by respondents. Sev-
eral interesting similarities and differences emerged between the gay and
lesbian and heterosexual response frequency. For heterosexual partici-
pants, the five most frequently mentioned maintenance behaviors were:
(1) Shared Tasks (83%), (2) Positivity-proactive (67%) and reactive
(67%) prosocial behaviors, (3) Positivity-favors and gifts (60%) and
Comfort and Support Behaviors (60%), (4) Openness (54%), and (5) Af-
fection (50%). I n comparison, the five most frequently mentioned mainte-
nance behaviors by gays and lesbians were: (1) Shared Tasks (73%), (2)
Metarelational Communication (53%), (3) Joint Activities (50%), (4)
Positivity-reactive prosocial behaviors (47%), and (5) Assurances (43%)
and Empathetic Behaviors (43%). What is interesting is that both groups
most frequently mentioned engaging in shared tasks as a relationship main-
tenance behavior. Dainton and Stafford (1993) argued that much of main-
taining relationships likely consists of mundane, routine activities through
engaging in relational duties and tasks (e.g., making coffee, taking out the
garbage, buying groceries, etc.). The fact that both lesbians and gay men
and heterosexuals mentioned sharing tasks most frequently provides fur-
ther evidence that these behaviors may be some of the most basic commu-
nicative behaviors in maintaining couple relationships.
Beyond the similarity of using shared tasks, some important differences
emerged between same-sex and heterosexual couples in the most fre-
quently mentioned behaviors. The second most frequently mentioned be-
havior by the lesbians and gay men was metarelational communication (i.e.,
talk about the relationship); whereas a focus on positivity ranked second for
heterosexuals. This finding likely underscores a need of same-sex couples to
continue to discuss the status of their relationships (e.g., the desire of part-
ners to maintain the relationship) to compensate for the lack of legal and so-
cial validation. Married heterosexual couples often may take their
relationships for granted and rely on legal sanctions as a maintaining force.
Thus, married persons likely perceive little need to discuss their desire to be
in the relationship on regular basis. For gays and lesbians, however, there is a
continuing need to validate and confirm both relational commit-
ment through metarelational talk as an important means of relationship
maintenance. McWhirter and Mattison (1982), however, found that some
gay couples may run the risk of over metacommunicating to the detriment
of their relationships. They observed that some gay couples “at times, pro-
cess their feelings and behaviors ‘to causing relationship fatigue and
distress” (p. 88). Therefore, an over-emphasis on metarelational communi-
cation could actually undermine same-sex efforts at relationship
maintenance. Additional research is needed to explore this area further.
10. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE IN SAME-SEX COUPLES t 223

Differences also occurred in the third, fourth, and fifth most frequently
mentioned behaviors. Joint activities, positivity, and relational assurances,
respectively, accounted for the next most frequently mentioned gay and
lesbian responses; whereas positivity (favors and gifts, openness, and affec-
tion) were mentioned by heterosexuals. This may indicate that same-sex
couples spend more time engaging in the same activities together and focus
on reassuring their partner through verbal expressions of love and caring.
Heterosexual couples, on the other hand, may place more emphasis on do-
ing favors or giving gifts, engaging in self-disclosure, and showing physical
affection. Due to the small sample size of this study, however, these find-
ings require further replication.
Finally, in a third study of 20 gay male couples (N = 40) in which one or
both partners were HIV positive or had AIDS (Haas, 1999a, 1999b), a
grounded theory of communicative normalization of illness emerged to ex-
plain the process of maintaining relationships dealing with chronic illness.
More specifically, normalization of illness primarily was found to be
achieved in these gay couples through the communicative management of
two relational dialectics: managing HIV-related communication engage-
ment and avoidance, and communicating to negotiate health autonomy
versus partner involvement.
Managing HIV-related communication involves the need for couples to
negotiate and balance discussions of HIV-related health concerns in order
to keep the relationship from being overwhelmed by the illness in
day-to-day interactions. In these drive for normalization, they fre-
quently utilized a combination of balance and cyclic alternation in manag-
ing the dialectic of openness and closedness of HIV-related
communication. Cyclic alternation appeared to be used more often be-
cause it allows couples to focus completely on HIV when issues arise or at-
tempt to downplay its influence when symptoms are not present. Couples
did appear to lie on a continuum regarding the actual amount of engage-
ment or avoidance of HIV-related communication overall. Following are
some examples of participant comments reflecting the frequency of com-
munication about HIV within their relationships. Alex, an HIV-positive
participant, stated, “We talk about my health almost every day. Probably
every day. We ignore it .” Jerry, another HIV-positive partner, de-
scribed similar HIV-related communication patterns. He commented,
“Daily we talk of HIV bring stress to either one of us. I keep ask-
ing. He says, ‘No, it bother
How couples manage openness about HIV with the other end of the dia-
lectic, communication avoidance of HIV-related topics, was equally impor-
tant. Most of the participants in this study reported that there are times
when they intentionally avoid communication about HIV or AIDS.
Helgeson and Taylor (1993) argued that individuals need to be able to re-
lease themselves from the stress and worry of chronic illness. William, an
224 +s HAAS

HIV-positive partner, described times when his partner did not always want
to be informed because it is a reminder of terminal illness. For dis-
cordant couples, there was a great tension to manage the dialectic of HIV
openness and closedness because any HIV-related communication is a re-
minder of “abnormality” in their struggle to establish normalization. An-
other HIV-negative partner, Larry asserted, “Quite honestly, say we try
to not talk about it [HIV] more than we try to talk about it.” Adam, who is
HIV positive, thinks of avoiding HIV-related communication as a positive
strategy in his relationship. He stated, “a lot of times I just take a positive ap-
proach and if nothing new, you know, I even say anything. And
so, luckily there been a lot new lately.” Avoiding communication
about HIV or AIDS also could be problematic for couples. Ed, an
HIV-positive partner, explained that his avoidance of HIV com-
munication had caused arguments in their relationship: there if an
emergency. Day to day not real interested in it [HIV] . . . I keep my
meds where I would use them. I know the kind of stuff he like
to see very much. And if complaining, it really bothers him. had
arguments about it. ”
In a similar way, relational partners have to communicatively manage
the degree of HIV-negative involvement in monitoring the health
of their HIV-positive partner, so that the positive partner maintains a sense
of autonomy, and yet still feels that the HIV-negative partner cares and is
concerned for their health. Among the discordant couples in this study, the
amount of partner involvement in the healthcare of the HIV-positive part-
ner ranged on a continuum from almost total noninvolvement (e.g., know-
ing nothing about what drugs partner is prescribed or what times he
takes them) to complete involvement (e.g., monitoring complete partner
adherence with his drug regimen). However, due to the drive for normal-
ization, there was a predominant sentiment among HIV-negative partners
that HIV was the positive primary domain. For example, Alan, an
HIV-positive partner, addressed how his HIV-negative partner is not very
involved in the daily management of his health. Alan described feeling that
his health care is his own responsibility:

my ballpark. Again, since not working at this point, a big part of


those things I have to do myself for myself. So I have to manage them, and I take
care of them.. . . So as far as the pills and those regimens, he really done
very much there, except for his concern once in a while that not taking them
appropriately.

On the other hand, Phil, an HIV-negative partner, explained his partner


involvement this way: “I read everything. I think I read more than he does.
I always want to know what this is about, or what this is going to do, or the
side effects. And try to keep on him about taking his medicine regularly,
without being bitchy.”
10. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE IN SAME-SEX COUPLES ++- 225

In general, it was found that too little partner involvement indicates disin-
terest and a lack of concern, whereas over involvement can be interpreted as
being overbearing, smothering, and untrusting of the competence for
self-care. How well couples communicated in managing these extremes had
an impact on effectively maintaining their relationships.
In addition to the emergence of these dialectics, evidence was found for
use of the Canary and Stafford (1992) and Dainton and Stafford (1993)
maintenance behaviors. The specific maintenance behaviors these gay
male couples reported were equity in shared tasks, openness, positivity
and favors, assurances, affection, sharing time together, social networks,
talk of day, and use of calls and letters and e-mail. Particular importance
was mentioned in maintaining equity of shared tasks. Equity was reflected
through a tendency to capitalize on the interests of each partner. Similar to
past research, duties and tasks were performed by one partner or the other
based on personal interest and ability to perform the task, not by role as-
signments. Also, equity was stressed as an important relational feature
helping to avoid establishing a caregiver-receiver relationship that was re-
jected by all couples in this study (see Haas, 2002, for a more detailed dis-
cussion of rejecting notions of “caregiving” by gay male couples). The
reported use of these behaviors by gay male couples coping with HIV pro-
vides additional support for the generalizability of the maintenance behav-
iors in more diverse populations than previously have been studied.
Finally, concerns surrounding same-sex relationship validation and sup-
port were described in this study to be as important to the gay male couples
as HIV-related social support. In other words, social stigma related to HIV
and homosexuality were both major relational stressors in maintaining these
same-sex relationships. Importantly, these couples reported that
HIV-related social support from family, friends, and others functioned both
as illness-related support and relationship maintenance support (Haas,
2002). is in d ing, yet again, highlights the need for social validation and
support as an important factor in same-sex relationship maintenance.

CONCLUSION

Existing research indicates that gay and lesbian relationships do not differ
overall in levels of quality and satisfaction from heterosexual couples (al-
though lesbian couples have scored highest across several studies). Some
differences also are indicated in the research such as more androgynous sex
roles and egalitarian relational ideals within same-sex couples. In addition,
the lack of social and legal validation of same-sex relationships has emerged
as having a large impact on same-sex couples and may require gays and les-
bians to establish greater relational commitment and effort at maintenance
to overcome these barriers. Despite the barriers, gay men and lesbian
women have been found to establish and maintain long-term relational
commitments. Importantly, Schmitt and Kurdek (1987) found that being
in a relationship strengthened positive gay identity, increased
self-concept over those not in a relationship, and that maintaining a rela-
tionship led to a greater belief in ability to have control over life
events and lowered levels of anxiety and depression. These relational bene-
fits underscore the need for additional research in further understanding
relationship maintenance in same-sex couples.
Understanding the communicative strategies and behaviors that gays
and lesbians use to create and maintain relationships in the face of social
stressors also expands our understanding of relational communication in
general. Twenty years ago, Peplau (1982) proposed that studies of
same-sex couples:

provide an opportunity to test the generalizability of social science theories of “hu-


man behavior” which have been derived almost exclusively from heterosexual
models and tested on heterosexual samples. In this way, research on lesbian and
gay male couples contributes not only to our knowledge about homosexuality but
also to our more general knowledge about close human relationships. (p. 7)

To date, gay and lesbian relationship maintenance research has been


sparse and further studies are required. Much of the current research has
been based on small sample sizes. This is partly due to the challenge of data
collection in community-based minority, and stigmatized groups such as
gay men and lesbian women. Adding to this difficulty, same-sex couples
tend to be a hidden segment of the gay and lesbian community (Tuller,
1978). Once in a relationship, many couples tend to spend less time in gay
and lesbian public environments (e.g., singles bars). Even at bars, individu-
als often are not interested in participating in research during their enter-
tainment and leisure time. All of these factors contribute to the
complexity of studying relationship maintenance in same-sex couples. Re-
search in this area, however, does require further research; for instance (a)
replication of past research findings in larger samples must be undertaken,
(b) dyadic analysis of same-sex couples should be explored, and finally (c)
longitudinal studies of long-term relationships have yet to be attempted.
Much remains to be learned. This chapter serves as a call for relational
scholars to pursue maintenance research in this understudied population.

Attridge, M. (1994). Barriers to dissolution of romantic relationships. In D. J. Canary &


L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational maintenance (pp. 187-2 17). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Baxter, L. A. (1994). A dialogic approach to relationship maintenance. In D. J. Canary
&L. Stafford (Eds.), C ommunication and relationship maintenance (pp. 233-254).
New York: Academic.
10. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE IN SAME-SEX COUPLES i71 22,T

Baxter, L., & Dindia, K. (1990). Marital perceptions of marital maintenance


strategies. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 187-208.
Baxter, L., & Simon, E. l? (1993). Relationship maintenance strategies and dialectical
contradictions in personal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 10, 225-242.
Bell, A. P, &Weinberg, M. S. (1978). H omosexualities: A study of diversity among men
and women. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Bern, S. L. (1974). Th e measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155-l 62.
Berger, R. M. (1990). Men together: Understanding the gay couple. Journal of Homo-
sexuality, 19, 3 l-49.
Blasband, D., & Peplau, L. A. (1995). S exual exclusivity versus openness in gay male
couples. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 14, 395-412.
Blumstein, P., & Schwartz, P (1983). A merican couples. New York: William & Morrow.
Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire. New York: Basic Books.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1992). Relational maintenance strategies and equity in
marriage. Communication Monographs, 59, 243-267.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1994). Maintaining relationships through strategic and
routine interaction. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and Rela-
tional Maintenance. New York: Academic Press.
Clark, W M., & Serovich, J. M. (1997). Twenty years and still in the dark? Content
analysis of articles pertaining to gay, lesbian, and bisexual issues in marriage and fam-
ily therapy journals. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 23, 239-253.
Cole, C. L., & Spanier, G. B. (1974). C o-marital mate-sharing and family stability. Jour-
nal of Sex Research, 10, 2 l-3 1.
Coyne, J. C., & Delongis, A. M. (1986). Going beyond social support: The role of social
relationships in adaptation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,
454-460.
Cutrona, C., & Suhr, J. (1994). S ocial support communication in the context of mar-
riage: An analysis of supportive interactions. In B. R. Burleson, T. L.
Albrecht, & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Communication of social support: Messages, inter-
actions, relationships, and community (pp. 113-l 35). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Dailey, D. M. (1979). Adjustment of heterosexual and homosexual couples in pairing
relationships: An exploratory study. Journal of Sex Research, 15, 143-l 57.
Dainton, M., & Stafford, L. (1993). Routine maintenance behaviors: A comparison of
relationship type, partner similarity, and sex differences. Journal of Social and Per-
sonal Relationships, 10, 255-272.
Dindia, K., & Baxter, L. A. (1987). Strategies for maintaining and repairing marital rela-
tionships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 4, 143-158.
Duffy, S. M., & Rusbult, C. E. (1985/l 986). Satisfaction and commitment in homosex-
ual and heterosexual relationships. Journal of Homosexuality, 12, l-23.
Fisher, H. (1987). The four year itch. Natural History, 10, 22-29.
Fitzpatrick, M., Jandt, F. E., Myrick, F. L., & Edgar, T (1994). Gay and lesbian couple
relationships. In R. J. Ringer (Ed.), Q ueer words, queer images: Communication and
the construction of homosexuality (pp. 265-277). New York: NYU Press.
Gilmartin, B. D. (1972). S ome social and personal characteristics of mate-sharing
swingers. In J. Smith & L. Smith (Eds.), Co-marital sex: Recent studies of sexual al-
ternatives in marriage (pp. 21-30). Boston: Little, Brown, & Co.
Glass, S. P, & Wright, T. L. (1985). S ex d i ff erences in extramarital involvement and
marital satisfaction. Sex Roles, 12, 1101-l 119.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
228 -e- HAAS

Green, R., Bettinger, M., & Zachs, E. (1996). Ar e 1esb ian couples fused and gay male
couples disengaged? In J. Laird & R. Green (Eds.), Lesbians and gays in couples and
famiZies (pp. 185-230). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Haas, S. M. (1999a). Relationship maintenance in gay male couples coping with
HIV/AIDS. Ann Arbor, MI: UMI Dissertation Services, Bell & Howell Co.
Haas, S. M. (1999b, November). A theory of communicative normalization in the rela-
tionship maintenance of couples coping with chronic illness: The case of male couples
with HIV Paper presented at the annual conference of the National Communication
Association; Chicago.
Haas, S. M. (2002). S ocial support as relationship maintenance in gay male couples
coping with HIV Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, I9( l), 87-111.
Haas, S. M., & Stafford, L. (1998). An initial examination of maintenance behaviors in
gay and lesbian relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15,
846-855.
Haas, S. M., & Stafford, L. (1997, November). Relationship maintenance behaviors in
same-sex and heterosexual couples: An exploratory comparison. Paper was presented
at the annual conference of the National Communication Association; Chicago,
1997.
Harry, J. (1982). D ecision making and age differences among gay male couples. Journal
of Homosexuality, 8, 9-2 1.
Helgeson, V S., & Taylor, S. E. (1993). Social comparisons and adjustment among car-
diac patients. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 117 l-l 195.
Homans, G. C. (1950). The human group. New York: Harcourt, Brace, & World.
Huston, M., & Schwartz, P (1995). Th e relationships of lesbians and of gay men. In J. T.
Wood & S. Duck (Eds.), Under-studied relationships: Off the beaten track (pp.
89-l 2 1). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Jay, K., & Young, A. (1977). The gay report: Lesbians and gay men speak out about sex-
ual experiences and lifestyles. New York: Summit Books.
Jones, R. W., & Cecco, J. I? (1982). Th e f emininity and masculinity of partners in het-
erosexual and homosexual relationships. Journal of Homosexuality, 8, 37-49.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the human
maze. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual behavior
in the human female. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
Knapp, J. J. (1976). An exploratory study of seventeen sexually open marriages. Jour-
nal of Sex Research, 12, 206-2 19.
Knapp, J. J., &Whitehurst, R. N. (1977). S exually open marriage and relationships: Is-
sues and prospects. In R. W. Libby & R. N. Whitehurst (Eds.), Marriage and alter-
natives: Exploring intimate relationships (pp. 147-l 60). Glenview, IL: Scott,
Foresman.
Kurdek, L. (1987). S ex role self schema and psychological adjustment in coupled ho-
mosexual and heterosexual men and women. Sex Roles, 17, 549-562.
Kurdek, L. (1988). Relationship quality of gay and lesbian cohabitating couples. Journal
of Homosexuality, 15, 93-l 18.
Kurdek, L. (1989). Relationship quality in gay and lesbian cohabitating couples: A
1 -year follow-up study. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6, 39-59.
Kurdek, L. (1993). The allocation of household labor in gay, lesbian, and heterosexual
married couples. Journal of Social Issues, 49, 127-l 39.
10. RELATIONSHIP MAINTENANCE IN SAME-SEX COUPLES * rZ:T

Kurdek, L. A., & Schmitt, J. I? (1985-l 986). Relationship quality of gay men in closed
and open relationships. Journd of Homosexuality, 22, 85-99.
Kurdek, L. A., & Schmitt, J. P (1986a). Interaction of sex role self-concept with rela-
tionship quality and relationship beliefs in married, heterosexual cohabitating, gay,
and lesbian couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 365-370.
Kurdek, L. A., & Schmitt, J. P (1986b). Relationship quality of partners in heterosexual
married, heterosexual cohabitating, gay and lesbian couples. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 51, 7 1 l-720.
Laird, J. (1993). W silences. In E. Imber-Black (Ed.), Secrets
in families and family therupy (pp. 243-267). New York: Norton.
Lee, J. A. (1991). Can we talk? Can we really talk? Communication as a key factor in
the maturing homosexual couple. Journal of Homosexuality, I 7, 143-l 68.
Lewis, R. A., Kozac, E. B., Milardo, R. M., & Grosnick, W. A. (1981). Commitment in
same-sex love relationships. Alternative Lifestyles, 4, 22-42
Lynch, F. R. (1987). Non-ghetto gays: A sociological study of suburban homosexuals.
Journal of Homosexuality, 13, 13-42.
Lynch, J. M., & Reilly, M. E. (1985-1986). R o 1e relationships: Lesbian perspectives.
JournuZ of Homosexuality, 12, 53-69.
Marecek, J., Finn, S. E., & Cardell, M. (1983). G en d er roles in the relationships of les-
bians and gay men. Journal of Homosexuality, 8, 45-50.
McCandlish, B. M. (1982). Therapeutic issues with lesbian couples. JournaZ of Homo-
sexuality, 8, 71-78.
McWhirter, D. P, & Mattison, A. M. (1982). Psychotherapy for gay male couples. Jour-
nal of Homosexuality, 8, 79-9 1.
McWhirter, D. P, & Mattison, A. M. (1984). The male couple: How relationships de-
velop. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Mendola, M. (1980). The Mendolu report:A new look at guy couples. New York: Crown.
Ossana, S. M. (2000). Relationship and couples counseling. In R. M. Perez, K. A.,
DeBord, & K. J. Bieschke (Eds.), Handbook of counseling and psychotherapy with
lesbian, gay, and bisexual clients (pp. 275-302). Washington, DC: American Psy-
chological Association
Patterson, D. G ., & Schwartz, I? (1994). The social construction of conflict in intimate
same-sex couples. In D. D. Cahn (Ed.), Conflict in personal relationships (p. 3-26).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Paulson, C., & Paulson, R. (1972). Swing in wedlock. Society, 9, 28-37.
Peplau, L. A. (1982). Research on homosexual couples: An overview. Journal of Homo-
sexuality, 8(2), 3-8.
Peplau, L. A. (1991). L es b ian and gay relationships. In J. C. Gonsiorek &J. D. Weinrich
(Eds.), HomosexuaZity: Research implications for public policy (pp. 177-l 96).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Peplau, L. A., & Cochran, S. D. (198 1). Value orientations in the intimate relationships
of gay men. Journal of HomosexuaZity, 6(3), l-19.
Peplau, L. A., Padesky, C., & Hamilton, M. (1982). Satisfaction in lesbian relationships.
JournaZ of Homosexuality, 8, 23-3 1.
Pietropinto, A., & Simenauer, J. (1979). H us b an d s and wives: A nationwide survey of
marriage. New York: Times Books.
Prager, K. J. (1995). The psychology of intimacy. New York: Guilford Press.
Ramey, J. R. (1975). Intimate groups and networks: Frequent consequences of sexually
open marriage. The Family Coordinator, 24, 5 15-530.
Reece, R., & Segrist, A. E. (198 1). Th e association of selected “masculine” sex-role
variables with length of relationship in gay male couples. Journal of HomosexuaZity,
7, 33-47.
Reilly, M. E., & Lynch, J. M. (1990). P ower-sharing in lesbian relationships. Journal of
Homosexuazity, 19, I-30.
Rubin, A. M. (1982). S exually open versus sexually exclusive marriage: A comparison
of dyadic adjustment. Alternative Lifestyles, 5, 101-108.
Saghir, M. T., &Robins, E. (l973).MaZeandfemalehomosexuuZity:Acomprehensivein-
vestigution. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.
Schmitt, J. P, & Kurdeck, L. A. (1987). P ersonality correlates of positive identity and
relationship involvement in gay men. Journal of Homosexuality, 13, 101-l 09.
Schullo, S. A., & Alperson, B. L. (1984). Interpersonal phenomenology as a function of
sexual orientation, sex, sentiment, and trait categories in long-term dyadic relation-
ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 983-1002.
Shackelford, T. K., & Buss, D. M. (1997). C ues to infidelity. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, 23, 1034-1045.
Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1991). Maintenance strategies and romantic relationship
type, gender, and relational characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relution-
ships, 8, 217-242.
Stafford, L., Dainton, M., & Haas, S. M. (2000). Measuring routine and strategic rela-
tional maintenance: Scale revision, sex versus gender roles, and the prediction of re-
lational characteristics. Communication Monographs, 67(3), 306-323.
Thibaut, J. W., &Kelly, H. H. (1959). e social psychology of groups. New York: Wiley.
Thompson, A. I? (1984). Emotional and sexual components of extramarital relations.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 46, 35-42.
Tuller, N. R. (1978). Couples: The hidden segment of the gay world. JournaZ of Homo-
sexuality, 3(4), 331-343.
Wagner, G. J., Remien, R. H., & Carballo-Dieguez, A. (1998). “Extramarital” sex: Is
there an increased risk for HIV transmission? A study of male couples of mixed HIV
status. AlDS Education and Prevention, 1 O(3), 245-256.
Watson, M. A. (1981). S exually open marriage: Three perspectives. Alternative Life-
styles, 4, 3-21.
Weinberg, M., & Williams, C. (1974). M a 1e h omosexuuls. New York: Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
Stanley 0. Gaines, Jr.
Brunei University

Christopher R. Agnew
Purdue University

ost newlywed couples believe that they will remain married for
the rest of their lives. Such optimism contrasts sharply with current di-
vorce rates (e.g., approximately one-half of all marriages in the United
States end in divorce; Waite, Bachrach, Hindin, Thomson, &Thornton,
2000). The discrepancy between expectation of marital in-
stability and the reality of marital instability is especially pronounced
among interethnic couples, who generally are more likely to divorce
than are intraethnic couples (e.g., approximately two thirds of interra-
cial marriages in the United States end in divorce; Gaines & Ickes,
1997; Waite et al., 2000).
What processes might contribute to the relatively high divorce rate
among intercultural couples (who, collectively, represent a subset of
interethnic couples; Gaines & Ickes, 1997), as compared with intracultural
232 -c=+ GAINES AND AGNEW

couples? Previous research has revealed similarities between interdepen-


dence processes (Kelley, 1979; Kelley & Thibaut, 1978; Thibaut & Kelley,
1959) in primarily intraethnic romantic relationships (Gaines, Reis, et al.,
1997) and in interethnic romantic relationships (Gaines, Granrose, et al.,
1999) as influenced by individual factors. However, previous research has
not addressed differences between interdependence processes in
intracultural relationships and in intercultural relationships, especially as
influenced by relational factors.
In the present chapter, a set of conceptual guideposts is proposed for
predicting relationship maintenance and stability in intercultural relation-
ships. In particular, we consider the likely impact of various relational fac-
tors (i.e., satisfaction level, quality of relationship alternatives, investment
size, and perceived prescriptive support) from (e.g., Cox,
Wexler, Rusbult, & Gaines, 1997; Rusbult, 1980; Rusbult, Drigotas, &
Verette, 1994; Rusbult, Martz, &Agnew, 1998) investment model on indi-
relational maintenance behaviors (i.e., decision to remain, re-
sponses to accommodative dilemmas, derogation of alternatives,
willingness to sacrifice, perceived superiority) in intercultural marriages,
as mediated by commitment level. As a prelude to our conceptual journey,
we briefly review the literature on intercultural relationships.

INT~KCULTUKAL RIZLATIONSHIPS:
Dll=i=lCULT TO ESTABLISH, DIl=J=lCULT TO MAINTAIN

An interethnic marriage (sometimes termed intermarriage; Gordon,


1964) is any marriage in which the husband and wife belong to two respec-
tive groups, apart from gender, that the society in which they live regards as
different. One type of interethnic marriage is an interracial marriage,
whereby the husband and wife belong to two respective groups that the so-
ciety in which they live regards as biologically different. Another type of
interethnic marriage is an intercultural marriage, whereby the husband and
wife belong to two respective groups that the society in which they live re-
gards as linguistically, religiously, or historically different. As Baptiste
(1984) pointed out, ‘(Although some interracial marriages are also
intercultural, all intercultural marriages are not interracial. [Nevertheless],
most interracial marriages are, to some degree, also intercultural, e.g., an
Asian-American married to [a European-] American” (p. 3 74).
Intercultural married couples (especially those who are also interracial),
along with any resulting offspring, tend to be ostracized by a variety of rela-
tionship outsiders (e.g., strangers, friends, relatives; Baptiste, 1984, 1990).
Given that intercultural couples often face intense scrutiny and disap-
proval from relationship outsiders, it is not surprising that intercultural
marriages are at greater risk for divorce than are intracultural marriages.
11. INTERCULTURAL COUPLES e 233

Some professional and lay observers have interpreted the relatively high
divorce rate among intercultural couples as evidence of underlying individ-
ual and relational pathology. However, neither the establishment nor the
dissolution of intercultural marriages can be regarded as reliable indicators
of individual or relational pathology (Crohn, 1995; Ho, 1990).
Although intercultural marriages can be difficult to maintain, there can
also be difficult to establish in the first place. One of the best predictors of
mutual liking and relationship formation is proximity or propinquity
(Berscheid, 1985), defined as geographic closeness. Within the United
States, members of a particular cultural group generally have few (if any)
opportunities to meet individuals from other cultural groups (Gaines,
Chalfin, Kim, & Taing, 1998), largely because culturally different groups
tend to live in physically separated communities.
Of course, many women and men successfully establish and maintain
stable intercultural marriages. However, successful intercultural marriages
rarely receive the same attention, whether from the mass media or from
the social science literature, that successful intracultural marriages receive
(Gaines & Liu, 2000). Aside from ethnographies (e.g., Gordon, 1964;
Porterfield, 1978; Rosenblatt, Karis, & Powell, 1995) and case studies
(e.g., Baptiste, 1984; Crohn, 1995; Ho, 1990), few empirical studies have
yielded substantive insight into the relationship maintenance processes
that promote marital stability among intercultural couples. Moreover,
much of the research on intercultural marriages has been atheoretical, thus
limiting opportunities for a priori hypothesis testing (Gaines & Liu, 2000).

INTlZKDIEPIENDlENClE TI-EORY AND PREDICTORS


Ol= COMMITMENT AMONG INTlXCULTUKAL COUPLES

Interdependence theory (Kelley & Thibaut, 1978; Rusbult, Arriaga, &


Agnew, 200 1; Thibaut & Kelley, 195 9) p rovides a broad conceptual frame-
work for understanding important relationship processes. One strength of
the theory is its flexibility, allowing it to be used to understand all types of
pairings, including intracultural and intercultural relationships. At its root
is the paramount role of interaction and recognition that an
outcomes can be influenced or determined by sources beyond the individ-
ual, including a relationship partner.
One particularly important psychological construct that influences so-
cial interaction is commitment, often defined as a long-term orientation t?-
ward a relationship and partner, including feelings of psychological
attachment and intentions to persist through both good and bad times
(Cox et al., 1997; see also Agnew, Van Lange, Rusbult, & Langston, 1998;
Arriaga & Agnew, 2001; Rusbult & Buunk, 1993). In general, commitment
is a positive predictor of relationship-promoting responses, and a negative
234 -e- GAINES AND AGNEW

predictor of relationship-threatening responses, following anger


or criticism (Rusbult, Verette, Whitney, Slovik, & Lipkus, 1991). Com-
mitment plays a pivotal role in investment model (1980; Rusbult
et al., 1994; Rusbult et al., 1998), which was derived from interdepen-
dence theory (Kelley & Thibaut, 1978; Thibaut & Kelley, 1959). Accord-
ing to the investment model, select social-psychological variables influence
commitment toward their romantic partners and commit-
ment mediates the impact of those social-psychological variables on indi-
decisions to remain in their current romantic relationships and to
engage in accommodation toward their relationship partners. A recent
meta-analysis of published and unpublished studies that test aspects of the
investment model provides strong overall support for the model (Le &
Agnew, in press). In this section, a recent formulation of invest-
ment model (Cox et al., 1997) is applied to the study of relationship main-
tenance in intercultural relationships. Specifically considered is the
influence of four social-psychological variables on relationship commit-
ment: (1) satisfaction, (2) alternatives, (3) investment, and (4) support.

Impact of Satisfaction Level on Commitment

To the extent that an individual receives positive (vs. negative) outcomes


from an interaction with a partner, the individual will be satisfied with the
relationship with that partner. Interdependence theory holds that a per-
outcomes are a function of the rewards and the costs that result from
To determine if outcomes are satisfactory, one compares
the outcomes that have been obtained with those obtained in past interac-
tions in a similar domain. For example, if a person has generally experi-
enced a number of rewards in past romantic relationships (e.g., thoughtful,
supportive partners) then new partners will be judged against this existing
high standard. In interdependence terminology, this is referred to as com-
parison Ze!eveZ
(or CL): people compare their currently obtained outcomes
to their CL to determine whether or not they are satisfied with the level of
outcomes that they are currently receiving.

‘The concepts of rewards and costs are central to reinforcement-based theories in general
(Berscheid, 1985). Another reinforcement-based theory that is relevant to the study of
intercultural relationship processes is resource exchange theory (Foa & Foa, 1974), which postu-
lates that much of human social behavior involves the give-and-take of commodities or resources.
From the perspective of resource exchange theory, individuals are likely to regard interactions
with relationship partners as rewarding to the extent that individuals receive affection (i.e., love,
or emotional acceptance of another person) and respect (i.e., status, or social acceptance of an-
other person) from their partners. Later in this chapter, discussed are the results of a study by
Gaines, Rios, et al. (1999), w h o examined patterns of interpersonal resource exchange among
interethnic and interracial couples.
11. INTERCULTURAL COUPLES ++i- 233

If the outcomes that one is currently receiving in a relationship are seen


as exceeding CL, then the person will be relatively satisfied with the
current relationship. If, on the other hand, the outcomes are seen as being
below CL, then the person will be dissatisfied with the relationship.
Thus, interdependence theory measures satisfaction level as the difference
between currently obtained outcomes and preexisting CL. Drawing
on these interdependence theoretic notions, (1980) investment
model holds that, to the extent that a person is satisfied with a given rela-
tionship (i.e., to the extent that obtained outcomes exceed CL), the
person will be committed to that relationship (and partner). This is not to
suggest that satisfaction alone leads to commitment. Rather, satisfaction is
one positive predictor of commitment.
We do not know of any direct evidence that individuals who marry
interculturally are less (or more) satisfied with their relationships than are
individuals who marry intraculturally. The only indirect evidence of which
we are aware is that intercultural marriages generally are less stable than
are intracultural marriages (Gaines & Liu, 2000). However, a lower level of
relationship satisfaction is but one possible explanation for the high rates of
intercultural divorce.

Impact of Quality of Akernatives on Commitment

Interdependence theory also holds that the presence of possible alterna-


tives to current relationship influences commitment to the cur-
rent relationship. Specifically, the theory holds that one considers the
outcomes that one believes might be obtained from the next best alterna-
tive to current relationship, referred to as the comparison level for al-
ternatives (or CL-alt). Alternatives may include specific other people as
well as spending time with friends or spending time alone. Having compel-
ling alternatives serves to decrease the amount of dependence on the
current relationship. Thus, interdependence theory measures relationship
dependence as the difference between currently obtained outcomes and
CL-alt. Drawing on these interdependence concepts,
(1980) investment model holds that, to the extent that a person does not
perceive desirable or compelling alternatives to a current relationship (i.e.,
to the extent that obtained outcomes exceed CL-alt), the person will
be committed to their existing relationship.
Individuals who date and marry interculturally tend to perceive them-
selves as having selected from a wider array of potential partners than do
individuals who date and marry intraculturally. Specifically, women and
men who date and marry interculturally generally view their pools of po-
tential partners as including persons of the opposite sex outside as well as
within their cultural ingroups (Crohn, 1995; Gurung & Duong, 1999; Ho,
236 -es GAINES AND AGNEW

1990; Shibazaki & Brennan, 1998; Tucker & Mitchell-Kernan, 1995). In


contrast, women and men who date and marry intraculturally generally
view their pools of potential partners as limited to persons of the opposite
sex within their cultural ingroups.
To the extent that individuals in intercultural marriages perceive them-
selves as having a wider variety of potential alternative partners than do in-
dividuals in intracultural marriages, we would expect the number of
attractive relationship alternatives to be greater-and, hence, to be more
likely to undermine commitment-within intercultural marriages than
within intracultural marriages. However, we are unaware of any direct evi-
dence that bears on this prediction. Even if results of empirical studies ulti-
mately lend support to our prediction concerning greater
perceived quality of alternative partners in intercultural as opposed to
intracultural relationships, such results would not stand as proof that indi-
viduals in intercultural relationships are likely to make fewer efforts to
maintain their relationships than are individuals in intracultural relation-
ships. The sections following on investments and prescriptive support as
predictors of commitment further discuss the issue of intercultural part-
efforts in maintaining their relationships in the face of societal pres-
sures favoring relationship dissolution.
Note that, in theory, one can be in an unsatisfying relationship yet re-
main committed to the relationship, due to a lack of perceived alternatives
to the unsatisfying relationship. For example, Rusbult and Martz (1995)
found that physically abused women were more likely to stay with their
husbands when they perceived few or no alternatives to their situation
than when they did perceive possible alternatives. Similarly, Johnson
(1995) found that unhappy women often remained in relationships, not
because of personal commitment, but rather because of structural com-
mitment (e.g., lack of finances, presence of children in the home). An im-
plicit assumption guiding our prediction concerning alternatives and
commitment in intercultural relationships is that the presence of alterna-
tives will serve to undermine commitment in the absence of actual, feared,
or threatened abuse by one partner toward the other partner. In an abusive
relationship (whether intercultural or intracultural), the link between al-
ternatives and commitment is likely to be diminished.

Impact of Investment Size on Commitment

Although satisfaction with and alternatives to a current relationship are


seen as important determinants of commitment to a relationship,
Rusbult (1980) add e d an additional predictive element, which led to the
naming of her investment model. Specifically, Rusbult held that the invest-
ments that people make in their relationships contribute to feelings of
11. INTERCULTURAL COUPLES 3L 237

commitment. Investments can be intrinsic to the relationship itself (e.g.,


amount of time spent with the partner, shared feelings) or extrinsic to it
(e.g., joint possessions, home ownership). Essentially, the more invest-
ments that a person has made in a given relationship, the more that person
will be committed to that relationship due to the sunken costs that have
been incurred over time. By ending the relationship, these investments
would be lost or mired (e.g., house will be sold, shared memories will be-
come tarnished).
Individuals who are married interculturally tend to have fewer invest-
ments in their relationships than do individuals who are married
intraculturally. In particular, women and men who are married
interculturally tend to have fewer children together than do women and
men who are married intraculturally (Gaines & Ickes, 1997). Also, individ-
uals who are married interculturally generally have spent less time in their
courtship phase of their relationships than have individuals who are mar-
ried intraculturally, partly because they are more likely to be in their sec-
ond marriages than are individuals who are married intraculturally (Gaines
& Liu, 2000).
To the extent that individuals in intercultural marriages tend to be less
invested in their relationships than are individuals in intracultural mar-
riages, we would expect investments (or, rather, the lack of investments)
to be more likely to undermine commitment in intercultural
marriages than in intracultural marriages. However, we have not seen any
direct evidence that addresses this prediction. At first glance, one might ar-
gue that our prediction is counterintuitive, given the relatively high level of
energy that individuals in intercultural relationships often must spend in
order to maintain their relationships. In fact, if one views effort as part and
parcel of overall investments (e.g., Wieselquist, Rusbult, Fos-
ter, & Agnew, 1999), then it seems reasonable to expect that intercultural
effort in itself will help compensate for lack of investments in
other respects, such as intercultural tendency to produce fewer
offspring than do intracultural couples. Even if empirical results eventually
lend support to our prediction concerning the relatively fewer overall in-
vestments of individuals in intercultural versus intracultural relationships,
it does not necessarily follow that intercultural partners will make fewer
efforts to maintain their relationships than will intracultural partners.

Impact of Prescriptive Support on Commitment

Dyadic relationships do not exist in isolation from the social world or apart
from a existing social network. Although one may fall deeply in
love with someone, it does not necessarily follow that family and
friends similarly will experience loving-or even liking-toward that per-
“58 -t+= GAINES AND AGNEW

son. It is reasonable to assume that the perceptions of others can have an


impact on own feelings of commitment toward a partner (Agnew,
Loving & Drigotas, 2001).
The extent to which one feels supported in a romantic relationship by
their social network, along with the influence of perceived support on com-
mitment, has become the focus of investigation in recent years. Cox,
Wexler, Rusbult, and Gaines (1997) made the distinction between two
types of support for a relationship: personal prescriptive support, based on a
relationship belief that he or she ought to persist in a relation-
ship; and social prescriptive support, based on a relationship
belief that friends and family members favor relationship continuation. Cox
and her colleagues found that both personal and social aspects of prescrip-
tive support were significant and positive predictors of commit-
ment to their romantic relationships. However, only social prescription was
found to account for unique variance in commitment beyond that ac-
counted for by satisfaction, alternatives, and investments; personal prescrip-
tion did not account for unique variance in commitment.
A recent study by Etcheverry and Agnew (200 1) lends further credence
to the idea that perceptions of social network support contribute to feel-
ings of commitment toward relationship partner. These researchers
assessed the degree to which subjective norms influence relationship com-
mitment. Adapted from the attitudes literature (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980),
subjective norms represent the influence of social referents on a
intentions to engage in a behavior. Subjective norms are operationalized as
perceived normative beliefs of specific social referents weighted by a per-
motivation to comply with each referent. As commitment has been
defined as including the intention to persist (Arriaga & Agnew, 2001),
Etcheverry and Agnew (2001) examined the influence of the investment
model variables along with subjective norms in predicting commitment to
a dating partner. Simultaneous multiple regression analyses indicated that
subjective norms, satisfaction, alternatives, and investments all signifi-
cantly predict commitment. Consistent with the study by Cox and col-
leagues (Cox et al., 1997), subjective norms were found to significantly
increase the amount of variance in commitment accounted for by the three
investment model variables.
The study of relationship maintenance in intercultural marriages might
yield different results for personal versus social components of prescrip-
tive support. On the one hand, we are unaware of any direct evidence that
personal prescriptive support varies as a function of the intercultural ver-
sus intracultural nature of marriage. On the other hand, the distinct lack of
support from interculturally married respective families of ori-
gin suggests that social prescriptive support tends to be lower for
interculturally married individuals than for intraculturally married individ-
uals (see Porterfield, 1978; Rosenblatt et al., 1995).
11. INTERCULTURAL COUPLES =e 239

To the extent that social prescriptive support is lower for individuals in


intercultural marriages than for individuals in intracultural marriages, we
would expect the lack of such support to be more likely to undermine indi-
commitment in intercultural marriages than in intracultural mar-
riages. In contrast, we would not expect such a prediction to hold true for
personal prescriptive support. On the contrary, intercultural
daily efforts at maintaining their relationships in spite of enormous opposi-
tion from strangers and acquaintances (and, perhaps, from friends and
family; Porterfield, 1978; Rosenblatt et al., 1995) may make personal pre-
scriptive support a stronger positive predictor of commitment among
intercultural partners than among intracultural partners. Nevertheless, to
our knowledge, no study has yet examined personal, social, or generic pre-
scriptive support as a predictor of commitment in intercultural versus
intracultural marriages.

COMMITMENT AS A MEDIATOR
OI= RlZLATIONSHIP MAINTlENANCE BEHAVIORS

The investment model not only details a number of theoretically impor-


tant antecedents of relationship commitment but also describes how com-
mitment leads to a number of important relationship maintenance
behaviors (for reviews, see Rusbult & Buunk, 1993; Rusbult et al., 1994).
Indeed, as can be seen in Fig. 11.1, commitment mediates the influence of
satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, investment size, and prescriptive
support on relationship maintenance behaviors. Note that according to
Rusbult and her colleagues, commitment is distinct from relationship sta-
bility. However, in Thibaut and Kelley ‘s (1959) original formulation of in-
terdependence theory, commitment and stability essentially are treated as
one and the same (i.e., CL-alt is associated with stability). Similarly, in
Lewis and (1979) f amily systems approach, commitment and
stability are treated as interchangeable constructs. The distinction be-
tween commitment and stability is influenced heavily by per-
spective, which-like any other reinforcement-based perspective-is not
immune to critique (see Duck, 1994).
One additional point to keep in mind concerning our conceptualization
of commitment is that, even if one accepts our premise that commitment
is conceptually and empirically distinct from relationship stability, one
might disagree with this conceptualization of commitment. Johnson
(1991a, 199Ib) h as argued that commitment ideally should be conceptu-
alized and measured as a multidimensional construct. Although debates
over the conceptualization of commitment as unidimensional versus mul-
tidimensional are beyond the scope of the present chapter (for a review,
see Arriaga & Agnew, 2001; Surra, Hughes, & Jacquet, 1999),
24-O -c=+ GAINES AND AGNEW

i Satisfaction
Level

+
,::__\
‘;11/“1 Accorkodate 1

Derogation
of
Alternatives

Willingness
To
Sacrifice

r Perceived
Superiority

FIG. 11.1. Theorized antecedents and consequences of relationship


commitment.

likelihood of persistence in intercultural relationships is facilitated by high


satisfaction, low alternatives, high investments, and high social (if not per-
sonal) prescriptive support, regardless of whether one regards commit-
ment as unidimensional or multidimensional (see Cox et al., 1997; Rusbult
& Buunk, 1993).

Impact 06 Commitment on Decision to Remain

Theoretically, commitment is positioned as a mediator of the impact of all


distal investment model variables on decision to remain in,
rather than leave, their intercultural relationships (Drigotas & Rusbult,
1992; see Fig. 11.1). Perhaps the most important conceptual link of all
(i.e., the presumed link between commitment and the decision to remain
rather than leave a relationship) might be undermined by the lack of direct
evidence of which we are aware that individuals who marry interculturally
are less committed than are individuals who marry intraculturally, despite
11. INTERCULTURAL COUPLES ++ 241

the differing divorce rates. However, just as it would be inappropriate to


cite high intermarriage divorce rates as evidence of lack of sat-
isfaction with their relationships, so too would it be inappropriate to cite
these high rates as evidence of lack of commitment toward
their relationships.
Even among relationships that presumably are intraracial in composi-
tion (i.e., relationships among members of fraternities and sororities at a
large Southern university; Drigotas & Rusbult, 1992), the role of commit-
ment as a mediator is not entirely consistent across studies. In the first of
two studies, Drigotas and Rusbult found that commitment completely
mediated the impact of the related construct of dependence on individu-
decision to remain. However, in a follow-up study, Drigotas and
Rusbult found that commitment was only a partial mediator of the impact
of dependence on decision to remain. In fairness to Drigotas
and Rusbult, it is possible that multicollinearity between dependence and
commitment made commitment appears to be less stable as a mediator
across the two studies than actually was the case (see Pedhazur, 1982;
Stevens, 1992). However, the mediational status of commitment is cer-
tainly open to debate.

ImpactoFCommitmenton Responses
toAccommodativeDiIemma.s

Just as commitment may tend to mediate the impact of satisfaction, alter-


natives, investments, and support on stay-leave decisions, so too might
commitment mediate the impact of investment model variables on other
social-psychological outcomes, even in relationships in which partners
have agreed to stay. One of the most theoretically compelling outcomes,
derived from interdependence theory, is accommodation, defined as indi-
refraining from responding to anger or criticism by ex-
pressing anger or criticism toward their partners (thus threatening
long-term relationship stability), and instead behaving in ways that defuse
conflict (thus promoting relationship stability; Rusbult et al., 1991). Ac-
commodation is operationalized in terms of four key dimensions: (1) exit
(i.e., an active, relationship-threatening response to the anger or
criticism, such as declaring that one never wants to see the partner again);
(2) voice (i.e., an active, relationship-promoting response to the
anger or criticism, such as asking the partner to sit down and talk about
whatever is bothering him or her); (3) ZoyaZty (i.e., a passive, relation-
ship-promoting response to anger or criticism, such as remaining
completely quiet); and (4) neglect (i.e., a passive, relationship-threatening
response to partners, anger or criticism, such as sulking; Rusbult,
Zembrodt, & Gunn, 1982). According to Rusbult and her colleagues, high
242 ++= GAINES AND AGNEW

levels of voice and loyalty responses, coupled with low levels of exit and
neglect responses, together constitute accommodation. Rusbult and
Arriaga (1997) argued that exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect may be viewed
more precisely as responses to accommodative dilemmas, or situations in
which an individual is faced with the daunting task of putting the welfare
of the relationship ahead of own welfare precisely when a
behavior (i.e., directing seemingly unprovoked verbal assaults toward the
individual) is at its most self-centered.
In two studies, Wieselquist and colleagues (Wieselquist, Rusbult, Fos-
ter, &Agnew, 1999) f ound that, consistent with predictions, commitment
was a positive predictor of accommodation and related prorelationship
mechanisms (e.g., willingness to sacrifice; see also Van Lange et al., 1997).
However, the studies by Wieselquist and colleagues (1999) were based on
samples of individuals who, for the most part, were in intracultural rela-
tionships. Whether any research in which commitment has been examined
as a predictor of accommodation (or, for that matter, other
pro-relationship behaviors) specifically in intercultural relationships is un-
known. Thus, the prediction that commitment will mediate the impact of
investment model variables on accommodation in intercultural relation-
ships has yet to receive empirical confirmation.
It has already been noted that, even in primarily intracultural relation-
ships, the consistency of commitment as a complete mediator of the impact
of dependence on decision to remain is uncertain (Drigotas &
Rusbult, 1992). A similar finding arose in a set of studies by Rusbult et al.
(199 1) concerning commitment as a mediator of the impact of satisfaction,
alternatives, and investments on responses to accommodative dilemmas. As
mentioned earlier with regard to commitment and dependence, it is possi-
ble that multicollinearity among commitment, satisfaction, alternatives, and
investments obscured the true value of commitment as a direct mediator
(see Pedhazur, 1982; Stevens, 1992). The problem of multicollinearity may
be due in part to similarity in item content among (1980) early
measures of satisfaction, alternatives, investments, and commitment (Surra
& Hughes, 1997). Fortunately, recent efforts to generate investment model
measures with demonstrated discriminant validity have proven successful
(Rusbult, Martz, & Agnew, 1998).

Impact of Commitment on Derogation of Akernatives

In recent years, Rusbult and her colleagues (Johnson & Rusbult, 1989) have
distinguished between perceptions of alternatives (discussed earlier in this
chapter) and derogation of alternutives, the latter of which refers to individu-
active efforts at devaluing, as opposed to simply recognizing, potential al-
ternative. Within conceptual framework, perception of alternatives
is positioned as an antecedent of commitment, whereas derogation of alterna-
11. INTERCULTURAL COUPLES -+ 243

tives is positioned as a consequence of commitment (for reviews, see Rusbult


& Buunk, 1993; Rusbult et al., 1994). Following Rusbult and her colleagues,
within intercultural relationships, commitment is likely to be a
positive predictor of derogation of alternatives. However, given
that individuals in intercultural relationships often are criticized by members
of their own cultural groups specifically for betraying opposite-sex cultural
ingroup members as attractive alternatives, individuals in intercultural rela-
tionships may find it difficult to derogate alternatives within their own cul-
tural groups without experiencing a great deal of cognitive dissonance
(Festinger, 195 7). F or example, an African-American male who prides him-
self on his “color-blindness” in dating a Euro-American woman may realize the
hypocrisy in such a self-congratulatory belief if and when he invokes racist ste-
reotypes of African-American women (e.g., as domineering and unfeminine)
in an attempt to avoid considering any African-American women as alterna-
tives, no matter how attractive many African-American women may be to
him (Porterfield, 1978; Rosenblatt et al., 1995). In any event, we do not know
of previous research that has tested our prediction concerning commitment
and derogation of alternatives among individuals in intercultural relationships.
One reassuring result across three studies by Johnson and Rusbult (1989)
is that, among individuals who presumably were in primarily intracultural
relationships, commitment (which is a direct predictor of derogation of al-
ternatives) consistently was a stronger predictor of derogation of alterna-
tives than was satisfaction (which we have cast as an indirect predictor of
derogation of alternatives). A cursory reading of Johnson and Rusbult might
lead one to conclude that this consistent result was due to the type of analy-
sis that they used (i.e., simultaneous regression analyses; cf. Cramer, 1972).
Unfortunately, the type of analysis they used does not control statistically
for multicollinearity (e.g., simultaneous regression analyses are often used in
an effort to interpret the results of polynomial regression analyses; see
Pedhazur, 1982). Moreover, the same type of analysis was used by Rusbult
and her colleagues (e.g., Drigotas & Rusbult, 1992; Rusbult et al., 199 1) in
studies that were cited earlier, in which commitment was found to be a par-
tial mediator. Johnson and Rusbult (1989) stopped short of concluding that
commitment mediated the impact of satisfaction on derogation of alterna-
tives. Although we do not doubt the utility of commitment as a mediator of
the impact of investment model variables on derogation of alternatives, such
a mediating effect has not been demonstrated unequivocally.

Imf3act of Commitment on Willingness to Sacrifice

Still another consequence of commitment within conceptual


framework is willingness to sacrifice, or choice of the long-term
welfare of their relationships over otherwise desirable outcomes that would
have been in their own, but not their self-interest (Van Lange et al.,
244 -I+= GAINES AND AGNEW

1997). Initially, the concept of willingness to sacrifice might sound similar to


the aforementioned concept of accommodation. However, as Rusbult and
Buunk (1993) ob served, accommodation involves overt accep-
tance of some personal cost (e.g., swallowing pride, in order to avoid
escalating interpersonal conflict that already has been instigated by
partner) over the short term. In contrast, willingness to sacrifice involves in-
overt rejection of some personal benefit (e.g., turning down a job
offer that would have earned a substantial raise in pay but that would have
required moving away from spouse, in order to start planning for a
family). According to Rusbult and colleagues (Van Lange et al., 1997), com-
mitment will be a positive predictor of willingness to sacrifice;
we would make a similar prediction within the particular context of
intercultural relationships. However, we do not know of any studies of
intercultural relationships that have tested such a prediction.
In a meta-analysis of results from six studies, Van Lange and colleagues
(1997) concluded that commitment wholly mediated the impact of in-
vestment model variables on willingness to sacrifice. Such en-
couraging results are tempered by the fact that, as was true of the other
studies by Rusbult and colleagues that were cited earlier concerning com-
mitment and its consequences, Van Lange and colleagues (1997) relied on
simultaneous regressions, which do not control for multicollinearity
among predictors. We do not doubt the utility of commitment as a media-
tor of the impact of investment model variables on willingness to sacrifice,
nor do we have any reason to expect individuals in intercultural relation-
ships to be less willing to sacrifice than are their counterparts in
intracultural relationships. However, we simply point to the need for re-
search on intercultural relationships that more directly tests this
mediational hypothesis.

lmpactokommitmenton Perceivedsuperiority

model of antecedents and consequences of commitment posits


that commitment also mediates the impact of investment model variables
on perceived relationship superiority, or view of
their relationship as having a larger number of positive qualities and a
smaller number of negative qualities than do relationships (Van
Lange & Rusbult, 1995). A ccording to model, commitment is a
positive predictor of perceived relationship superiority.
We concur with Rusbult and would make similar predictions concerning
individuals in intercultural relationships. Unlike the other explored conse-
quences of commitment, Rusbult and her colleagues have not published
any research that directly tests the prediction set forth in the preceding
paragraph. For instance, Van Lange and Rusbult (1995) did not refer to
commitment or any of the investment model variables as predictors of in-
11. INTERCULTURAL COUPLES =++ 245

perceived relationship superiority. Therefore, no research is


known-whether on individuals in intercultural or intracultural relation-
ships-that would support the prediction concerning investment model
variables, commitment, and perceived relationship superior-
ity. This is not to cast doubt upon the conceptual viability of this hypothe-
sis. Rather, among all of the predictions included in framework
(Fig. 11. l), this one has received the least empirical examination.

PIXRSONALITY VARIABLE5 A5 DIRlECT PRIZDICTORS


Ol= ACCOMMODAT-ION

As Fig. 11.1 indicates, conceptualization of the antecedents and


consequences of commitment has tended to favor social-psychological,
rather than personality, predictors. However, Rusbult and her colleagues
acknowledged that certain stable individual-difference variables (which
Rusbult termed “dispositions”; e.g., Rusbult & Buunk, 1993, p. 183) may
influence accommodation in personal relationships. Further-
more, much of own research (e.g., Gaines, Reis, et al., 1997;
Rusbult et al., 1987; Rusbult et al., 1991; Rusbult, Zembrodt, & Iwaniszek,
1986) supports the view that personality factors predict significant vari-
ance in accommodation.
Perhaps the most influential personality theory within the field of per-
sonal relationships in general, and within the study of accommodation in
particular, has been attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1980). Ac-
cording to attachment theory, humans and other primates are motivated to
form socioemotional bonds with caregivers from infancy onward; such
psychological bonds promote the propagation of the species. However, not
all individuals are equally successful at meeting their socioemotional
needs, such as love and esteem. Although a plurality (and, perhaps, a ma-
jority) of individuals are securely attached (i.e., feel comfortable establish-
ing relationships and placing trust in relationship partners), a substantial
minority of individuals are insecurely attached (i.e., feel uncomfortable es-
tablishing relationship and placing trust in relationship partners, either be-
cause they view potential partners as unlikely to meet their socioemotional
needs, or because they view themselves as unable to meet their
socioemotional needs; Gaines, Reis, et al., 1997).
Some relationship scholars have questioned whether attachment style
(Bartholomew & Horowitz, 199 1; Hazan & Shaver, 198 7) actually is a person-
ality construct (for a review, see Feeney & Noller, 1996). Indeed, recent re-
sults by Gaines et al. (2000) revealed that the impact of attachment style on
responses to accommodative dilemmas among young adults cannot be gener-
alized from romantic relationships to friendships or parent-offspring relation-
ships. Nevertheless, at least within the domain of adult romantic
246 -is+ GAINES AND AGNEW

relationships, attachment style clearly functions as a stable, individ-


ual-difference influence on accommodative responses. Especially
relevant to the present chapter is the fact that, in a study of interethnic (and
primarily interracial) romantic relationships, Gaines, Granrose, et al. (1999)
found that securely attached individuals were more likely to engage in accom-
modation, in particular by refraining from reciprocating anger or
criticism (i.e., refraining from engaging in exit and neglect responses) than
were insecurely attached individuals. Moreover, the study by Gaines,
Granrose, et al. (1999) replicated and extended results from previous studies
of primarily intraethnic, intraracial romantic relationships by Scharfe and
Bartholomew (1995) and by Gaines, Reis, et al. (1997).
Attachment style is the only personality variable that has been examined as
a predictor of responses to accommodative dilemmas specifically in
interethnic (and, presumably, intercultural) relationships. Some theorists
might argue that attachment style is an all-encompassing personality con-
struct, thus making it unnecessary for researchers to consider additional per-
sonality variables (for a review, see Feeney & Noller, 1996). However, at the
present stage of development within the field of personal relationships, no sin-
gle personality theory-including attachment theory-has achieved the sta-
tus of a “master theory” (Berscheid & Reis, 1998). In fact, Rothbaum and his
associates (Rothbaum, Weisz, Pott, Miyake, & Morelli, 2000) challenged the
assumption that the results of studies on attachment style and personal rela-
tionship processes can be generalized beyond the borders of Western nations.
It is not clear whether other personality variables, such as cultural value ori-
entations (Gaines, Marelich, et al., 1997; Gaines, Rios, et al., 1999), gen-
der-related personality characteristics (Bern, 1974), or self-esteem
(Rosenberg, 1965), would have the same impact on responses to
accommodative dilemmas in inter! :ultural romantic relationships as has been
observed in romantic relationships as a whole. nationality appears
to moderate the impact of certain personality constructs on accommodation.
For example, within the United States (an English-speaking Western nation),
collectivism, romanticism, and positive femininity all promote accommoda-
tion; whereas individualism, familism, spiritualism, positive masculinity, and
self-esteem all yield inconclusive to null results in primarily intracultural ro-
mantic relationships (Gaines, Kim, et al., 2002; Rusbult et al., 1987; Rusbult
et al., 1991; Rusbult et al., 1986). Yet, within Jamaica (an English-speaking
Caribbean nation), the “me-orientation” of individualism clearly undermines
the process of accommodation; whereas the “we-orientations” of romanti-
cism, familism, and spiritualism tend to be unrelated to accommodation in
primarily intracultural romantic relationships (however, collectivism is nega-
tively related to exit; Gaines, Kim, et al., 2002).
Even with regard to attachment style, at least one set of studies
(Wieselquist et al., 1999) h as cast doubt on the assumption that personal-
ity variables explain unique variance in accommodation, be-
11. INTERCULTURAL COUPLES i7-p 247

yond that explained by commitment in romantic relationships. Part of the


problem may lie in the conceptual overlap between commitment and at-
tachment style. For example, the definition of commitment often includes
the notion of psychological “attachment” to relationship partner
(Arriaga & Agnew, 200 1). As students of multiple regression analysis and
structural equation analysis can attest, covariance between predictors
(e.g., commitment and attachment style) may lead to results in which the
computer algorithm arbitrarily assigns the bulk of explained variance in
outcome measures (e.g., exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect) to only one of the
predictors (see the earlier discussion in this chapter concerning commit-
ment and dependence as predictors of decision to remain).
Ridge regression and other advanced techniques sometimes are required to
correct the resulting statistical problems (Pedhazur, 1982; Stevens, 1992).
It is possible that part or all of the explained variance in accommodation is
shared by commitment and attachment style, rather than attributable
solely to commitment or solely to attachment style.

DlRKTlONS FOR I=UTURtE RESEARCH

One obvious challenge for relationships researchers will be to measure all


of the investment model variables among individuals who marry
interculturally and individuals who marry intraculturally. If, for example,
commitment turns out to be significantly lower among individuals in
intercultural marriages than among individuals in intracultural marriages,
then the potential of the investment model variables to help delineate the
specific factors that contribute to this difference is enormous.
Another challenge will be for personal relationship scholars to test the
investment model across multiple types of interethnic marriages. For ex-
ample, although it is difficult to conceive of interracial marriages that are
not also intercultural (Baptiste, 1984), one can readily conceive of
intercultural marriages that are not also interracial (e.g., a marriage be-
tween a Spanish-speaking Black Latin0 born in Havana and an Eng-
lish-speaking Black Latina born in New York City; see Baptiste, 1990). By
comparing means and correlations of investment model variables between
individuals in “purely” intercultural marriages and individuals in marriages
that are interracial as well as intercultural, personal relationship research-
ers will be able to determine the degree to which the investment model ac-
counts for the greater tendency of interracial marriages to end in divorce,
relative to (other) intercultural marriages (see Gaines & Ickes, 1997).
Finally, although personal relationship scholars are encouraged to assess
the relative importance of social-psychological variables (e.g., investment
model variables) as well as personality variables (e.g., cultural value orien-
tations) in predicting relationship outcomes (e.g., response to accommo-
dative dilemmas) in intercultural relationships, scholars are also cautioned
2-H -f++ GAINES AND AGNEW

to think carefully about conceptual and empirical overlap between so-


cial-psychological and personality predictors. The concepts of commit-
ment and attachment, previously mentioned, overlap to some extent.
Similarly, Wieselquist and colleagues (1999) proposed that commitment
may give rise to “a collectivistic, communal orientation, including tenden-
cies to respond to a needs in a rather unconditional manner” (p.
944). Even if one were to set aside potential concerns over terminology
(e.g., the tendency to respond to a needs in an unconditional
manner actually sounds more akin to romanticism than to collectivism; see
Gaines, Rios, et al., 1999), such a proposal may be problematic because it
seems to imply that cultural value orientations-which many theorists and
researchers in cultural psychology and cross-cultural psychology view as
originating within ethnic groups and societies (e.g., 1980;
Triandis, 1995)-originate within relationship dyads.2 If one subsequently
were to place investment model variables and cultural value orientations
into regression models or structural equation models as competing predic-
tors of accommodative responses without controlling statistically for
covariance between the two sets of predictors, then one might encounter
the same problems with statistical and theoretical interpretation that we
have noted. Therefore, hopefully personal relationship scholars will resist
the temptation to dismiss either personality or social-psychological vari-
ables prematurely as predictors of accommodation, stay-leave behaviors,
and other outcomes in intercultural relationships.

CONCLUDING THOUGHTS

At the outset of this chapter, discrepancy between newlywed


positive illusions and the high divorce rates that currently are prevalent in
many Western nations was mentioned. In one sense, such positive illu-
sions may be functional, especially in interethnic romantic relationships.
For example, among interracial couples, romanticism is a positive predic-
tor of displays of affection and respect toward each other
(Gaines, Rios, et al, 1999). However, for many intercultural couples,
positive illusions may not be sufficient to survive widespread societal dis-
approval. investment model, which focuses on factors that con-

‘Some theorists (e.g., Coates, 1999) have proposed that with regard to personal relationship
processes, causal relations between individual and ethnic group, or between individual and society,
are reciprocal. On the one hand, the individual is born into an ethnic group and into a society; vari-
ous socializing agents (e.g., parents, teachers) communicate customs and practices to the individ-
ual (e.g., the expectation that the individual will marry someone from his or her own cultural
group, possibly through arranged marriage). On the other hand, as the individual progresses from
infancy and childhood to adolescence and adulthood, he or she may engage in behaviors (e.g.,
elopement with someone outside own cultural group) that challenges and, possibly, changes
the customs and practices of the ethnic group or society.
11. INTERCULTURAL COUPLES ++ 249

tribute to relationship maintenance via commitment processes, is seen as


an aid to personal relationship researchers in understanding why marital
dissolution is more common among intercultural couples than among
intracultural couples.
In closing, despite the odds that they face, many intercultural couples
successfully maintain stable, satisfying relationships over time. Neither
intercultural nor intracultural couples can be viewed accurately as homo-
geneous. Perhaps those intercultural couples who survive and thrive over
time have implicitly taken to heart the same commitment-enhancing fac-
tors (e.g., extra effort may be required to offset hostile societal pressures)
that have been made explicit in the present chapter. In fact, as is evident
from e-mail messages posted on Internet sites devoted to intercultural
couples (see Gaines & Ickes, 1997), it is not uncommon for women and
men in intercultural relationships to discuss such factors explicitly, be-
tween and among couples. Moreover, as is evident from the results of vari-
ous empirical studies (e.g., Porterfield, 1978; Rosenblatt et al., 1995),
some intercultural couples not only surmount difficult relationship sur-
vival odds but also produce offspring who are physically and psychologi-
cally vibrant. Therefore, hopefully readers will come to appreciate the
successes as well as the challenges that intercultural couples encounter on
a daily basis in striving to maintain their relationships.

RH=lXiENClZ.S

Agnew, C. R., Loving, T. J., & Drigotas, S. M. (2001). Substituting the forest for the
trees: Social networks and the prediction of romantic relationship state and fate.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1042-l 05 7.
Agnew, C. R., Van Lange, I? A. M., Rusbult, C. E., & Langston, C. A. (1998). Cognitive
interdependence: Commitment and the mental representation of close relation-
ships. Journul of Personality & Social Psychology, 74, 939-954.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behav-
ior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Arriaga, X. B., & Agnew, C. R. (2001). Being committed: Affective, cognitive, and co-
native components of relationship commitment. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 2 7, 1190-l 203.
Baptiste, D. A., Jr. (1984). Marital and family therapy with racially/culturally inter-
married stepfamilies: Issues and guidelines. Family Relations, 33, 373-380.
Baptiste, D. A., Jr. (1990). Therapeutic strategies with Black-Hispanic families: Iden-
tity problems of a neglected minority. Journal of Family Psychotherapy, I, 15-38.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A
test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61,
226-244.
Bern, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Coun-
seling & Clinical Psychology, 42, 15 5-162.
Berscheid, E. (1985). Interpersonal attraction. In G. Lindzey and E. Aronson (Eds.),
Handbook of social psychoLogy (vol. 2, 3rd ed., pp. 413-484). New York: Random
Hnrlv
2j0 -e=+ GAINES AND AGNEW

Berscheid, E., & Reis, H. T. (1998). Attraction and close relationships. In D. T Gilbert,
S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook ofsocialpsychology (Vol. 2,4th ed.,
pp. 193-281). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss (WZ. I : Attachment). New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss PZ. 2: Separation). New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and Zoss ml. 3: Loss). New York: Basic Books.
Coates, D. L. (1999). The cultured and culturing aspects of romantic experience in ad-
olescence. In W. Furman, B. B. Brown, & C. Feiring (Eds.), The development of ro-
mantic relationships in adolescence (pp. 330-363). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Cox, C. E., Wexler, M. O., Rusbult, C. E., & Gaines, S. O., Jr, (1997). Prescriptive sup-
port and commitment processes in close relationships. Social Psychology Quarterly,
60, 79-90.
Cramer, E. M. (1972). Significance tests and tests of models in multiple regression.
American Statistician, 26, 26-30.
Crohn, J. (1995). M ixe d marriages. New York: Fawcett Columbine.
Drigotas, S. M., & Rusbult, C. E. (1992). Should I stay or should I go? A dependence
model of breakups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 62-87.
Duck, S. W (1994). Meaningful relationships: Talking, sense, and relating. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Etcheverry, I? E., &Agnew, C. R. (2001, February). Perceptions ofsocial network support
and commitment to romantic relationships. Paper presented at the annual meeting of
the Society of Personality and Social Psychology; San Antonio, TX.
Feeney, J., & Noller, I? (1996). Adult attachment. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row-Peterson.
Foa, U. G., &Foa, E. B. (1974).S ocietalstructures ofthe mind. Springfield, IL: Thomas.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., Chalfin, J., Kim, M., & Taing, I? (1998). Communicating prejudice in
personal relationships. In M. L. Hecht (Ed.), Communicating prejudice (pp.
163-l 86). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., Granrose, C. S., Rios, D. I., Garcia, B. F., Page, M. S., Farris, K. R., &
Bledsoe, K. L. (1999). Patterns of attachment and responses to accommodative di-
lemmas among interethnic/interracial couples. Journal of Social and Personal Rela-
tionships, 16, 275-285.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., & Ickes, W (1997). Perspectives on interracial relationships. In S.
Duck (Ed.), Handbook ofp ersonal relationships: Theory, research and interventions
(2nd ed., pp. 197-220). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., Kim, M., Gilstrap, S., Yi, J., Rusbult, C. E., Hardin, D., Gaertner, L.
A., Ramkissoon, M., & Matthies, B. K. (2002). impact of cultural values on accom-
modative responses in the United States and-Jamaica. Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., & Liu, J. H. (2000). Multicultural/multiracial relationships. In C.
Hendrick & S. S. Hendrick (Eds.), Close relationships:A sourcebook (pp. 97-108).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., Marelich, W. D., Bledsoe, K. L., Steers, W N., Henderson, M. C.,
Granrose, C. S., Barajas, L., Hicks, D., Lyde, M., Takahashi, Y., Yum, N., Rios, D. I.,
Garcia, B. F., Farris, K., & Page, M. S. (1997). Links between race/ethnicity and cul-
tural values as mediated by racial/ethnic identity and moderated by gender. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1460-I 4 76.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., Reis, H. T., Summers, S., Rusbult, C. E., Cox, C. L., Wexler, M. O.,
Marelich, W D., & Kurland, G. J. (1997). Impact of attachment style on reactions
to accommodative dilemmas in close relationships. Personal Relationships, 4,
93-113.
11. INTERCULTURAL COUPLES * 251

Gaines, S. O., Jr., Rios, D. I., Granrose, C. S., Bledsoe, K. L., Farris, K. R., Page Youn,
M. S., & Garcia, B. F. (1999). R omanticism and interpersonal resource exchange
among African American/Anglo and other interracial couples. Journal ofBZu& Psy-
chology, 2.5, 461-489.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., Work, C., Johnson, H., Page Youn, M. S., & Lai, K. (2000). Impact of
attachment style and self-monitoring on responses to accommodative
dilemmas across relationship types. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, I 7,
767-789.
Gordon, A. (1964). Intermarriage. Boston: Beacon Press.
Gurung, R. A. R., & Duong, T. (1999). M ixing and matching: Assessing the
concomitants of mixed-ethnic relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relu-
tionships, 16, 639-657.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, I? (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment pro-
cess. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 5 1 l-524.
Ho, M. K. (1990). Intermarried couples in therapy. Springfield, IL: Thomas.
Hofstede, G. (1980). consequences. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Johnson, D. J., & Rusbult, C. E. (1989). Resisting temptation: Devaluation of alterna-
tive partners as a means of maintaining commitment in close relationships. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 967-980.
Johnson, M. P (1991a). Commitment to personal relationships. In W. H. Jones, & D.
Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 3, pp. 117-l 43). London:
Jessica Kingsley.
Johnson, M. I? (1991 b). Reply to Levinger and Rusbult. In W H. Jones, & D. Perlman
(Eds.), Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 3, pp. 17 l-l 76). London: Jessica
Kingsley.
Johnson, M. P. (1995). Patriarchal terrorism and common couple violence: Two forms
of violence against women. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 283-294.
Kelley, H. H. (1979). Personal relationships: Their structures and processes. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kelley, H. H., & Thibaut, J. W. (1978). Interpersonal relations: A theory of interdepen-
dence. New York: Wiley.
Le, B., & Agnew, C. R. (in press). Commitment and its theorized determinants: A
meta-analysis of the Investment Model. Personal Relationships.
Lewis, R. A., & Spanier, G. B. (1979). Th eorizing about the quality and stability of mar-
riage. In W H. Burn, R. Hill, F. I. Nye, & I. L. Reiss (Eds.), Contemporary theories
about the family (Vol. 1, pp. 268-294). New York: Free Press.
Pedhazur, E. J. (1982). MultipZe regression in behavioral research (2nd ed.). New York:
Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Porterfield, E. (1978). Black and White mixed marriages. Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Rosenblatt, I!! C., Karis, T. A., & Powell, R. D. (1995). Multiracial couples: Black and
White voices. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J., Pott, M., Miyake, K., & Morelli, G. (2000). Attachment and
culture: Security in the United States and Japan. American Psychologist, 55,
1093-l 104.
Rusbult, C. E. [ 1980). C ommitment and satisfaction in romantic associations: A test of
the investment model. Journal of Experimental SociaZ Psychology, 16, 172-l 86.
Rusbult, C. E. (1991). C ommentary on “Commitment to personal relation-
ships”: What interesting and new? In W. H. Jones & D. Perlman (Eds.), Ad-
vances in personal relationships (Vol. 3, pp. 15 l-l 69). London: Jessica Kingsley.
252 +- GAINES AND AGNEW

Rusbult, C. E., & Arriaga, X. B. (1997). Interdependence theory. In S. Duck (Ed.),


Handbook ofpersonal relationships: Theory, research and interventions (2nd ed., pp.
22 l-250). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Rusbult, C. E., Arriaga, X. B., & Agnew, C. R. (2001). Interdependence in close rela-
tionships. In G. J. 0. Fletcher & M. S. Clark (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social
psychology, Vol. 2: lnterpersonalprocesses (pp. 359-387). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Rusbult, C. E., & Buunk, B. I? (1993). C ommitment processes in close relationships: An
interdependence analysis. Special Issue: Relational maintenance. Journal of Social
and Personal Relationships, 10, 175-204.
Rusbult, C. E., Drigotas, S. M., & Verette, J. (I 994). The investment model: An inter-
dependence analysis of commitment processes and relationship maintenance phe-
nomena. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational
maintenance (p. 115-139). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Rusbult, C. E., & Martz, J. M. (1995). R emaining in an abusive relationship: An invest-
ment model analysis of nonvoluntary dependence. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 21, 558-571.
Rusbult, C. E., Martz, J. M., & Agnew, C. R. (1998). The Investment Model Scale:
Measuring commitment level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and invest-
ment size. Personal Relationships, 5, 357-391.
Rusbult, C. E., Morrow, G. D., &Johnson, D. J. (1987). Self-esteem and problem solv-
ing behavior in close relationships. British Journal ofSocial Psychology, 26,293-303.
Rusbult, C. E., Verette, J., Whitney, G. A., Slovik, L. F., & Lipkus, I. (1991). Accommo-
dation processes in close relationships: Theory and preliminary evidence. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 53-78.
Rusbult, C. E., Zembrodt, I. M., & Iwaniszek, J. (1986). The impact of gender and
sex-role orientation on responses to dissatisfaction in close relationships. Sex Roles,
1.5, l-20.
Rusbult, C. E., Zembrodt, I. M., & Gunn, L. K. (1982). Exit, voice, loyalty, neglect: Re-
sponses to dissatisfaction in romantic involvements. Journal of Personality and So-
cial Psychology, 43, 1230-l 242.
Scharfe, E., & Bartholomew, K. (1995). Accommodation and attachment representa-
tions in young couples. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, I2,389-401.
Shibazaki, K., & Brennan, K. A. (1998). When birds of different feathers flock to-
gether: A preliminary comparison of intra-ethnic and inter-ethnic dating relation-
ships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, IS, 248-256.
Stevens, J. (1992). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (2nd ed.).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Surra, C. A., & Hughes, D. K. (1997). Commitment processes in accounts of the devel-
opment of premarital relationships. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 59,5-2 1.
Surra, C. A., Hughes, D. K., & Jacquet, S. E. (1999). The development of commitment
to marriage: A phenomenological approach. In J. M. Adams, & W. H. Jones (Eds.),
Handbook of interpersonal commitment and relationship stability (pp. 125-l 48).
New York: Plenum.
Thibaut, J. WI, & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of groups. New York:
Wiley.
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Tucker, M. B., & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (1995). S ocial structural and psychological corre-
lates of interethnic dating. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 12,
341-361.
Van Lange, I? A. M., & Rusbult, C. E. (1995). My relationship is better than-and not as
bad as-yours is: The perception of superiority in close relationships. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 32-44.
11. INTERCULTURAL COUPLES 3t, 253

Van Lange, l? A. M., Rusbult, C. E., Drigotas, S. M., Arriaga, X. B., Witcher, B. S., &
Cox, C. L. (1997). Willingness to sacrifice in close relationships. JournaZ ofPersonal-
ity and Social Psychology, 72, 1373-l 395.
Waite, L. J., Bachrach, C., Hindin, M., Thomson, E., & Thornton, A. (Eds.). (2000).
The ties that bind: Perspectives on marriage and cohabitation. New York: Aldine de
Gruyter.
Wieselquist, J., Rusbult, C. E., Foster, C. A., & Agnew, C. R. (1999). Commitment,
pro-relationship behavior, and trust in close relationships. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 77, 942-966.
This page intentionally left blank
Maintai “ages
in Russia: Managing
Societal Influences
and Communication Dynamics

Deborah S. Ballard-Reisch
Marat Zaguidoulline
Daniel J.
University of Nevada, Reno

or U.S. couples, the road to domestic bliss is a rough one. Due to a vari-
ety of complex political, economic, and social factors, it is even rougher for
Russian couples. Although over 90% of Americans (Gottman & Carrere,
1994) and Russians (Boutenko & Razlogov, 1997) will at some time be mar-
ried, a significant percentage of marriages in both countries will end in di-
vorce. For U.S. couples, the odds of divorce are about 50-50 (Olson &
Matskovsky, 1994) with the number of divorces leveling off in recent years
(Teachman, 2000). For Russian couples, the divorce rate is increasing. In
1970, approximately 30% of marriages ended in divorce. By 1995, the rate
had risen to more than 60% (Russian Statistical Yearbook, 1996).
For more than 20 years in both the U.S. (see Adams &Jones, 1999)
and Russia (see Vannoy et al., 1999), the dynamic nature of marriages

‘The authors would like to thank Marcia Sarratea and Mary Elton for their assistance at various
stages of the preparation of this manuscript.
255
zjo -i++ BALLARD-REISCH, ZAGUIDOULLINE, WEIGEL

and high rates of divorce have led social scientists to study why some
marriages succeed while so many others fail. Researchers have contrib-
uted significant time and effort trying to understand what factors
within and outside marriages impact their success, and how strategies
successful couples use to sustain their marriages differ from the strate-
gies of couples who divorce.
The purpose of this chapter is to explore the factors that impact the
maintenance of marriages in Russia in this time of social, political, and eco-
nomic uncertainty. To understand marriages in Russia at the beginning of
the 21 st century, one must first understand the complex array of both
macrolevel and microlevel factors that impinge on family stability. In order
to perform this analysis, the “Ecological influences on marital commit-
ment” component of Ballard-Reisch and (1999) “Model of Com-
munication Processes in Marital Commitment” is used as an organizing
framework. In addition, the results of research in Russia are compared
with research findings on U.S. couples.

A MODEL OF COMMUNICATION PROCEiSES


IN MARITAL COMMITMENT

In general, the Ballard-Reisch and Weigel (1999) model advances that


through communication, couples negotiate the roles, power, and cli-
mate within their marriages. This dynamic process of negotiation is also
impacted by the context or “ecological influences” within which rela-
tionships exist. Based on the ecological perspective of Bronfenbrenner
(1986), marriages are seen as existing within a “series of expanding
spheres of influence” that have the potential to impact the marital rela-
tionship (p. 4 17). These spheres of influence can be viewed as concen-
tric circles. In the center circle is the marital couple sphere. This circle is
composed of the marital relationship and the internal communication
dynamics and patterns through which a couple develops, maintains, and
changes their marriage. The next circle out contains the immediate so-
cial sphere of the couple and is composed of people and forces such as
immediate and extended family and friends who implicitly or explicitly
sanction the relationship and have the potential to impact the marriage.
The final circle is the societal influences sphere, including values and
norms, “socioeconomic, cultural, political, technological, and environ-
mental influences” (p. 4 18) that impact marriages and families. For the
purposes of this chapter, the societal influences sphere and the marital
couple sphere are examined, as these are the areas that have thus far
been researched in Russian culture.
12. MAINTAINING MARRIAGES IN RUSSIA -a- 257

Societal influences Sphere: Russian Marriages in Context


At the societal level, the cultural environment, including historical, social,
political, and economic factors, has the ability to impact decisions
regarding their marriages. Such issues as moral constraints to remain in a
marriage and rigid divorce laws (Johnson, 199 l), cultural encouragement
to leave unsatisfying marriages, or to stay in unsatisfying marriages for the
sake of the children (Vannoy et al., 1999), and political or economic condi-
tions (Ballard-Reisch, Weigel & Zaguidoulline, 1999) are all societal level
issues that have the potential to exert pressure on couples that may impact
their relationships directly or indirectly. This section explores historical,
economic, and demographic factors as they impact the stability of Russian
marriages.

Historical Factors
Russian culture has traditionally been grounded in essentialism, the view
that there are inherent biologically based differences between women and
men that manifest themselves in psychological and behavioral differences
(see Vannoy et al., 1999). In the family, fathers were supposed to be author-
ity figures (head of the family) who manifested love for their families not so
much emotionally as practically, by protecting the family and providing for
their needs (Attwood, 1990). They were to maintain order through encour-
aging appropriate behavior and punishing inappropriate behavior. The natu-
ral “masculine approach” of fathers allowed them to exercise necessary
strictness with children, a role contrary to the affectionate and compassion-
ate nature of mothers (heart of the family) (Attwood 1990).
A family thus represented a complementary union of female and male
roles that were to balance each other in the process of the upbringing of
children. In this context, the division of labor in the family was seen as nat-
ural. The primary role of bread-winner required utilization of his
physical strength and other qualities in order to support the family and his
role was coordinated with role in running the household, pleasing
her husband, and caring for their children (Rimashevskaya, 1992).
Overlaying this fundamental essentialism was a belief in the basic supe-
riority of men over women (see Gal & Khgman, 2000). From the time of
the Tsars when the Russian Orthodox Church proclaimed the natural infe-
riority of females to males and the responsibility of males to make certain
that females followed the teachings of the church, until the October Revo-
lution in 19 17, this perspective was the foundation of Russian culture.
Shortly after the October Revolution in 19 17, the Bolsheviks declared
the full economic, social, and political equality of women and men in Rus-
sia and abolished all laws restricting the rights of women. In addition,
228 -es BALLARD-REISCH, ZAGUIDOULLINE, WEIGEL

age-old presumptions of male dominance were to be rooted out and re-


placed by new principles of Marxist-Leninist ideology (see Lenin, 1966).
The conflict between beliefs regarding biological essentialism and patri-
archy in contrast to communist notions of equality were further com-
pounded by inconsistency on the part of the government in advancing
equality. These contradictions set the stage for enduring confusion regard-
ing the appropriate roles of women and men in Russian marriages.
It was goal to liberate women from the bondage of what he
called the “bourgeois family,” especially from domestic work that he de-
scribed as ‘(barbaric, unproductive, petty, enervating, stupefying, and de-
pressing,” by giving women the freedom to work and transforming
household maintenance and childrearing into communal responsibilities
(Hansson & Liden, 1983; Morvant, 1995). This was to have allowed
women to participate actively in the construction and administration of
the new society. By the 192Os, however, the plan for the transference of
the economic and educational functions of the household to the state had
been put aside in order to meet other more pressing needs (Lapidus,
1983). With World War II and its aftermath participation in the
workforce became more an economic necessity than an ideological issue
for the state due to a sharp decline in male workers. Women, pressured to
enter the workplace and to retain full responsibility for their homes and
families (Lapidus, 1983), were forced into a “double burden” that they
still experience today (Rimashevskaya, 1992; Roudakova & Ballard-
Reisch, 2000).
Since the 197Os, the Soviet concern about potential
unemployment and falling birthrates has led to the revision of its ideo-
logical stance on the “woman question.” The new policy manifested it-
self in public messages that full employment had led to the loss of
femininity in women, and that women have a natural need to give birth
to children and to care for the household (Attwood, 1990; Sperling,
2000). These statements served as the foundation of the “women go
home” ideology perpetuated throughout much of the late Soviet period
up to the present (Mamonova, 1994; Sperling, 2000). Pragmatically,
however, women cannot escape their “double burden” as their eco-
nomic participation remains imperative for the survival of their families
(Silverman & Yanowitch, 1997).
Reflecting on the experience of women under socialism, Gal and
Kligman (2000) concluded that there were both benefits and disadvan-
tages. They wrote:

On the one hand, women gained a sense of gratification, moral superiority, and
power in the household from their centrality and apparent indispensability. T&y
also gained a somewhat different, more autonomous sense of self-worth and
self-esteem from participation in the labor force.. . . On the other hand, the con-
12. MAINTAINING MARRIAGES IN RUSSIA ++I- 25:

d&ions of work, the low wages, and the magnitude of demands on them pro-
duced a sense of victimization and perennial guilt at their never being able to do
enough of anything, especially mothering. (p. 53)

In many ways, Soviet men fared worse than Soviet women. Although
there were at least some positives for women who bore this “double burden”
in terms of self-esteem and self-worth, men found themselves unable to ful-
fill their roles as breadwinners due to economic factors that were beyond
their control and without alternative roles to play. Salaries average workers
received in the 1960s to the 1980s were not enough to provide adequately
for the whole family. To meet a modest subsistence level both spouses had to
work and earn the average national wage (Matthews, 1986). Struggling to
support their families, resilient men held several part-time or temporary
jobs in addition to their full-time jobs (e.g., reselling goods, providing private
services, manufacturing goods at home), engaged in the illegal production of
goods, and took part in small- and large-scale theft from their places of work
(Schapiro & Godson, 1984). This mode of operation normally meant a con-
stant absence of the husband and father from daily family life. Such condi-
tions, compounded by constant shortages of goods and housing (see
Matthews, 1989; Morton, 1987; Seeger, 1984), left Soviet men no grounds
on which to justify their dominant position in the family.
In addition, the communist regime usurped “head of household” as a
masculine image and produced very few alternative pictures of masculinity
linked to roles in the family or household (Gal & Kligman, 2000).
Gal and Kligman summarized what has become a popular conception of
Soviet men. Although dominant in the work place, Soviet men were “dis-
organized, needy, dependent, vulnerable, demanding to be taken care of
and sheltered, to be humored as they occasionally acted out with aggres-
sion, alcoholism, womanizing, or absenteeism” in the home (p. 54).

Summary

Gal and Kligman (2000) made clear that role conflict for both men and
women when combined with lax divorce laws and state support of women
and children led to the extreme fragility of marriage ties. In a culture in which
women are supposed to manage the home and nurture the family, but must
also work full time outside the home in order to supplement the financial sup-
port their husbands give the family, women feel overburdened, oppressed,
and exhausted. In a culture in which men find identity in their ability to be
heads of households, to provide for their families, and yet are unable to do so
because of economic conditions that create a need for their spouses to work,
men become disconsolate. Neither of these conditions bode well for strong,
sustainable marriages and high levels of dissension and divorce are the result.
Economic issues following perestroika did nothing to improve the situation.
2a +i==+ BALLARD-REISCH, ZAGUIDOULLINE, WEIGEL

Economic Factors

The period of economic transition that began in 199 1 led to a more se-
vere deterioration of living standards. Attempts by the government to in-
troduce capitalism quickly resulted in skyrocketing consumer and
industrial prices, decreasing production, increasing unemployment, and
loss of savings. According to the World Bank (1996), from 1989 to 1996,
the Gross Domestic Product (GDP) in Russia declined approximately
50%. In comparison, during the Great Depression in the U.S., the GDP
fell only about 30% (Connor, 2000).
Consumer prices increased a staggering 3,204% from the years 1991
through 1998 (IMF, 2000; United Nations, ZOOOb), whereas monthly
wages increased only 1,915% (Robertson, 2000). To further compound
this problem, government wages were often not paid on time. By February
1999, the government owed wages to 118,076 enterprises totaling approx-
imately $2,7 15 million U.S. dollars (Robertson, 2000). At the end of 1999,
35% of the population had incomes below the subsistence wage (Robert-
son, 2000).
Unemployment has also been a significant problem ranging from 4.8%
of the potential workforce in 1992 to 11.5% in 1998 (Connor, 2000). One
important trend has been the disproportionate number of women unem-
ployed. At times, women have made up more than 90% of the unemployed
in some cities (Sperling, 2000).

Summary
Overall, although the economic situation during the Soviet period was
difficult, the period of economic transition has proven to be even more of a
hardship for Russian families. The economic conditions have a crucial im-
pact on the number of Russian marriages, their duration, reproductive ac-
tivity, and health and life expectancy. These areas are covered in the next
section.
Demographic Factors
The Russian family is in crisis. The population is shrinking, life expec-
tancy is shortened, the marriage rate is declining, and the divorce rate con-
tinues to be high. Population growth has been negative since 1992. In
1998, the population represented only 98.7% of the 1991 population
(Robertson, 2000). Life expectancy at birth has declined from 74.3 years
for women and 63.8 years for men in 1990 to 72.9 years for women and
6 1.3 years for men in 1998 (United Nations, 2000a).
Likewise, marital stability has decreased. The marriage rate declined
from around 8.9% in 1990 (United Nations, 2000a) to 5.8% in 1998 (Rob-
ertson, 2000). Age at marriage increased for women from 22.6 years of age
12. MAINTAINING MARRIAGES IN RUSSIA ==a- &I

in 1990 to 22.7 in 1998 and for men from 24.7 to 25.0 years (United Na-
tions, 2000a). The divorce rate remains high with 59.8% of marriages end-
ing in divorce in 1997 and 59.1% in 1998 (Robertson, 2000).
Women in Russia today will have an average of 1.2 children down from
1.9 in 1990 (United Nations, 2000a). On a positive note, children born
will have a better chance of survival. The infant mortality rate increased
from 1991 to 1993 from 17.8 deaths per 1000 people in 1991 to 19.9 in
1993 and steadily decreased to 16.5 deaths per 1000 people in 1998 (Rob-
ertson, 2000). At the same time, the rate of abortions to live births, al-
though on the decline since 1990, still remains very high with 1,971.l
abortions per 1,000 live births in 1990 and 1,722.3 abortions per 1,000 live
births in 1998. Comparable U.S. statistics indicated 386.9 abortions per
1,000 live births in 1990 and 348.9 per 1,000 in 1996 (United Nations,
2000a). Since Soviet times, abortion has been the major source of birth
control in Russia due in part to the unavailability or low quality of alterna-
tive methods of contraception (Olson & Matskovsky, 1994).

Summary

Societal influences impacting Russian families have continuously under-


gone change and transformation since the October Revolution of 19 17.
Economic and political forces have powerfully impacted the private lives
of individual couples and the ways they have created and maintained their
marriages. Unresolved contradictions between ideology, cultural tradition,
and economic practices during Soviet times and since perestroika have
given rise to and sustained universal role conflicts that have perpetuated a
“double burden” for women and an identity crisis for men. The declining
quality of life, health, and reproductivity serve as indicators of the degree
of physical and psychological hardship experienced by families in contem-
porary Russia.
As the Ballard-Reisch and Weigel(1999) model of communication pro-
cesses in marital commitment indicates, the strategies couples use to nego-
tiate roles, power, and their marital climate are clearly impacted by the
nature of the societal influences acting upon them. By the same token, the
communication patterns they develop and the structures they create
within their relationships are as much a result of internal relational dynam-
ics as they are a response to societal influences. In the next section, re-
search concerning the marital couple sphere of the Ballard-Reisch and
Weigel (1999) model is examined. The communication patterns couples
develop in their relationships and the relationship structures that emerge
from and sustain these patterns are explored.
202 ++ BALLARD-REISCH, ZAGUIDOULLINE, WEIGEL

Marital couple sph ere: Constructing Relationships


Through Communication

According to the Ballard-Reisch and Weigel(1999) model, it is through in-


teraction that married couples both structure their relationships, negotiat-
ing roles, power, and a marital climate, and develop patterned approaches
to maintaining or changing their marriages. These two dynamics mutually
influence one another. Communication within a marriage forms, changes,
and reinforces the structure of marital relationships while the structure of
a marriage impacts communication within that marriage (Ballard-Reisch &
Weigel, 1999). Many researchers have argued that the quality of a marriage
depends on the interaction between partners (Canary & Stafford, 1992;
Dainton & Stafford, 1993). Through interaction couples share their per-
spectives and form and reinforce their joint reality (Duck, 1994). As Wood
(1995) concluded, it is through the large and small conversations that cou-
ples “weave their lives together” (p. 6).
This section of the chapter focuses on (a) the emerging body of research
on family interaction patterns and their impact on marital quality out-
comes, specifically research on relationship maintenance behaviors and
feminine and masculine communication patterns; and (b) research con-
cerning relationship trajectories including: the approaches to, types of, and
developmental stages within Russian families.

Family Interaction Patterns


Russia has a long history of studying interpersonal relationships in fami-
lies. Although theory and research have been grounded in a variety of pri-
marily psychological theories and sociological investigations, it is
important to note that Soviet ideology impacted not only the conclusions
drawn from data, but also both the questions asked and the answers of-
fered. A decade after perestroika, a growing number of international inves-
tigations comparing Russian and U.S. families in particular are coming to
fruition. Both the theoretical foundation in Soviet times and this new
emerging body of research will be explored in this section.
In the early 198Os, Sysenko (1981, 1983) identified a two pronged ap-
proach underlying the study of families in Russia. The first, typically utiliz-
ing demographic indicators, focused on the stability of Russian marriages.
Studies in this tradition examined falling birth rates, rising divorce rates,
decreasing marriage rates, etc. Trends found in these studies were dis-
cussed earlier under demographics.
The second approach to the study of families in Russia has emphasized
the sustainability of marriages. One emerging focus in the literature is
communication. Within this focus, researchers have examined internal re-
12. MAINTAINING MARRIAGES IN RUSSIA -+- 263

lationship dynamics, roles, attitudes, behaviors, etc. In an essay on the fam-


ily during Soviet times and beyond, Malysheva (1992) summarized
primary themes that guided family research in this tradition during the So-
viet period. They included marital role breakdown, the structure of power
in families, divorce and conflict, motives for marriage and marriage disso-
lution, family hierarchy and values, and family problems following divorce.
In summarizing the research on Soviet families, Boss and Gurko (1994)
emphasized that Soviet family specialists often stress the relationship be-
tween effective spousal communication and marital quality. In addition, in
reference to current family status in Russia they conclude, “In a society
characterized by confusion and conflict, clear communication, empathy,
and mutual understanding are at a premium” (p. 46).

Marital Quality Outcomes


In studies with U.S. couples, quality communication is one of the key
components of successful marriages (Noller & Fitzpatrick, 1990). Re-
search has consistently shown that constructive, positive communication
is associated with marital satisfaction (Holman & Brock, 1986; Noller &
Fitzpatrick, 1990) and marital commitment (Rusbult & Buunk, 1993;
Sprecher, 1988). Noller and Fitzpatrick (1990) found that satisfied cou-
ples can be distinguished from dissatisfied couples on the basis of their use
of positive affect, accurate interpretation of one communication
behaviors, and positive communication. Additionally, satisfied couples
spend more time communicating and use more constructive communica-
tion than do less satisfied couples (White, 1983; Zuo, 1992).
Similar results have been found in research on Russian couples. Boss and
Gurko (1994), f or example, identified the factors most important to rela-
tional satisfaction for both wives and husbands as their manifestations of
good feelings toward one another, psychological support from spouse,
and the ability of spouses to understand one another. On an interesting note,
wives in 1984 study (cited in Boss & Gurko, 1994) reported under-
standing their husbands more than husbands reported understanding their
wives. While in fact, wives may understand husbands better than their hus-
bands understand them, it is important to note that it is a traditional pre-
scription in Russian culture that “in every woman there should be an
enigma” (B xaqoii xenrrr;nne AoJrma GHT~ saronKa). In other words,
husbands are not supposed to be able to understand their wives. Wives are
supposed to maintain an element of mystery for their husbands.
Research conducted by Gurko in 1989 (unpublished data cited in Boss
& Gurko, 1994) reported on a study of 233 married couples in Moscow.
She found that for wives, the major factors impacting their overall marital
satisfaction were (in rank order): (1) satisfaction with her atti-
tude toward her, (2) satisfaction with sexual relations with her husband,
304 +- BALLARD-REISCH, ZAGUIDOULLINE, WEIGEL

(3) satisfaction with the assistance her parents provide in terms


of child care, (4) satisfaction with how her husband spends his free time,
(5) satisfaction with housing, and (6) satisfaction with the distribution of
household responsibilities (pp. 55 & 73).
For husbands, the major factors effecting their martial satisfaction were
(in rank order): (1) satisfaction with his attitude toward him, (2)
satisfaction with sexual relations with his wife, (3) satisfaction with his job,
(4) satisfaction with how he spends his free time, (5) satisfaction with his
friends and acquaintances, and (6) satisfaction with the ways deci-
sions are made in the family (Boss & Gurko, 1994, pp. 55 & 73). In inter-
preting these results, Boss and Gurko (1994) concluded that satisfaction
with their emotional and sexual relationships and the manner of communi-
cation with their spouses are of primary importance in determining the
martial satisfaction of Russian couples. But there are gender differences.
Whereas husbands find it important to be satisfied with their jobs and how
they spend their free time, wives are more concerned with family values
including the upbringing of their children and how their husbands spend
free time (staying at home and helping out). In a study of 200 married cou-
ples in Moscow, Gozman and Aleshin (1987) . found that satisfaction with
marriages was impacted by issues around responsibilities in childrearing,
satisfaction with their employment outside the home, and the
quality of their social lives.
Research on U.S. couples (Dainton, Stafford & Canary, 1994; Stafford
& Canary, 1991; Weigel & Ballard-Reisch, 1997) and with Russian couples
(Ballard-Reisch, Weigel & Zaguidoulline, 1999) have shown that the be-
havior of both wives and husbands impact self and partner outcomes like
satisfaction and commitment to the marriage. Two specific approaches to
research have proven illustrative in both U.S. and Russian marriages, the
study of relationship maintenance behaviors and the study of the impact of
gender orientation characteristics.

Relationship Maintenance Behaviors


A variety of behaviors have been found to play a positive role in preserv-
ing ongoing relationships. These behaviors have been termed relationship
maintenance behaviors. According to Stafford (1994), relationship main-
tenance behaviors are those that ensure the continuation of valued rela-
tionships through the prevention of their decline, through their
enhancement, or through their repair and reestablishment. Among them
are strategic behaviors including the use of assurances about the future of
the relationship, positivity (communicating in a cheerful, optimistic, and
uncritical manner), openness (communicating directly and openly about
the relationship) (Canary & Stafford, 1992), conflict management, and ad-
vice giving (Stafford, Dainton & Haas, 2000). Additionally, routine behav-
12. MAINTAINING MARRIAGES IN RUSSIA N 265

iors, including sharing tasks and joint activities within social networks
(Dainton & Stafford, 1993), h ave been identified as useful in maintaining
relationships. Some of these areas have been studied within Russian mar-
riages whereas others remain to be studied.

Assurances and Posi tivity. In their study of 32 1 married couples in


the Tatarstan region of Russia, Ballard-Reisch, Weigel, and
Zaguidoulline (1999) f ound that for both wives and husbands, their
own personal use of assurances and positivity were linked to their per-
ceptions of marital quality. This positive self-communication was even
more important to satisfaction than the behavior of spouse. Addi-
tionally, communication of assurances and positivity were
strongly related to satisfaction.

Openness. use of openness was negatively associated with


their perceptions of marital satisfaction indicating that “while dealing with
economic, political, and social turmoil outside the marriage, husbands
need positive communication in general, but they do not necessarily wish
to communicate openly about the marital relationship” (Ballard-Reisch et
al., 1999, p. 694).

Conflict Management. In a study assessing conflict management in


Russian couples living in Moscow, Pskov, and Saratov, and its impact on
marital quality, Vannoy et al. (1999) f ound that couples who reported
low levels of conflict in their marriage also reported higher levels of
marital quality. Among those couples who did report conflict, those
who reported communicating to resolve conflicts through discussion
and compromise reported higher marital quality than couples who did
not talk to resolve their conflicts or work toward a compromise accept-
able to both spouses.

Sharing Tasks. The findings regarding the impact of sharing tasks on


outcomes in Russian marriages seem to be inconsistent across studies,
perhaps because, as noted in earlier sections, the messages within Russian
culture regarding roles and relationships have been inconsistent (see
Arutiunian, 1987; Arutiunian & Zdravomyslova, 1994). For example,
Gozman and Aleshin (1987) f ound that both wives and husbands become
more traditional as their relationships develop, particularly through the
childrearing period with wives wanting more emotional and practical
contact with their husbands, whereas husbands want less of both with
their wives. They conclude that as a result of these trends, marital satis-
faction seems to be related to tolerance of their inde-
pendence and autonomy, and assessments of the extent to
266 -c=+ BALLARD-REISCH, ZAGUIDOULLINE, WEIGEL

which wives play their traditional roles as mothers and supporters


(Gozman & Aleshin, 1987). Gurko (1983), reported in Boss & Gurko
(1994), supported this conclusion finding that both and
expectations regarding roles become more conservative and traditional as
their marriages progress. Further, as Vannoy et al. (1999) found, more
traditional approaches to roles seem to lead to higher satisfaction for
wives and husbands in both urban and rural areas with the exception of
husbands in dual career couples who were more satisfied if their wives
had less traditional gender roles.
In a number of U.S. studies, satisfaction with both their roles and their
perceptions of the equity of the division of labor within the house are posi-
tively related to marital outcomes (Berk, 1985; Coltrane, 1989; Weigel &
Weigel, 1993). This is similar to findings with Russian couples. For exam-
ple, Ballard-Reisch, Weigel, and Zaguidoulline (1999) found that the will-
ingness of Russian wives to carry out their expected tasks was connected
with their personal marital satisfaction as well as their satisfac-
tion with and commitment to the marriage. Similarly, perfor-
mance of expected tasks were positively related to both their personal
satisfaction with and commitment to the marriage and with their
commitment to the marriage (Ballard-Reisch et al., 1999).
In their study of married couples, Vannoy et al. (1999) concluded that
“satisfaction with the division of labor in housekeeping and child care is im-
portant for both marital quality, especially When either
wives or husbands feel that the division of labor is unjust or inappropriate,
their marital quality declines” (p. 138).

Feminine and Masculine Communication Styles


An emerging area of study in U.S. and Russian families is concerned
with the impact of use of traditional feminine and masculine
communication styles and marital quality outcomes. The traditional femi-
nine communication style has been viewed as more expressive and rela-
tionship oriented, whereas traditional masculine communication has been
seen as more instrumental and individual oriented (Bern, 1974).
In recent research on the relationship between gender orientation and
relational maintenance behaviors in U.S. couples, Stafford, Dainton, and
Haas (2000) f ound that the femininity subscale of the Bern Sex Role In-
ventory (BSRI) was more strongly correlated with all relational mainte-
nance factors: assurances, positivity, openness, conflict management,
advice giving, sharing tasks, and social networks, than were masculinity or
biological sex. These findings are consistent with prior research that
views femininity, and traditionally, females, as a more relationally ori-
ented construct than masculinity, and traditionally, males (Wood, 1993).
As Ragsdale (1996) summarized, maintaining relationships is
12. MAINTAINING MARRIAGES IN RUSSIA +=+ 267

work. Interestingly, however, the picture is not that simple. Results also
indicate that the masculinity subscale of the BSRI, which presupposes a
more instrumental or task oriented approach to relationships, was related
to four of the relational maintenance factors in the Stafford, Dainton,
and Haas (2000) study. The four factors were advice giving, assurances,
openness, and positivity. Instrumentality, however, accounted for signif-
icantly less variance than did sensitivity.
Vannoy et al. (1999) studied masculinity and femininity as they re-
lated to both self- and partner perceptions of marital quality in Russian
marriages. Their findings were consistent with those advanced by
Stafford, Dainton, and Haas (2000). Instrumentality (the masculinity
subscale of the BSRI) was positively related to marital quality in wives
and husbands if used by husbands, but not in either husbands or wives if
used by wives. They saw this finding as “consistent with traditional, patri-
archal assumptions that men are to direct and women are to soothe feel-
ings” (p. 180). More significantly, they found that both self- and partner
assessments of sensitivity (the femininity subscale of the BSRI) were
strongly related to both wives and husbands assessments of marital qual-
ity. They concluded that “husbands and wives who are emotionally sensi-
tive and attuned to the feelings of others increase their own and their
spouses marital quality” (p. 1SO).

Summary

Clearly a growing body of research is beginning to examine couple communi-


cation and its impact on outcomes in Russian marriages. In addition to marital
quality outcomes, however, couple communication in conjunction with social
factors facilitates the emergence of relationship trajectories as couples commu-
nicate to negotiate, maintain, and change roles, power, and marital climate
(Ballar *d-Reisch & Weigel, 1999). In this final section, approaches to marriage,
family types, and developmental stages in Russian marriages are examined.

Relationship Trajectories

Patterns that emerge within relationships are impacted by societal influ-


ences like history, economics, and demographics. The patterns themselves,
however, are formed within relationships through interaction. In an effort
to understand the dynamics of Russian marriages, three approaches to re-
search on Russian families have attempted to identify patterned ways in
which Russian couples structure their relationships: (a) overarching ap-
proaches to marriage, (b) f amily types that have emerged during different
historical periods, and (c) developmental stages that seem to emerge from
couple accounts of their satisfaction and perceptions of marital quality.
Each is explored in turn in this section.
268 -is+ BALLARD-REISCH, ZAGUIDOULLINE, WEIGEL

Approaches to Marriage

In advancing a conceptual framework for understanding the differences in


perceptions regarding marriage in the U.S. and Russia, Annoy et al. (1999)
identified two fundamental approaches to marriage, the egalitarian approach
that they see as more common in U.S. culture and the symbiotic approach
which is characteristic of cultural tradition in Russia. The egalitarian approach
is grounded in the assumption that partners can love one another and be com-
mitted to one another while maintaining their own individuality. Egalitarian
relationships are based on emotional reciprocity and interdependence be-
tween equal partners. The symbiotic approach has a vastly different founda-
tion principle. Symbiotic relationships are based on the assumption that
everyone needs to find their perfect match, their other half. Vannoy et al.
(1999) view symbiotic relationships as unions between two dependent per-
sons, each of whom needs the other to become complete. Mothers and
daughters in the Roudakova and Ballard-Reisch (2000) study clearly articu-
lated the widespread belief that a woman is not complete without a husband
and a man in anchorless without a wife. In addition to the basic approaches to
marriage in Russia, researchers have also explored family types.

Family Types
Based on research with couples in St. Petersburg, Golod (1984, 1996)
identified three predominant family types in Russian society: the patriar-
chal, the child-focused, and the spouse-focused. The patriarchal famiZy
contains a dominant male head of household who is the leader of the fam-
ily. In such a family, women obey men and younger members of the family
show deference to their elders. Roles and communication patterns are
highly traditional with the father seen as provider for and protector of the
family and the mother playing the role of supporter and nurturer. Accord-
ing to Golod (1984, 1996), the patriarchal family type was the dominant
form in Russia prior to World War II.
The second type of family emerged following World War II and was the
dominant form until approximately 1990 and the end of perestroika-the
child-focused family. This type of family placed high value on the well being of
children. Spousal happiness was a secondary consideration. Roles and com-
munication patterns were still highly traditional and the good of the children
was the overarching concern. During this time, it was not uncommon for cou-
ples to choose to stay together for the sake of their children.
The third family type, the spouse-focused family, has emerged since
perestroika and is more aligned with western values. Roles and communi-
cation patterns are typically more egalitarian than in either of the other two
types and the stability of the marriage depends on the desire of spouses to
remain in the marriage and their perceptions of marital quality (Golod,
1984, 1996). Recent research has found that couples who could be classi-
12. MAINTAINING MARRIAGES IN RUSSIA =+i-

fied into this third type report a high level of cooperation in their relation-
ship and a democratic approach to family tasks (see Arutiunian, 1987)
including more flexible approaches to parental responsibilities (see Gurko,
1997). Going beyond family types, research on Russian families has also
examined developmental stages in Russian marriages.

Developmental Stages in Russian Marriages


Through an analysis of the sociological and demographic factors impact-
ing marital stability, Golod (1984) identified a dynamic three stage process
in the evolution of Russian marriages similar to findings in U.S. marriages
(see Sillars & Wilmot, 1989). In the early years of marriage, adaptation is re-
quired as couples develop rules for their relationships and learn how to live
together. In the middle stages, feelings of connection and a sense of compan-
ionship are important and finally, in the later stages of marriage, autonomy
on the part of both partners is a common characteristic. Golod (1984) ex-
plained this third phase by concluding that at a certain point, couples ex-
haust the resources of their marriage. They have nothing left to talk about,
and although couples often remain married, they seek external relationships
through which to meet their needs.
Gozman and Aleshin (1987) f ound a pattern with slight differences
from the Golod model. They concluded that as Russian marriages develop,
husbands and wives move farther apart. Consistent with research on U. S .
families (Cole, Williams & Cole, 1999), the main period of divergence,
when couples reported the lowest levels of marital satisfaction, occurred
during childrearing years when children from infancy through the teen
years lived in the home. This finding was also supported by Vannoy et al.
(1999), who concluded that couples with young children through teenag-
ers are less satisfied with their marriages than couples without children in
the home. Consistent with the findings of Golod (1984), once children
reach adolescence, husbands and wives often continue to live together,
even reporting increased satisfaction with their relationships, but they of-
ten live separate lives, basing their satisfaction in marriage not on interac-
tion with one another, but primarily on their own behavior or external
relationships (Gozman & Aleshin, 1987).

Summary

As the previous review indicates, Russian married couples create both


unique communication patterns and relational structures in order to nego-
tiate their marriages in response to societal influences and relational dy-
namics. A small, but growing body of research has started to illuminate the
nature of these structures and the interaction characteristics that lead to
marital quality outcomes.
270 -e+ BALLARD-REISCH, ZAGUIDOULLINE, WEIGEL

The goal of the present review was to analyze the factors that impact the
maintenance of marriages in Russia. The Ballard-Reisch and Weigel(l999)
“Model of Communication Processes in Marital Commitment” was used
as an organizing framework, as it allowed for the assessment of external as
well as internal factors that impact relationship development and mainte-
nance processes within marriages.
At the level of societal influence, historical, political, and economic de-
velopments as well as how they have resulted in complex conceptualiza-
tions of roles for women and men both in families and in society as a whole
were explored. At the couple level, within the marital dyad, the emerging
body of literature that focused on specific communication strategies that
couples use to maintain their relationships was examined. In addition,
structures and forms that couples develop in order to manage these power-
ful external factors and at the same time negotiate and develop their per-
sonal relationships were considered.

CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS

Clearly, as Olson and Matskovsky (1994) concluded, “change character-


izes family life in Soviet and American societies. Whereas the nature of so-
cial change in the United States might best be described as evolutionary,
social changes in the former USSR have been truly revolutionary” (p. 9).
One hallmark of research since perestroika has been the role of interna-
tional collaboration on the development of a serious, although small body of
research on Russian families. Significant long-term collaborations like those
between Maddock, Hogan, Antonov, and Matskovsky (from 1985 to I995)
and Vannoy, Rimashevskaya, Cubbins, Malysheva, Meshterkina, and
Pisklakova (from 1994 to present) have resulted in books published on their
findings in both English and Russian languages. They include Maddock et al.
(1994), Antonov et al. (1995), Vannoy et al. (1999), and Rimashevskaya et
al. (1999). One advantage of such research is the insights gained regarding
marriages in each culture through the taking of new perspectives and the il-
lumination of previously unconsidered lines of research.
However, several potential limitations emerge from this approach. The
first limitation is a concern regarding cultural congruence. Since perestroika,
little theory concerning Russian marriages has been developed. Russian psy-
chologists and sociologists have largely focused on the translation of research
instruments developed in other, particularly U.S ., cultures and their utiliza-
tion within the Russian population. Although this is a place to begin inquiry in
that it allows for the comparison of results between U.S. and Russian popula-
tions, it is necessary to undertake the process of designing research instru-
ments grounded in Russian culture and theories. Given the unique historical
and cultural developments in Russia (Soviet Union) in the course of the 20th
century, the likelihood is very small that models and research instruments de-
12. MAINTAINING MARRIAGES IN RUSSIA ++- 271

veloped in the United States, with its vastly different historical background,
will allow researchers to understand the dynamics of Russian marriages.
It is likely that the theory and conceptualizations on which these instru-
ments are based do not correspond adequately with the realities of Russian
marriages. There are certainly indications of conceptual and content dif-
ferences, like the egalitarian versus symbiotic conceptualization of mate
selection, that make the issue of cultural congruence a significant concern.
A related issue is conceptual congruence including linguistic congru-
ence. For example, there is no word for the concept of commitment as ap-
plied to relationships in the Russian ‘language. There are related concepts
like obligation (06X3aTeJIbCTBO), loyalty (npeAaHHocTb), and faithfulness
(BepHocTb), but the overarching concept does not exist. Similarly, the con-
gruence between the meaning of maintaining relationships in Russia versus
the United States has not been adequately assessed. How does one main-
tain a relationship with other half? Is such maintenance necessary?
How is it perceived?
To really understand marriages in Russia, social scientists must take a
grounded approach to inquiry and try to uncover the culture specific con-
cepts underlying relational stability. This will require both the generation
of new theory and the development of culturally appropriate measure-
ment instruments. Russian social scientists, often in collaboration with
U.S. colleagues, are beginning this process.
Further, this literature review has illuminated a variety of avenues for
future research. Although additional research in the three ecological
spheres identified in the Ballard-Reisch and Weigel (1999) model would
add significantly to an understanding of the context within which Russian
marriages exist as well as the dynamic factors both within and outside mar-
riages that impact their resilience, research in the immediate social sphere
is most strongly lacking. In Russia, where webs of relationship are funda-
mental to survival on many levels and where there is significant evidence
that social support and family and friendship networks are of central im-
portance (Ballard-Reisch et al., 1999), there is a clear need to assess the im-
pact of these networks on the resiliency of marriages.
Additionally, research on marriages in Russia has been focused primarily
on self report measures of behavior. As with studies on U.S. couples, it
would be useful to identify and assess the actual use of behaviors married
couples engage in and their impact on relationships.
Finally, it is unclear at this time what beliefs, attitudes, values, and ex-
pectations Russians have regarding marriage and how they effect
relationships. As Russia shifts from a strongly collectivist culture to one
that also acknowledges individual level variables, these questions will be-
come increasingly salient. Investigation of these issues, in addition to the
development of culturally appropriate questionnaires, would aid in an un-
derstanding of the dynamics of marriage in Russia.
-/
772 +=+ BALLARD-REISCH, ZAGUIDOULLINE, WEIGEL

Adams, J. M., &Jones, W. H. (1999). Interpersonal commitment in historical perspec-


tive. In W. H. Jones &J. M. Adams (Eds.), Commitment in closepersonal relution-
ships: Theory and research (pp. 3-33). N ew York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Antonov, A. I., Matskovsky, M., Maddock, G., & Hogan, M. (Eds.). (1995). na
poroge tysiacheletiia [Family on the threshold of the third millenium] . Mos-
cow, Russia: Institute of Sociology, Russian Academy of Sciences.
Arutiunian, M. Y. (1987). Distribution of obligations in the family and relationships be-
tween spouses. In M. S. Matskovsky (Ed.), Family and social structure (pp. 63-70).
Moscow, Soviet Union: Institute of Sociology, Soviet Academy of Sciences.
Arutiunian, M. Y., & Zdravomyslova, 0. M. (1994). A double identity problem: Partic-
ulars of the traditional conflict between family and work. In 0. M. Zdravomyslova &
M. Y. Arutiunian (Eds.), Demography and sociology: Vol I I. Personality and family
in the period of social transition (pp. 71-85). Moscow, Russia: Institute of
Socio-economic Problems of Population.
Attwood, L. (1990). Th e new Soviet man and woman: Sex role socialization in the USSR.
Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
Ballard-Reisch, D. S., & Weigel, D. J. -(1999). C ommunication processes in marital
commitment: An integrative approach. In J. M. Adams &W H. Jones (Eds.), Hund-
book of interpersonal commitment and relationship stability (pp. 407-424). New
York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Ballard-Reisch, D. S., Weigel, D. J., & Zaguidoulline, M. [1999). Relational mainte-
nance behaviors, marital satisfaction, and commitment in Tatar, Russian, and mixed
Russian/Tatar marriages: An exploratory analysis. Journal of Family Issues, 20(S),
677-697.
Bern, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155-l 62.
Berk, S. F. (1985). The gender factory: The apportionment of work in American house-
holds. New York: Plenum Press.
Boss, P G., & Gurko, R. A. (1994). Th e relationships of men and women in marriage.
In J. W. Maddock, M. J. Hogan, A. I. Antonov, &M. S. Matskovsky (Eds.), Families
before and after perestroika (pp. 36-75). New York: The Guilford Press.
Boutenko, I. A., & Razlogov, K. E. (Eds.). (1997). Recent social trends in Russia
1960-l 995. Buffalo, NY University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development:
Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 723-742.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1992). Relational maintenance strategies and equity in
marriage. Communication Monographs, 59, 243-267.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1994). Maintaining relationships through strategic and
routine interaction. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relu-
tional maintenance (pp. 3-21). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Cole, C. L., Williams, G. D., & Cole, A. L. (1999). The stability of stability in marital
systems. Paper presented at the annual conference of the National Council on Fam-
ily Relations, November, Irvine, CA.
Coltrane, S. (1989). H ousehold labor and the routine production of gender. Social
Problems, 36, 473-490.
Connor, W. D. (2000). N ew world of work: Employment, unemployment, and adapta-
tion. In M. G. Field &J. L. Twigg (Eds.), torn safety nets: Health and social
welfare during the transition (pp. 191-212). New York: St. Press.
12. MAINTAINING MARRIAGES IN RUSSIA ++- 273

Dainton, M., & Stafford, L. (1993). Routine maintenance behaviors: A comparison of


relationship type, partner similarity, and sex differences. Journal of Social and Per-
sonal Relationships, 10, 25 5-2 7 1.
Dainton, M., Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1994). Maintenance strategies and physical
affection as predictors of love, liking, and satisfaction in marriage. Communication
Reports, 7, 89-98.
Duck, S. (1994). Steady as (s)he goes: Relational maintenance as a shared meaning sys-
tem. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relationaL mainte-
nance (pp. 45-61). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Gal, S., & Kligman, G. (2000). The politics of gender after socialism: A comparu-
tive-historical essay. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Golod, S. I. (1984). Sotsiologicheskii i demogruficheskii aspekty
[Family stability: Sociological and demographic aspects]. Leningrad, Soviet Union:
Nauka.
Golod, S. I. (1996). Contemporary family: A plurality of models. Sotsiologicheskiie
Issledovaniia, N3/4, 99-108.
Gottman, J. M., & Carrere, S. (1994). Why men and women get along? Develop-
mental roots and marital inequities. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communi-
cation and relational maintenance (pp. 203-229). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Gozman, L. Y., & Aleshin, Y. A. (1987). Contacts and development of spousal relations.
In G. M. Andreeva & J. Janousek (Eds.), Communication and optimization in joint
activities (pp 30-42). Moscow, Soviet Union: Moscow State University.
Gurko, T A. (1983). E mer g ence of a young family in a large city: Conditiojns and fac-
tors of stability. Moscow, Soviet Union: Institute of Sociology, Soviet Academy of
Sciences.
Gurko, T. A. (1984). Formation of a youngfamily in a big city: Conditions andfactors of
stability. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Institute of Sociology, Soviet Academy
of Sciences, Moscow, Soviet Union.
Gurko, T. A. (1997). v izmenyayuschikhsya usloviyakh
[Parenthood in changing sociocultural conditions]. Sotsiologicheskiie Issledovaniia,
Nl, 72-79.
Hansson, C., & Liden, K. (1983). Moscow women: Thirteen interviews. (G. Bothmer,
G. Blecher, & L. Blecher, Trans.). New York: Pantheon Books.
Holman, T. B., & Brock, G. W. (1986). Implications for therapy in the study of commu-
nication and marital quality. Family Perspective, 20, 85-94.
International Monetary Fund. (2000). World economic outlook, October 2000: Focus on
transitional economies. Washington, DC: International Monetary Fund.
Johnson, M. I? (1991). Commitment to personal relationships. In W. H. Jones & D. W.
Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships, Vol. 3 (pp. 117-143). London:
Jessica Kingsley.
Lapidus, G. (1983). Introduction. In C. Hansson & K. Liden, Moscow women: Thirteen
interviews (G. Bothmer, G. Blecher, & L. Blecher, Trans.) (pp. ix-xvii). New York:
Pantheon Books.
Lenin, I? I. (1966). The emancipation of women: From the writings of V 1. Lenin. New
York: International Publishers.
Maddock, J. W., Hogan, M. J., Antonov, A. I., & Matskovsky, M. S. (1994). FumiZies be-
fore and after perestroika. New York: The Guilford Press.
Malysheva, M. (1992). S ome thoughts on the Soviet Family. In J. Riordan (Ed.), Soviet
social reality in the mirror of glasnost (pp. 3-14). New York: St. Press.
Mamonova, T. (1994). glasnost vs. naglost: Stopping Russian backlash.
Westport, CT Bergin & Garvey.
2,?‘+ -c=+ BALLARD-REISCH, ZAGUIDOULLINE, WEIGEL

Matthews, M. (1986). The poverty in the Soviet Union: The life-styles of the underprivi-
leged in recent years. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Matthews, M. (1989). Patterns of deprivation in the Soviet Union under Brezhnev and
Gorbachev. Stanford, CA: Hoover Institution Press.
Morton, H. W (1987). Housing quality and housing classes in the Soviet Union. In H.
Hertlemann (Ed.), Quality of lif e in the Soviet Union (pp. 95-115). Boulder, CO:
Westview Press.
Morvant, I? (1995). B earing the “double burden” in Russia. Transition 9(8), 4-9.
Noller, I?, & Fitzpatrick, M. A. (1990). Marital communication in the eighties. Journal
of Marriage and the Family, 52, 832-843.
Olson, D. H., & Matskovsky, M. S. (1994). Soviet and American families: A compara-
tive overview. In J. W Maddock, M. J. Hogan, A. I. Antonov, & M. S. Matskovsky
(Eds.), Families before and after perestroika (pp. 9-35). New York: The Guilford
Press.
Ragsdale, J. D. (1996). G en d er, satisfaction level, and the use of relational maintenance
strategies in marriage. Communication Monographs, 63, 354-369.
Rimashevskaya, N. (1992). P erestroika and the status of women in the Soviet Union. In
S. Rai, H. Pilkington, &A. Phizacklea (Eds.), Women in the face of change: The Soviet
Union, Eastern Europe, and China (pp. 11-19). New York: Routledge.
Rimashevskaya, N., Vannoy, D., Malysheva, M., Meshterkina, E., Pisklakova, M., &
Cubbins, L. (1999). Okno v russkuiu chastnuiu Supruzheskie pary v 1996
godu [A window into Russian private life: Marital couples in 19961. Moscow, Russia:
Academia.
Robertson, L. R. (Ed.). (2000). R ussia & Eurasia facts &figures annual. Vol. 26, Part I:
CJS and Russia. Gulf Breeze, FL: Academic International Press.
Roudakova, N.V, & Ballard-Reisch, D. S. (2000). Femininity and the double burden:
Dialogues on the socialization of Russian daughters into womanhood. Paper pre-
sented at the annual conference of the National Communication Association Con-
vention, Seattle, WA.
Rusbult, C. E., & Buunk, B. P (1993). C ommitment processes in close relationships: An
interdependence Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, IO,
175-204.
Russian Statistical Yearbook. (1996). Moscow: Logos.
Schapiro, L., & Godson, J. (Eds.). (1984). The Soviet worker: From Lenin to Andropov.
New York: St. Press.
Seeger, M. (1984). Eye-witness to failure. In L. Schapiro &J. Godson (Eds.), The Soviet
worker: From Lenin to Andropov (pp. 77-107). New York: St. Press.
Sillars, A. L., & Wilmot, W W (1989). Marital communication across the life-span. In J.
F. Nussbaum (Ed.), L 2‘f e -sp an communication: Normative issues (pp. 120-I 47).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Silverman, B., &Yanowitch, M. (1997). N ew rich, new poor, new Russia.. Winners and
losers on the Russian road to capitalism. New York: M. E. Sharpe.
Sperling, V (2000). Th e “new” sexism: Images of Russian women during the transition.
In M. G. Field &J. L. Twigg (Eds.), torn safety nets: Health and social wel-
fare during the transition (pp. 173-l 89). New York: St. Press.
Sprecher, S. (1988). I nvestment model, equity, and social support determinants of re-
lationship commitment. Social Psychological Quarterly, 52, 3 18-328.
Stafford, L. (1994). T racing the threads of spider webs. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford
(Eds.), Communication and relational maintenance (pp. 115-l 39). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
12. MAINTAINING MARRIAGES IN RUSSIA +=+ 272

Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1991). Maintenance strategies and romantic relationship
type, gender, and relational characteristics. JournaZ of Social and Personal Relution-
ships, 8, 217-242.
Stafford, L., Dainton, M., & Haas, S. (2000). M easuring routine and strategic relational
maintenance: Scale revision, sex versus gender roles, and the prediction of relational
characteristics. Communication Monographs, 67(3), 306-323.
Sysenko, V A. (198 1). bruku: Problemy, fuktory, usloviia [Marriage sus-
tainability: Problems, factors, and conditions]. Moscow, Soviet Union: Finances &
Statistics Publishing House.
Sysenko, V A. (1983). Supruzheskie konjlikty [Spouse conflicts]. Moscow, Soviet Un-
ion: Finances & Statistics Publishing House.
Teachman, J. D. (2000). Diversity of family structure: Economics and social influences.
In D. H. Demo, K. R. Allen, &M. A. Fine (Eds.), Handbook offumily diversity (pp.
32-58). New York: Oxford.
United Nations. (2000a). Women and men in Europe and North America 2000. New
York: United Nations.
United Nations. (2OOOb). World economic and sociaZ survey 2000: Trends and policies
in the world economy. New York: United Nations.
Vannoy, D., Rimashevskaya, N., Cubbins, L., Malysheva, M., Meshterkina, E., &
Pisklakova, M. (1999). M urriages in Russia: Couples during the economic transition.
Westport, CT Praeger.
Weigel, D. J., & Ballard-Reisch, D. S. (1997). All marriages are not maintained equally:
Marital type, marital quality, and the use of maintenance behaviors. Paper pre-
sented at the annual conference of the International Network on Personal Relation-
ships. Oxford, OH.
Weigel, D. J., & Weigel, R. R. (1993). Gender roles and family functioning: Gender dif-
ferences in farm families. In C. Berryman-Fink, D. S. Ballard-Reisch, & L. H.
Newman (Eds.), Communication and sex-roZe socialization (pp. 83-97). New York:
Garland Publishing.
White, L. K. (1983). Determinants of spatial interaction: Marital structure or marital
happiness. Journal of Marriage and the FumiZy, 45, 5 11-5 19.
Wood, J. T. (1993). Engendered relations: Interaction, caring, power, and responsibility
in intimacy. In S. Duck (Ed.), S ociaZ context and relationships (pp. 26-54).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Wood, J. T. (1995). Relational communication: Continuity and change in personal reZa-
tionships. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
World Bank. (1996). Statistical handbook: States of the former Soviet Union, 2 l(386).
Washington, DC: World Bank.
Zuo, J. (1992). The reciprocal relationship between marital interaction and marital
happiness: A three-wave study. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54,870-878.
This page intentionally left blank
Maintaining ReIationships
in Korea and the United
States: Features OF Korean
dture that Affect
Relational Maintenance
Beliefs and Behavior.

Young-Ok Yum
Kansas State University

Daniel J. Canary
Arizona State University

ecent years have seen numerous studies on the topic of relational


maintenance (e.g., Baxter & Dindia, 1990; Canary & Stafford, 1992;
Dainton & Stafford, 1993; Guerrero, Eloy, & Wabnik, 1993). However, re-
search has been scarce with respect to cross-cultural variations in the as-
sumptions and behavioral patterns related to relational maintenance.
Empirical studies of relational maintenance have mostly been conducted
in the U.S., leading scholars to question their generalizability to other dis-
tinct cultures (e.g., Stafford, 1994). Studies conducted in such areas as in-
terpersonal communication and social, cross-cultural psychology have
unanimously found that expectations and attitudes toward personal rela-
2,7s ++ YUMAND CANARY

tionships diverge considerably from one culture to another (e.g.,


Gudykunst et al., 1996; Kamo, 1993; Levine, Sato, Hashimoto, & Verma,
1995; Lin & Rusbult, 1995).
The present chapter addresses the issue of cross-cultural variability in
relational maintenance behaviors, examining Korean culture in light of ex-
isting research. More specifically, discussed are: (a) propositions impli-
cated by previous research comparing Koreans and European Americans,
(b) cultural characteristics contributing to unique Korean relational main-
tenance style, (c) relevant empirical, yet, counterintuitive findings, (d)
conclusions regarding the unique communication style of Koreans with re-
spect to relational maintenance, and (e) conclusions and implications for
future research.

l=RAMMS Ol= RW=lX~NCiZ RIEGARDING KOREAN


VIERRSUS AMIXJCAN RELATIONSHJPS

Three general propositions reflect our understanding of the communica-


tion styles of Korean relational partners, and they offer separate frames of
reference for understanding possible differences in relational maintenance
patterns in Korea versus other cultures, particularly, U.S., for most
cross-culture research on relational maintenance patterns compared Kore-
ans to Euro-Americans. The first is that Koreans are less likely to engage in
explicit communicative behavior to maintain their relationships as com-
pared to their American counterparts. Studies concerning whether or not
and to what extent Koreans and Americans vary in communicative behav-
iors used to sustain their personal relationships have drawn on the concep-
tual framework validated by social scientific research across disciplines:
individualism and collectivism. Cross-cultural researchers frequently em-
ployed this particular dimension of (1980) cultural value sys-
tems as their theoretical frame of reference to predict or explain apparent
differences. By and large, they found that the individualism-collectivism
distinction consistently confirms their predictions and helps explain the
variation in communication style and behavior in interpersonal interac-
tions (e.g., Chang & Holt, 1991; Ch en, 1995; Gudykunst & Ting-Toomey,
1996; Kim, 1994; Kim et al., 1996; Ting-Toomey, 1991; Wheeler, Reis, &
Bond, 1989; Yum, 1988).
Gao and Gudykunst (1995), f or instance, surveyed romantic couples in
China and U.S. and found significant cultural differences in high-context
attributional confidence (i.e., dyadic perspective taking). Chinese partici-
pants were significantly higher in high-context attributional confidence
than were Americans. Individuals in collectivistic cultures tend to suppress
their feelings, as opposed to their individualistic counterparts, to avoid
hurting feelings (Frymier, Klopf, & Ishii, 1990; Kim, 1994). In a
13. KOREA AND THE U.S. ++I- 2j79

similar vein, Kim and Wilson (1994) noted that, as opposed to Americans,
Koreans are highly collectivistic and tend to view direct requests as the
least effective strategy for accomplishing their goals. Yi and Park (199 1)
stated that Koreans enact a unique communication style that includes indi-
rect expressions of opinions and feelings, and silence. In this manner, Kore-
ans are often implicit or tacit in expressing their affection and do not
expect a partner will exert overt communicative efforts to convey his or
her intent to maintain a relationship. On the other hand, it appears that
Americans anticipate and actively look for signals from a partner that dis-
play relational commitment and other positive qualities. Much of the re-
search on relational maintenance strategies in the U.S. has focused on
explicitly proactive and constructive message behaviors (Guerrero et al.,
1993). Yet, whereas individualism and collectivism provide a general ap-
proach to compare traits and attributes that exist universally across cul-
tures, many cross-cultural scholars maintain that there are other equally or
more important factors that are uniquely found in a certain culture that
may have greater impact on human behavior (i.e., culture-specific ap-
proach; e.g., Triandis, 1995).
The second proposition is that Koreans andAmericans hold different at-
titudes and expectations with respect to personal relationships. For instance,
Koreans (just as Japanese and Chinese) tend to consider free-choice dating
relationships and romantic love as rather “irrelevant and even disastrous to
marriage because they disrupt the tradition of arranged or at least fam-
ily-approved marriage choices” (Simmons, von Kolke, & Shimizu, 1986, p.
328). One of the reasons for this belief is that romantic love frequently en-
tails intimacy and sexual activities that are counter to a long-held virtue of
premarital chastity sought in a marital partner. Likewise, in India and Paki-
stan, a majority of people believe that romantic love and intense emotional
attachment are a threat to the existing family structure and that they
would marry without love (Levine, Sato, Hashimoto, & Verma, 1995).
However, in Western individualistic cultures (e.g., Germany, Sweden, and
France), chastity is less important and may even be seen as a shortcoming
(Buss, 1989).
The expression of intimacy appears to be more common in individualis-
tic (vs. collectivistic) cultures. For instance, Argyle, Henderson, Bond,
Iizuka, and Contarello (1986) reported that intimacy is more strongly re-
lated to marital satisfaction in individualistic cultures than in collectivistic
cultures. Intimacy and commitment were two typical characteristics in the
British romantic relationships, which was not the case for Japanese rela-
tionships. Specific examples of intimacy behaviors in this study were dis-
cussing sex, showing anger, and showing distress. Emotional commitment
was expressed by showing mutual trust, sharing news and schedule, and
displaying faithfulness. On the other hand, collectivists tend to restrain
themselves from expressing (negative) emotions to avoid conflict or losing
280 +- YUMAND CANARY

face, for they have been taught to control emotions and suppress the ex-
pression of spontaneous and honest feelings (Argyle et al., 1986).
Moreover, romantic relationships serve a pragmatic function in Korea
and other collectivistic cultures. Individuals in collectivistic cultures often
seek social status and financial security from their marriage, whereas indi-
viduals in individualistic cultures place more value in love and compatibil-
ity (Kamo, 1993). Th us, dating and marriage are frequently initiated and
sustained by extended-familial interventions in Korea. Korean parents
tend to consider dating and marriage to an “improper” person a disgrace to
the entire family, with improper person meaning person of a lower socio-
economic status, education, or a certain trait. Thus, parents prefer to ar-
range dating and marriage by investigating the socioeconomic status,
family background, occupation, position, and religion of a prospective
partner through an informal or a formal intermediary, for dating is only
seen as preparation for marriage, and marriage is considered an extension
of family network and merging with another family. Accordingly, once a ro-
mantic relationship is formed, it is taken for granted and not seen as special
and valuable, because it is not own conscious and independent choice
and effort that has formed and sustained the relationship. Frequently, it is
the wishes of parents or other authoritative family members that keep
partners together, for they cannot afford to allow anybody to degrade the
the-myon or ol-gool (similar to face in the Western concept) of the entire
family (Lim & Choi, 1996). A s in d ivi d ua 1s care about face, so do families.
Family members often work to save the collective face of family at the ex-
pense of an face. Other important culture-specific features
that may cause variation in relational maintenance communication be-
tween Koreans and Americans are discussed in the next section.
Our third and final proposition is that research has assumed that a lack
of active and constructive communication would induce lower levels of de-
sired relational qualities [e.g., commitment, trust, liking, and control mutu-
ality] for both Koreans and Americans. Canary and Stafford (1994) stated
that commitment, trust, liking, and control mutuality (i.e., agreement
about rightful influence power and decision making) are universal charac-
teristics valued in romantic relationships across cultures. Some studies in-
dicate that communicative endeavors and skills are also viewed important
in non-Western, collectivistic cultures. For instance, Hatfield and Sprecher
(1995) surveyed young college students in U.S ., Russia, and Japan and dis-
covered that younger participants in Japan consider good communication
qualities and skills a critical factor in relationship development. Japanese
participants indicated that good communication qualities are, for example,
understanding, expressiveness and openness, and use of humor. Potential
for success, money, status, and position were relatively unimportant fac-
tors for romantic relationship among these participants. On the other
hand, in a study of Korean husbands and wives, Yi and Park (199 1) found
13. KOREA AND THE U.S. +=a 281

that relational communication is not considered important and not based


on mutual influence or agreement between partners in Korea, which may
be attributed to greater reliance on a negative communication
style than a positive style. A vast majority of participants indicated they
prefer to dominate or avoid communication than to engage in mutual and
bilateral communication for relational problem solving or deci-
sion-making. It is speculated that because Korean romantic partners are
not as likely to use positive communicative strategies as American part-
ners, Koreans experience lower relational quality (e.g., report lower levels
of commitment, trust, liking, etc.) when compared to Americans.
Beyond these three propositions, also found are features and principles
that are specific to the Korean culture and provide a closer analysis of cul-
tural differences. The following section presents this material.

KOREAN f=EATUKRS AND PRINCIPLE


THAT At=I=lECT R~LATlONSH1P.S

Several cultural features and principles appear to affect Korean beliefs and
attitudes toward an intimate relationship and their styles of relational
maintenance communication: eui-ri, jung, noon-chi, and yon. These four
features also generally coincide with the collectivistic disposition of Kore-
ans. Each is briefly reviewed at this point.

h-h-i

Eui-ri refers to attachment and loyalty to his or her important rela-


tional partner(s). Eui-ri originated from the Confucian philosophy that
stresses loyalty and trust in male-male relationships (e.g., between king
and subject, between father and son, between friends; Yum, 1988); how-
ever, the notion of eui-ri also applies to women. For Korean women, eui-ri
has primarily been manifested in the form of marital fidelity. Especially,
until the recent past, Korean widows (not widowers) were expected to dis-
play eui-ri by not getting remarried and keeping sexual fidelity or chastity
in memory of the late husbands. Some widows even had to commit suicide
to prove that they have kept (or wish to keep) eui-ri with their late
spouses, which was considered an admirable act and honored by the soci-
ety. Continued from this tradition, the concept of eui-ri underlies almost
every personal and social relationship in Korea. Eui-ri is rather an
index of loyalty and commitment to a network of relationships and
ties surrounding the individual, suggesting that social network sys-
tem greatly influences the formation and maintenance of a relationship. In
addition, a person with eui-ri is strongly committed to his or her decision
anrl action once made.
282 -is=+ YUMAND CANARY

Eui-ri presumes a long-term, obligatory nature and asymmetrical reci-


procity in interpersonal relationships. In Korea, personal relationships are
typically viewed as complementary or asymmetrically reciprocal, whereas
partners in the United States tend to adhere to short-term and symmetri-
cal, or contractual, reciprocity (Roloff, 1987; Yum, 1987). Eui-ri dictates
that a partner should not calculate what she or he gives and receives imme-
diately in return, for a person is forever indebted to others, who are, in
turn, constrained by other debts (Yum, 1988).
In a romantic relationship, as long as a person believes that his or her
partner maintains eui-ri, then that person has to do the same and continue
the relationship although he or she may no longer feel passionately in love
or happy. Otherwise, that person would be considered as someone without
eui-ri. On the other hand, in the United States, relationships are
formed voluntarily by individuals who remain equal and independent, and,
therefore, commitments and obligations are mutual and reciprocal, and
both partners are expected to give and receive benefits equally (Yum,
1988). Americans strive to obtain fairness in the exchange of maintenance
efforts and relational outcomes (Canary & Stafford, 1992) and, therefore,
tend to expect their partners to engage in activities that unambiguously in-
dicate their intention to continue the relationship. However, Koreans are
likely to take for granted their intention to stay in the relationship
and pay relatively little attention to what their partners contribute to and
what they receive in their ongoing relationships. Because Americans (vs.
Koreans) are more cognizant of contributions to the relationship
and anticipate display of affection and commitment, Americans
are more likely to look actively for use of maintenance efforts and
to recognize them when they occur (Yum & Canary, 1997).

Jung

A second principle affecting interpersonal behavior is iung. Jung


refers to the “state where each unit of and has turned into one uni-
fied unit of (Lee, 1994, p. 92). Jung is a much broader concept than
love, which is “a feeling of affection for or attachment to an object or ob-
jects” (Lim & Choi, 1996) an d more prevalent in intimate relationships
than in non-intimate relationships. According to Lee (1994), Korean inter-
personal relations have traditionally involved the space of jung wherein ex-
change relations cannot exist. In this sense, “we” is the expansion of self
that includes others within oneself and allows him or her to view others
from their perspective and to “initiate non-self-serving behavior volun-
tarily” (Lee, 1994, p. 95). Jung is also an unconscious but voluntary attach-
ment between two partners who have been through a long history of a
relationship that encompasses both bitter feelings and warm, caring feel-
13. KOREA AND THE U.S. 31, 283

ings (Lim & Choi, 1996). Similarly, Choi and Choi (1990) identified four
properties of jung, which are duration, togetherness, warmth, and solidar-
ity. In other words, jung requires a long history of interaction, mutual ex-
periences, deeply rooted feelings, and mutual interdependence. Lim and
Choi (1996) further pointed out that jung-associated contacts do not have
to be rewarding, and jung can be established between rivals who have to
meet each other frequently for an extended period of time. You can fre-
quently hear some people say that they can not completely ignore someone
or break up from the relationship because the “damned jung” with him or
her is too thick and strong to cut off, which signifies both negative and posi-
tive feelings. This kind of jung is what Koreans call “bitterness-based jung,”
of which the origin is bitter, but the end is sweet (Lim & Choi, 1996). Jung
can drag a distressed and unhappy relationship for a long, long time.

Noon-chi
Noon-chi represents a uniquely Korean strategy to ascertain the
attitudes, desires, and moods (Lim & Choi, 1996). It is a tacit,
high-context communication tactic that allows a person to understand in-
direct or unspoken messages. Noon-chi is distinct from the Western con-
cept of “reading between the lines” in that people using noon-chi do not
necessarily want their interaction partner to know what the message con-
veyed by noon-chi means or necessarily assume that others will be able to
decipher its correct meaning. For instance, if a person ingratiates his or her
supervisor without that ingratiation being noticed by the supervisor or oth-
ers, then that individual uses noon-chi effectively. If a person does not have
noon-chi, then he or she will most likely be caught while flattering his or her
supervisor. A person with noon-chi tends to be successful in developing and
maintaining relationships, for he or she is capable in identifying what the
situation demands, what a potential date desires (even from a brief con-
tact), how to encode and decode relational messages, and how to present
himself or herself as desirable. Within a relationship, noon-chi helps to sus-
tain a relationship by suppressing negative feelings and avoiding conflict.
Noon-chi is manifested by a competent as well as avoidant communica-
tion tactic used to protect self-image or relationship. For example, people
will often advise a person anxiously preparing for his or her important job
interview to use noon-chi so as to avoid looking incompetent or embarrass-
ing himself or herself in front of a prospective employer. People who read
situational cues well and react promptly are considered being adept in
noon-chi or having good noon-chi. People often use noon-chi to protect their
face (Lim & Choi, 1996). In particular, powerless people tend to use
noon-chi to protect their face and project a positive image.
Korean women are expected to be experts in using noon-chi to keep sta-
bility and harmony in relationship. Traditionally, women have sometimes
284 w YUMANDCANARY

been blamed for a breakup of marriage, for they are supposed to have used
noon& effectively in the first place to prevent it (i.e., keep their spouses
happy and keep them from drifting away from the marriage). Simply put,
the woman has created marital conflict and dissolution, and, eventually,
termination of marriage because she did not have good (or enough)
noon&. In modern Korea, men or women are both encouraged to have
good noon-& to avoid conflict and confrontation that may cause dissolu-
tion of a relationship. Overall, noon-chi is a requisite for a successful rela-
tionship.

Yon

In addition to the three features already discussed, yon helps explain an-
other facet of the typical Korean communication style, that is, passive and
dismissive relational communication behavior, as opposed to the
Euro-American style that involves more proactive relational communica-
tion behaviors (e.g., Rusbult, Drigotas, & Verette, 1994; Stafford & Ca-
nary, 1991). Yon is a prevalent belief or attitude toward relationship
processes in the Korean culture. Yon is related to the belief that relation-
ships are formed, maintained, and terminated by uncontrollable external
forces, not by conscious efforts (Chang & Holt, 199 1). According to
the yon principle (a.k.a. yuan in Chinese; Chang & Holt, 1991, Goodwin &
Findlay, 1997), relationships are predetermined or sustained by yon, rather
than by active and constructive communicative tactics that partners use. In
other words, a relationship will be maintained only until the relationship
has sufficient yon that ties the partners. Without yon, the relationship will
fall apart no matter what relational maintenance strategy partners enact,
which will naturally discourage individuals from exerting conscious com-
municative efforts to maintain a relationship.
Eui-ri, jung, noon-chi, and yon constitute unique characteristics that
have strategic relevance to communication used within personal relation-
ships in Korea, which help explain a complementary, communal, implicit,
and dismissive Korean communication style that is distinct from a recipro-
cal, exchange-oriented, explicit, and active Euro-American style. These
characteristics also help explain and predict that Koreans are likely to be
indirect and implicit, use hints and avoidant behavior to cope with prob-
lems, whereas Americans tend to engage in overt, positive communication
behaviors in order to continue a relationship by maintaining desired rela-
tional qualities. Although the roles of these cultural features in relational
maintenance, separately or in combination, have not yet been systemati-
cally theorized or empirically tested from either a Korean or Western
(rather, American) perspective, some prior research evidence needs to be
13. KOREA AND THE U.S. =+- 28j

presented to further discuss research and offer theoretical implications of


these features regarding relational maintenance.
It is not clear whether these features are truly culturally specific and
uniquely Korean or are cultural universals that simply vary across cultures.
Although no research has been conducted focusing on the universality of
these constructs, one previous study found some preliminary evidence
that they may indeed be culturally unique for Koreans. Considering Kore-
reliance on and consciousness of external barriers, commitment, and
attachment to preexisting relationships, Yum (2000) speculated that Ko-
reans may feel more invested in their relationship than U.S. Americans.
According to Rusbult, Drigotas, and Verette (1994), investments are “the
resources that become attached to a relationship and would decline in
value or be lost if the relationship were to end” (p. 119). Investments are
both intrinsic and extrinsic to a relationship. Yen, jung, uye-ri, and noon-chi
are intrinsic and intangible, emotional and behavioral investments that
cannot be retrieved once the relationship is terminated. Accordingly, Yum
(2000) hypothesized that Koreans would have more investments than
would Americans; yet, the results indicated no significant differences be-
tween the two cultures.
Using the same sample, Yum (2000) a1so investigated the other two key
variables of the investment model, which are quality of alternatives (i.e.,
the quality of relationships or activities outside the current relationship)
and satisfaction level (see also Gaines and Agnew, chap. 11, this volume).
Although there were no differences regarding quality of alternatives, there
were significant differences in satisfaction level, revealing that Americans
feel considerably more satisfied with their ongoing relationship than do
Koreans. Although Koreans and Americans did not differ in investment
size, Americans used more proactive and constructive maintenance strate-
gies than did Koreans. It is difficult to interpret these findings from an in-
vestment model perspective, because according to the investment model,
greater investment should be associated with greater maintenance effort
(i.e., loyalty should be used, regardless of satisfaction level).
These results tentatively suggest that yon, uye-ri, jung, and noon-chi
may not necessarily be associated with investments. The Western concept
of investment (i.e., putting something into a relationship anticipating re-
wards) may not be as salient for Koreans as for Americans because Koreans
tend to feel a relative lack of control over the fate of a relationship or feel
pressured into relationship maintenance. Also, the relatively low level of
satisfaction for Koreans (vs. Americans) implies that these cultural fea-
tures play an important role in relationship maintenance for Koreans. Re-
maining in a relationship due to “damned” jung (a strong sense of we-ness
mixed with love and hate), eui-ri (moral obligation), yon (destiny), or
noon-chi (sensitivity to situation and partner) is not likely to induce greater
satisfaction. Instead, these features would likely yield complicated and
280 -e=+ YUMAND CANARY

ambivalent feelings. Based on these remotely related findings, it appears


that these cultural features are culturally specific for Koreans and inde-
pendent of Western theorizing; however, to make a claim with a greater
conviction, they need to be investigated specifically in association with
Western relationship maintenance theories.

IEMPIRICAL IWDIZNCIZ RIEGARDING KOREAN USI?


OI= MAINTIZNANCE BIEHAVIORS

km-chconducted
Most research findings point to cross-cultural differences in relational
maintenance communication, although one should note that these findings
are from cross-cultural comparison of culturally universal traits and fea-
tures (e.g., individualism and collectivism) and that no research has yet at-
tempted to link these culture-specific features to relational
communication. Therefore, based on these findings, one can only specu-
late the implications of these features on maintenance. Yum and Canary
(1997) hypothesized that culture influences the perceived use of relational
maintenance strategies and qualities and compared 273 Koreans and 177
Americans involved in a romantic relationship that lasted at least one year
at the time of data collection. Participants were Korean and American uni-
versity students (American sample consisted of approximately 97%
Euro-Americans and no Korean Americans). For Koreans, the question-
naire was translated into Korean using quasi-back translation method to re-
tain accurate and adequate meanings from the original (Banks & Banks,
1991).
Using Canary and (1992) positivity-assurances-openness-
networks-tasks typology and their measure of relational characteristics
(i.e., control mutuality, liking, commitment, and trust; Canary & Stafford,
1994), Yum and Canary (1997) f ound that American participants reported
the use of all five strategies significantly more than did their Korean coun-
terparts. In addition, as predicted, Americans reported significantly higher
amounts of commitment, liking, control mutuality, and trust than did Ko-
reans. Importantly, correlations between maintenance strategies and rela-
tional characteristics were greater for Americans, suggesting that relational
quality for Americans is more directly connected to maintenance efforts.
However, we should note that maintenance strategies had a significant im-
pact on relational qualities for Koreans as well. These findings indicate that
maintenance behaviors directly influence relational characteristics for
both Americans and Koreans, but more so for Americans. In other words,
for Koreans, the use of positive maintenance strategies (and lack thereof)
13. KOREA AND THE U.S. -=a- 287

does not matter to perceptions of relationship as much as it does


for Americans.
Some cross-cultural studies found that relational quality is largely deter-
mined by situational factors in collectivistic cultures. Kamo (1993) found
that for Japanese participants marital satisfaction was largely determined
by rewards from instrumental or socioeconomic aspects (e.g.,
income level), whereas for American participants marital satisfaction was
predicted by rewards from relational interactions (e.g., emotional support
and openness). In a similar vein, external pressure is another ex-
tra-relational factor that appears critical among Koreans for relational
maintenance. According to Yi and Park (199 l), Koreans are inclined to ac-
commodate in ongoing relationships simply because they are conditioned
to seek security and harmony with the extended family and social network.
In addition, Korean partners may not feel that they have much control over
the fate of their relationship (i.e., yon) and not be motivated to put suffi-
cient individual-level effort. Comparatively speaking, Koreans may feel
obligated or preordained to stay in a relationship, whereas Americans may
consciously choose to continue a relationship due to liking or self-induced
commitment.
Another study provides evidence for cultural variation in communica-
tive choices in relational communication between Koreans and Americans.
Yum (2000) f ound that, overall, Americans display constructive communi-
cation behaviors significantly more than do Koreans. Although Yum and
Canary (1997) f ound that culture affected all five maintenance strategies,
Yum (2000) f ound that culture significantly affected only three strategies:
social networks, sharing tasks, and positivity. As before, Americans em-
ployed these strategies significantly more than did Koreans. The slight dis-
crepancy in the two studies is likely due to the fact that Yum included
nonromantic relationships as well as romantic relationships, whereas Yum
and Canary only focused on romantic relationships.
One finding in common in the aforementioned studies, however, is that
cultural variation accounted for social networks and positivity more than
other strategies. We speculate that Americans intentionally involve com-
mon friends and family members to signal love and commitment,
whereas Koreans do so as a matter of routine in their everyday lives out of a
sense of duty and while viewing social networking as largely irrelevant to or
interfering with maintenance endeavors. Possibly, many Korean couples in
(2000) study remain in their relationships only due to jung, yen, and
eui-ri and do not have room to care about the quality of an ongoing relation-
ship in question. In addition, they may be still in a relationship because
their noon-chi tells them that not a good time for breakup. Still, these
points should be further investigated.
In addition to engaging in ongoing, positive communicative activities,
relational maintenance frequently requires problem solving after some-
285 -e= YUMAND CANARY

thing has gone wrong (Dindia & Baxter, 1987) or after one partner has
found the behavior problematic (Rusbult et al., 1991). One study
specifically investigated problem-solving strategies as part of relational
maintenance and found that individuals reportedly prefer to use construc-
tive strategies as compared to destructive strategies (Yum, 2000). Yum
speculated that problem-solving behavior, especially a negative one, in a
personal relationship also comprises a significant portion of relational
maintenance and that individuals who are likely to report displaying con-
structive behavior in problem solving are less likely to report enacting de-
structive behavior.
Using Rusbult and (1983) exit-voice-loyalty-neglect
(EVLN) typology, Yum (2000) measured the extent to which Koreans and
Americans differ in the use of problem-solving behavior in reaction to part-
problematic behavior. Exit and neglect represent negative prob-
lem-solving behavior (i.e., intent to end the relationship) that likely
jeopardize the stability and continuance of an ongoing relationship,
whereas voice and loyalty are constructive problem-solving strategies used
to show commitment and intent to keep the relationship in tact. Yum
found that Koreans reported the use of both exit and neglect significantly
more than did Americans, although both Koreans and Americans, on aver-
age, relied on these destructive maintenance strategies (i.e., exit and ne-
glect) considerably less than they did constructive strategies (i.e., voice
and loyalty). (Koreans and Americans rated their use of destructive behav-
ior lower than 3 on 7-point scales, whereas they rated constructive strate-
gies greater than 5).
These results comport with a prior finding by Yum (2000) that Koreans
and Americans do not differ in their perceived level of investments and
support the reasoning that Koreans rely on negative strategies more than
do Americans because of such cultural features as yon, eui-ri, jung, and
noon-&. Previously, Rusbult, Verette, Whitney, Slovik, and Lipkus
(1991), using only a U.S. sample, found that highly invested individuals
were significantly more likely than less invested people to enact construc-
tive behavior, and they were less likely to use destructive behavior. It ap-
pears that, although Koreans and Americans do not differ in their degrees
of investment, they may vary in the extent to which investment affects
communication (probably, due to the functions of these cultural features).
Similar findings exist in the literature that provides indirect support for
the impact of these unique cultural features on Korean maintenance com-
munication in that Koreans tend to employ negative relational mainte-
nance tactics more than positive ones. Yi and Park (1991) conducted a
self-report study with Korean marital couples regarding couple communi-
cation style and found that Koreans employ negative strategies more so
than positive ones. Koreans reported engaging in domineering, authoritar-
ian, submissive (or accommodating), and dismissive communication
13. KOREA AND THE U.S. -+ 289

styles, instead of a mutually controlling (i.e., fair, rational, and supportive)


styles. Both husbands o an d wives (3 1%) reported engaging in a domi-
neering style most frequently. A domineering style refers to a negative
communication style that reflects disinterest in communication, irratio-
nality, and a lack of accommodation and perspective-taking (i.e.,
self-centeredness). An accommodating style reflects high obedience, sub-
missiveness, inequality, concession, and lack of rationality. Although this
study focused only on Koreans with no comparison to other cultures, it
nevertheless shows how generally prevalent negative communication tac-
tics are in Korean relationships.
If one places this ongoing discussion on the effects of cultural features
on Korean communication style in relational maintenance aside for a mo-
ment, it is interesting to discover that Koreans use destructive strategies
significantly more than do Americans, whether in a passive (i.e., neglect,
submissive, and dismissive strategies) or active (i.e., exit, domineering, and
authoritarian strategies) manner. These findings do not exactly coincide
with the typical research findings concerning the Korean communication
style. Numerous studies have documented that Koreans care about the
face, attend to appropriateness concerns, and use indirect commu-
nication style more than do their American counterparts (e.g., Gudykunst
et al., 1996; Kim et al., 1996). Some explanations are in tall order.

explanations For the Counter-intuitive Findings

One explanation for the aforementioned findings is that, in ongoing stable


relationship contexts, Koreans may not be motivated to put effort in rela-
tional maintenance, generally speaking, due to excessive support and inter-
vention from people outside a relationship and thus feel pressured to
continue the relationship, which people with high noon-chi cannot fail to
perceive and react to. Along the same lines, Koreans have stronger percep-
tual barriers to relationship breakup that have been built through the ac-
quisition of such beliefs as jung, yon, and eui-ri. More use of negative
strategies by Koreans may represent a negative reaction and resistance to
these cultural features and reflect their confusion in a culture that is in a
rapid transition of westernization, industrialization, or modernization
(Goodwin, 1999).
Another possible explanation may be that Koreans as a whole are by and
large reticent communicators who suffer from communication apprehen-
sion (CA), leading their partners to perceive their communicative efforts
as either lacking or poor. This would suggest why Koreans rely less on ex-
plicit and positive communication behaviors as well. According to
McCroskey (1984), high CA can inhibit individuals from becoming a com-
petent and motivated communicator. High CA individuals are very likely
290 -I+=+ YUMAND CANARY

to make poor choices of communicative strategies, to avoid communica-


tion, and, in some cases, to engage in overcommunication (McCroskey,
1984). However, the research suggests otherwise. Koreans (relative to
Americans) are less apprehensive (Klopf, 1984) and more talkative and
dominant (Park, Cambra, & Klopf, 1979). Park et al. further found that
talk tends to be more task centered and less relationship or peo-
ple centered. Therefore, communication apprehension does not ade-
quately explain the discrepancy between Koreans and Americans in
relational maintenance.
Finally, it is possible that an interaction effect obtains between cultural
and individual difference factors that can help explain why Koreans rely
less on maintenance strategies. For example, Yum (1999) compared the as-
sociations between relational maintenance behavior and loneliness for Ko-
reans and Americans and found that lonely individuals in both Korea and
U.S. tend to perceive that their partners use relational maintenance behav-
iors significantly less than do nonlonely individuals, and that loneliness is
inversely related to relational satisfaction. Also, in both Korean and Ameri-
can sample, lonely individuals reported significantly lower levels of con-
structive maintenance strategies than did their nonlonely counterparts.
Interestingly, results showed that Koreans feel significantly greater loneli-
ness than do Americans. In light of the culture-specific features presented
earlier, Koreans feel lonelier in their current relationships than do Ameri-
cans due to their learned helplessness (e.g., eui-ri and jung) and lack of
control (e.g., yon) over their relationship and life. This explanation sug-
gests that in Korean (as well as other) cultures the features and principles
that help cement personal relationships work against need fulfillment in
those relationships. Of course, this explanation must be more carefully ex-
amined and tested.

CONCLUSIONS AND i=UTURlE DIRECTIONS

The present chapter has reviewed relevant propositions about Ko-


rean-American differences, unique cultural characteristics, research on
maintenance, and their possible explanations for communication
patterns in relationship maintenance processes. This review points to sev-
eral conclusions.
Overall, Koreans appear to hold different attitudes and beliefs about re-
lationships and engage in relational maintenance behaviors to a lesser ex-
tent, and perceive relationship experiences differently, as compared to
people in Euro-American and potentially other Western cultures. Cul-
ture-specific features dictate distinct roles, expectations, and behaviors
13. KOREA AND THE U.S. =a 291

with respect to maintaining relationships. Koreans do not always believe


that communication is an integral or expected part of relational mainte-
nance and display communicative strategies as much as Americans do.
Nevertheless, the use (and lack of use) of relational maintenance strategies
appears to affect perceptions of the quality of relationship.
In particular, culturally specific features and principles of eui-ri, jung,
noon-chi, andyon function to guarantee the stability of personal relation-
ships. At the same time, they appear to countermand the need for rela-
tional partners to work at maintaining their relationships. Given the
positive associations between relational maintenance efforts and relational
quality indicators (e.g., commitment, trust, and satisfaction), it appears
that while stability might be achieved, relational quality is less likely to oc-
cur in Korea. Moreover, the findings that Koreans are generally more lonely
than Americans might be explained by the fact that these cultural features
promote an external locus of control or learned helplessness when it comes
to maintaining close involvements. Accordingly, Koreans might feel
trapped in their close relationships out of obligation due to these external
barriers (Attridge, 1994). In brief, whereas Korean relationships (relative
to U.S. relationships) are more stable, they are also less rewarding.
Whereas the unique properties of Korean culture probably affect
communication in their relationships, cross-cultural commonalities also
need to be noted. First, for both Koreans and Americans, communication
appears to be an important factor in keeping a relationship. Koreans, who
on average perceive their partners engage in constructive communication
behavior significantly less, as compared to Americans, feel lower commit-
ment, trust, liking, and mutual influence.
Second, across cultures, individuals in stable (vs. unstable) relationships
enjoy higher quality relationships, regardless of culture. Even after cul-
ture-specific factors have been taken into account, Koreans who happen to
be in a stable relationship overall report greater degrees of positive rela-
tional characteristics (Yum, 2000). Finally, Koreans, like Americans, prefer
to enact positive behavior (e.g., Canary and strategies) than neg-
ative behavior (e.g., Rusbult and destructive strategies). These
similarities not only imply that some cultural features and behaviors are
universal but also suggest that changes in beliefs and attitudes toward rela-
tionship and communication have occurred in the Korean culture in the di-
rection of westernization. According to Goodwin (1999), large-scale social
change can impact the microlevel relationship behavior. Korea has under-
gone a rapid social and political change for the last few decades, what is
called industrialization, modernization, or westernization, which may pro-
vide some new insight to understand previous findings that do not neces-
sarily seem consistent with unique Korean cultural characteristics (e.g.,
jung, yon, eui-ri, and noon-chi) and culturally universal dimensions (e.g.,
707
“/L -e- YUM AND CANARY

individual-collectivism). The following section discusses several implica-


tions of these findings and avenues for future research.

hlpl icationsfor~utureResearch
The present chapter has shown that Koreans hold unique cultural charac-
teristics and views that generally govern their behavior in personal relation-
ships. Accordingly, future research should consider these features and
their effects on maintaining relationships.
First, future research may need to investigate the potential implications
of these culture-specific characteristics in interethnic and interracial rela-
tionships. When both partners (and close kin members) bring in relational
assumptions and expectancies formed through interaction with others in
their cultures of origin, it is likely that the partners will sometimes encoun-
ter profound differences and difficulties. Partners in these relationships
may need to work harder to maintain them. For instance, Dainton (1999)
found that interracial couples were distinct from same-race couples in that
their stories about their own marriage displayed more emotional attach-
ments and maintenance efforts than did those by same-race couples.
Dainton speculated, meanings and experiences are shaped by
cultural norms regarding how to enact a relationship as well as by everyday
interaction with spouse” (p. 147). Although these culture-bound
meaning structures and experiences must cause initial differences in mari-
tal communication, interracial couples in this study were “more interactive
and in tune with each thoughts,” and “more behaviorally flexible,”
and created a unique “subculture” of marriage (p. 163; probably, to make
up for these differences). results indicate a possibility that cul-
turally unique features may influence biracial relational partners to work to
overcome cultural biases, enrich their relational culture, broaden their rep-
ertoire of maintenance strategies, deepen understanding of each other, and
enhance relational quality. Still, additional large-scale research is war-
ranted to understand how interracial partners interact and connect with
each other and transform their relationship to another level.
Second, methodological limitations of previous studies need to be ad-
dressed in future research endeavors. More in-depth, descriptive, and
field-based observation data are imperative to conduct research that cap-
tures cultural nuances and innuendos occurring during interactions. Al-
though previous studies informed and unveiled general traits and features
associated with maintenance communication styles, researchers need to
collect data from real people through their diaries and logs, and their own
voices through face-to-face interviews. (1999) interview
method was adequate for her purpose by gathering in-depth information
that a large-scale survey might have missed. Numerous previous studies
have employed a self-report measure with college students (e.g., Yum,
13. KOREA AND THE U.S. +=a- 293

2000; Yum & Canary, 1997), leaving questions regarding the representa-
tion of general population. Future studies need to address this issue by
varying methods of data collection (e.g., observation and in-depth inter-
views) and data analysis (e.g., content analysis and path analysis).
Third, we should examine how relational maintenance efforts are linked
to loneliness across cultures. Clearly, the use of proactive and positive
maintenance actions and activities promotes higher quality involvements.
And it appears that people who are lonely, especially chronically lonely, en-
gage in fewer communicative efforts to maintain the relationships they
have (Henson, Dybvig, & Canary, 2001; Spitzberg & Canary, 1985; Yum,
1999). What is less clear concerns whether lonely people in high-context
and collectivistic cultures would benefit from efforts to engage explicitly
in relational maintenance strategies.
Finally, and importantly, as Goodwin (1999) suggested, in order to
make sense of cultural differences in relational maintenance, we need to
develop a comprehensive hypothesis that combine etic, or culturally uni-
versal, factors (e.g., individualism-collectivism and power) with such sa-
lient emit, or culturally specific, factors presented in this chapter. For
instance, people universally prefer positive over negative communicative
behaviors, and proactive maintenance strategies positively link to funda-
mental relational characteristics and qualities. At the same time, it appears
that some cultural features discourage the use of such behaviors, ironically
relying on principles presumably followed to keep close relationships in-
tact. It is possible that high-maintenance relationships are universally
problematic, and that people in some cultures have rules or principles that
guide them in the maintenance of such relationships. Regardless, it appears
that both etic and emit factors must be considered in examining the role of
communication in cross-cultural and intercultural relationships.

Argyle, M., Bond, M., Iizuka, Y., & Contarello, A. (1986). Cross-cultural variations in
relationship rules. Znternational Journal of Psychology, 2 1, 287-3 15.
Attridge, M. (1994). B arriers to dissolution of romantic relationships. In D. J. Canary &
L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational maintenance (pp. 141-163). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Banks, A., & Banks, S. P (1991). Unexplored barriers: The role of translation in inter-
personal communication. In S. Ting-Toomey & F. Korzenny (Eds.), Cross-cultural
interpersonal communication (pp. 171-185). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Baxter, L. A., & Dindia, K. (1990). Marital perceptions of maintenance strate-
gies. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 187-209.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypoth-
eses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, l-l 4.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1992). Relationship maintenance strategies and equity in
marriage. Communication Monographs, 59, 243-267.
294 -E==+ YUMANDCANARY

Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1994). Maintaining relationships through strategic and
routine interaction. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and rela-
tional maintenance (pp. 3-22). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Chang, H., &Holt, G. R. (1991). Th e concept of yuun and Chinese interpersonal rela-
tionships. In S. Ting-Toomey & F. Korzenny (Eds.), Cross-cultural interpersonal
communication (pp. 28-57). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Chen, G. (1995). 1 erences in self-disclosure patterns among Americans versus Chi-
nese. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, ZS, 84-91. -
Choi, S. C., & Choi, S. C. (1990). Psychological structure of jung (cheong). Paper pre-
sented at the annual meeting of the Korean Psychological Association; Pusan, Korea.
Dainton, M. (1999). Af rican:American, European-American, and biracial
meanings fir and-experiences in marriage. In-T. J. Socha & R. C. Diggs (Eds.), Com-
munication, race, and family: Exploring communication in Black, White, and biru-
ciul families (pp. 147-l 65). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Dainton, M., & Stafford, L. (1993). Routine maintenance behaviors: A comparison of
relationship type, partner similarity, and sex differences. JournuZ of Social and Per-
sonal Relationships, 10, 255-272.
Dindia, K., & Baxter, L. A. (1987). Strategies for maintaining and repairing marital rela-
tionships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 4, 143-l 58.
Frymier, A., Klopf, D., & Ishii, S. (1990). Japanese and Americans compared on the af-
fect orientation construct. Psychological Reports, 66, 985-986.
Gao, G., & Gudykunst, W. B. (1995). Attributional confidence, perceived similarity,
and network involvement in Chinese and European American romantic relation-
ships. Communication Quarterly, 43, 431-445.
Goodwin, R. (1999). Personul relationships across cultures. London: Routledge.
Goodwin, R., & Findlay, C. (1997). We were just fated together. Chinese love and the
concept of yuan in Hong Kong and England. Personal Relationships, 4, 85-92.
Gudykunst, W. B., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-Toomey, S., Nishida, T., Kim, K., & Heyman, S.
(1996). The influence of cultural individualism-collectivism, self-construal, and in-
dividual values on communication styles across cultures. Human Communication
Research, 22, 5 1 O-543.
Gudykunst, W B., & Ting-Toomey, S. (1996). Communication in personal relationships
across cultures: An introduction. In W. B. Gudykunst, S. Ting-Toomey, & T Nishida
(Eds.), Communication in personaL relationships across cultures (pp. 3-l 6). Thou-
sand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Guerrero, L. K., Eloy, S. V, & Wabnik, A. I. (1993). in in g maintenance strategies to
relationship development and disengagement: A reconceptualization. Journal of So-
cial and Personal Relationships, 10, 285-304.
Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S. (1995). and mate preferences in the
United States, Russia and Japan. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 26,728-50.
Henson, D. F., Dybvig, K. C., & Canary, D. J. (2001, July). The effects of loneliness and
communication apprehension on relational maintenance behaviors: An attributional
perspective. Paper presented at the International Network on Personal Relation-
ships/International Society for the Study of Personal Relationships Conference;
Prescott, AZ.
Hofstede, G. (1980). consequences. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Kamo, Y. (1993). Determinants of marital satisfaction: A comparison of the United
States and Japan. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 55 l-568.
Kim, M. S. (1994). Cross-cultural comparisons of the perceived importance of conver-
sational constraints. Human Communication Research, 2 1, 128-l 5 1.
13. KOREA AND iHE U.S. =a- 295

Kim, M. S., Hunter, J. E., Miyahara, A., Horvath, A. M., Bresnahan, M., & Yoon, H. J.
(1996). I n d ivi d ua l- vs. culture-level dimensions of individualism and collectivism:
Effects of preferred conversational styles. Communication Monographs, 63,29-49.
Kim, M. S., &Wilson, S. R. (1994). A cross-cultural comparison of implicit theories of
requesting. Communication Monographs, 61, 2 1O-235.
Klopf, D. W. (1984). C ross-cultural apprehension research: A summary of Pacific Basin
studies. In J. A. Daly & J. C. McCroskey (Eds.), Avoiding communication: Shyness,
reticence, and communication apprehension (pp. 157-l 69). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Lee, S. W (1994). The Cheong Space: Azone of non-exchange. In G. Yoon & S. C. Choi
(Eds.), Psychology of the Korean people: Collectivism and individualism (pp.
85-99). Seoul: Dong-A Publishing.
Levine, R., Sato, S., Hashimoto, T., & Verma, J. (1995). Love and marriage in eleven
cultures. Journul of Cross-cultural Psychology, 26, 554-571.
Lim, T. S., & Choi, S. H. (1996). Interpersonal relationships in Korea. In W B.
Gudykunst, S. Ting-Toomey, & T. Nishida (Eds.), Communication in personal relu-
tionships across cultures (pp. 122-136). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Lin, Y. W, & Rusbult, C. E. (1995). C ommitment to dating relationships and cross-sex
friendships in America and China. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 12,
7-26.
McCroskey (1984). The communication apprehension perspective. In J. A. Daly & J.
C. McCroskey (Eds.), Avoiding communication: Shyness, reticence, and communi-
cation apprehension (pp. 13-38). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Park, M. S., Cambra, R. E., & Klopf, D. W (1979). Ch aracteristics of Korean oral com-
munication patters. Korea Journal, 19, 4-8.
Roloff, M. E. (1987). C ommunication and reciprocity within intimate relationships. In
M. E. Roloff & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Interpersonal processes: New directions in com-
munication research (pp. 1 l-38). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Rusbult, C. E., Drigotas, S. M., & Verette, J. (1994). The investment model: An inter-
dependence analysis of commitment processes and relationship maintenance phe-
nomena. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational
maintenance (pp. 115-l 39). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Rusbult, C. E., Verette, J., Whitney, G. A., Slovik, L. F., & Lipkus, I. (1991). Accommo-
dation processes in close relationships: Theory and preliminary empirical evidence.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 53-78.
Rusbult, C. E., & Zembrodt, I. M. (1983). R es p onses to dissatisfaction in romantic in-
volvements: A multidimensional scaling analysis. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 19, 2 74-293.
Simmons, C. H., von Kolke, A., & Shimizu, H. (1986). Attitudes toward romantic love
among American, German, and Japanese students. The Journal of Social Psychology,
126, 327-336.
Spitzberg, B. H., & Canary, D. J. (1985). L oneliness and relationally competent com-
munication. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 2, 387-402.
Stafford, L. (1994). T racing the threads of spider webs. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford
(Eds.), Communication and relational maintenance (pp. 297-306). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1991). Maintenance strategies and romantic relationship
type, gender and relational characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 8, 2 17-242.
Ting-Toomey, S. (1991). Intimacy expressions in three cultures: France, Japan, and the
United States. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 1.5, 29-46.
Triandis, H. C. (1995). I n d ivi d uu 2ism & coZZectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press,
296 +I++ YUMAND CANARY

Wheeler, L., Reis, H., & Bond, M. (1989). Collectivism-individualism in everyday so-
cial life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 79-86.
Yi, J. S ., & Park, S. Y. (199 1). Husbands and wives communication styles in marital in-
teraction. The Journal of Korea Home Management Association, 29, 175-l 90.
Yum, J. 0. (1987). The practice ofuye-r-i in interpersonal relationships in Korea. In D. L.
Kincaid (Ed.), Communication theory: Eastern and Western perspectives (pp.
87-100). New York: Academic Press.
Yum, J. 0. (1988). Th e impact of Confucianism on interpersonal relationships and
communication patterns in East Asia. Communication Monographs, 5.5, 374-388.
Yum, Y. (1999, November). The effect of loneliness on relationship satisfaction and
self/partnerpro-relationship behavior: A cross-cultural comparison between the U.S.
and Korea. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of National Communication
Association, Chicago.
Yum, Y. (2000). C ross-cultural comparisons of links among relational maintenance be-
haviors, exchange factors, and individual characteristics in close relationships. Un-
published doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA.
Yum, Y., & Canary, D. J. (1997, July). The role of culture in the perception of mainte-
nance behaviors and relational characteristics: A comparison between the U.S. and
South Korea. Paper presented at the International Network on Personal Relation-
ships Conference, Oxford, OH.
This page intentionally left blank
Ie

Marianne Dainton
La Salle University

he present volume focuses on how communication functions to main-


tain relationships, centering on three variations: relational type (e.g., mar-
riage, friendships, etc.), structural constraints (e.g., long-distance
relationships, workplace relationships), and culture (e.g., intercultural re-
lationships, relationships in cultures outside of the U.S.). Identifying and
focusing on these variations is important, as one of the problems with the
corpus of maintenance literature is that research within each area has been
published in isolation, with little effort made to synthesize the insights into
maintenance processes that might hold true regardless of relational type,
structural constraints, or culture. The goal of this chapter is to integrate the
previous chapters in such a way that an overarching framework for under-
standing relational maintenance emerges. In so doing, areas of future re-
search are highlighted.
In reviewing the previous chapters, it became clear that two dimensions
are important in capturing the variations in how maintenance is achieved.
These dimensions center on choiceof maintaining the relationship
(ranging on a continuum from the purely voluntary to the purely involun-

299
300 -c=+ DAINTON

tary) and on the intention&y of maintenance enactment (ranging on a


continuum from the wholly intentional to the wholly unintentional). In ad-
dition, it also became clear to me that maintenance is a function of, and is
influenced by, varying contextual levels. That is, the individual, the rela-
tional system, the larger network, and the culture all impact maintenance
processes. This chapter focuses on each of the dimensions and contextual
levels. In the process, a framework is developed that illustrates what is
known so far, and where research should be headed in communication and
relational maintenance. Then, theoretical implications are discussed.

INTEGRATING INSIGHTS

Dimension 1: choice in Relational Maintenance

The old cliche that you can “pick your friends but you pick your fam-
ily” speaks to a central issue in relational maintenance; how a relationship is
maintained is in large part dependent on the desire to be in that
relationship. Several authors in this book have focused on the extent to
which a relationship is voluntary, and the implications of choice on rela-
tional maintenance. As Hess (chap. 5, this volume) points out, the majority
of studies have focused on the maintenance of voluntary relationships such
as marriages and friendships. Indeed, most of the chapters in the present
volume also focus on voluntary relationships. The notion of choice is rarely
discussed in this research; the desire to be in the relationship is simply as-
sumed. On the other hand, Hess reminds us that unwanted relationships
are also maintained by people on a regular basis. In particular, Hess identi-
fies three reasons people might maintain unwanted relationships: social
ties (e.g., family relationships); work ties; and proxemic ties (e.g., being
neighbors). Clearly, the extent to which a relationship is desired will im-
pact the maintenance of that relationship.
Moreover, relationships might not be purely voluntary or purely invol-
untary, but exist on a continuum of choice. For example, Vogl-Bauer (chap.
2, this volume) discusses family relationships, which are paradoxically
both voluntary and involuntary. That is, one is born into or raised by a fam-
ily, making such familial relations in part involuntary; short of significant
estrangements, you are “stuck” with your family. However, as Vogl-Bauer
indicates, one might also chooseto maintain family relationships. This dis-
tinction points to important definitional issues related to maintenance. If
maintenance is defined as simply keeping a relationship in existence
(Dindia & Canary, 1993), f am&al relationships may be maintained by vir-
tue of the role relationships inherent in family life; maintenance communi-
cation is probably unnecessary. However, if maintenance is defined as
keeping a relationship in a specified state or condition (Dindia & Canary,
AN EPILOGUE =+ 301

1993), then maintenance communication may be necessary to achieve that


condition. Also, in the nether world of neither purely voluntary nor purely
involuntary, Waldron (chap. 8, this volume) discusses work relationships,
some of which might be voluntary (e.g., “blended relationships”) and some
of which might be involuntary (e.g., maintaining a relationship with a diffi-
cult team member). Finally, the same relationship might be voluntary at
some points in time and involuntary at other points in time. For example,
many siblings view their relationship as involuntary when living under the
same roof, but that same relationship is perceived as voluntary at later
points of life.
Interestingly, the previous chapters also force us to challenge some pre-
conceptions regarding the relationships that often are presumed to be
purely voluntary. Yum and Canary (chap. 13, this volume), for example,
suggest that romantic relationships in Korea are typically viewed as an obli-
gation born by cultural values, rather than the dominant U.S. belief in the
voluntary nature of romantic relationships. On the other hand, Gaines and
Agnew (chap. 11, this volume) argue that individuals who date or marry
interculturally may perceive themselves as having a wider array of poten-
tial partners, making their relationships even more voluntary than the typi-
cal romantic relationship.
Beyond the sheer importance of recognizing that not all relationships
are voluntary, the previous chapters illustrate and allude to at least three
ways that the maintenance process itself might be affected by the extent to
which a relationship is perceived as voluntary. First, although social sup-
port is likely an important maintenance behavior for all relationships (see,
e.g., Haas, 1999; Messman, Canary, & Hause, 2000; Stafford, Dainton, &
Haas, 2000), such support may be particularly important for involuntary
relationships, and also is likely to come from different sources. Hess (chap.
5, this volume), for example, indicates that coping behaviors, particularly
those involving social support, are specific communication processes that
assist in maintaining unwanted relationships. More importantly, it may be
that the difference between voluntary and involuntary relationships is not
in the relative importance of social support, but, rather, in who is providing
that support. In voluntary relationships, support comes from both within
the system as well as from outside the system. For example, married part-
ners are expected to support each other, but extended family and friends
are also expected to support the couple; Stafford et al. (2000) delineated
both internal social support (called advice) and external social support
(called shared networks). In the case of involuntary relationships, however,
support most likely comes from people outside the relationship. Such a
distinction regarding the locus of support makes clear the differences in
maintaining voluntary versus involuntary relationships.
A second way that the maintenance process might vary when consider-
ing voluntary versus involuntary relationships occurs in the extent to which
302 -e= DAINTON

maintenance behaviors are constructive versus destructive. The distinc-


tion between constructive and destructive maintenance strategies is not
new; Baxter and Dindia (1990) ask e d married couples to sort maintenance
strategies and found three underlying dimensions-constructive versus
destructive, ambivalence versus satiated use, and proactivity versus passiv-
ity. Much of the maintenance research since that time has focused on
proactive, constructive maintenance (Guerrero, Eloy, & Wabnik, 1993).
This focus is not surprising, as most research has also focused on voluntary
relationships. That is not to say that destructive and antisocial acts do not
contribute to relational maintenance in voluntary relationships; the nega-
tive co-occurs with the positive in all relationships (Baxter & Montgomery,
1996; Gottman & Carrere, 1994). Instead, it may be that the less volun-
tary the relationship, the greater the use of antisocial behaviors (Hess,
chap. 5, this volume). Consider, as an example, the maintenance of affec-
tion in a family; sibling rivalry, which typically involves antisocial acts be-
tween brothers and sisters, may be a means for children to maintain what
they perceive as equitable levels of parental affection.
The aforementioned example suggests yet a third way that the mainte-
nance process itself might be affected by the extent to which the relation-
ship is perceived as voluntary; the consciousness of the maintenance
activity. On the one hand, people are probably more conscious of their
maintenance activities in nonvoluntary relationships; maintenance may be
perceived as more work in these unions. One can easily relate to the effort
involved in remaining pleasant to a disliked co-worker. Conversely, people
are probably less conscious of their maintenance activity in nonvoluntary
relationships as compared to voluntary connections. Many nonvoluntary
relationships might be taken for granted, such as the sibling relationship
described earlier. Performing antisocial acts associated with sibling rivalry
probably is not a conscious decision on the part of siblings. Accordingly,
people likely maintain these relationships mindlessly. This brings us to the
second dimension.

Dimension 2: lntentionahy of Maintenance

A second issue that is discussed throughout the book is the intentionality of


maintenance enactment. As Dindia (chap. 1, this volume) discusses, the
extent to which people achieve maintenance strategically (i.e., consciously
and intentionally) versus routinely (e.g., at lower levels of consciousness
and without the intent of relational maintenance) is an area for future re-
search. As with the previous dimension, whether a maintenance behavior
is considered strategic or routine is not a simple either-or issue. Instead,
the consciousness-intentionality of maintenance enactment also is likely
to exist on a continuum.
AN EPILOGUE ++- 30:

Dindia (2000) argued that three possible relationships exist between


strategic and routine maintenance. First, some behaviors might start off
as strategies for relational partners, but over time become routinized.
Tentative support for this assertion can be found in several studies. For
example, Dindia and Baxter (1987) f ound a negative correlation be-
tween the use of maintenance strategies and relational length. Dainton
and Aylor (2002) f ound positive relationships between relational length
and the routine performance of three maintenance strategies (network,
sharing tasks, and conflict management), and a negative relationship be-
tween relational length and the strategic use of openness. In short, there
is some preliminary evidence that suggests that, indeed, over time
maintenance enactment becomes routinized.
Dindia (2000) suggested that a second relationship between routine
and strategic maintenance is that some behaviors might be performed pri-
marily routinely by some partners and primarily routinely by others. Indi-
rect evidence for this was found by Dainton and Aylor (2002). These
authors found moderate to strong, positive correlations among the strate-
gic use of maintenance behaviors, and also moderate to strong, positive
correlations among the routine use of maintenance behaviors. However,
they found virtually no relationship between the routine and strategic use
of maintenance; those relationships that did exist were small and negative.
Finally, Dindia (2000) proposed that the same behavior might on some
occasions be used strategically, and on other occasions be used routinely by
the same relational partner. As tentative evidence for this possibility,
Dainton and Aylor (2002) f ound that individuals in romantic relationships
reported using all of the maintenance behaviors at times routinely and at
times strategically. In short, it appears that all three relationships that
Dindia (2000) p ro p osed between routine and strategic maintenance might
hold true.
Stafford and colleagues (e.g., Dainton & Stafford, 1993; Stafford, chap.
3, this volume; Stafford et al., 2000) have focused a great deal of attention
on the issue of intentionality in relational maintenance. In fact, Stafford
(chap. 3, this volume) argues that the typical term used for nonstrategic
maintenance behavior, routine, is misleading, as it might refer to either
nonstrategic behavior (which is the most common interpretation of the
term) or to mundane daily life (which is the root of the term). Because ter-
minology is an important issue, in this chapter the term intentional refers
to strategic, conscious maintenance efforts, and the term unintentional re-
fers to routine, less conscious maintenance efforts.
Several chapters within this volume have explicitly discussed the
intentionality of maintenance enactment. Waldron, (chap. 8, this vol-
ume) for example, talks about the importance of informal or unplanned
maintenance within the workplace setting. As he points out, however,
research has not yet focused on unintentional maintenance in the orga-
304 M DAINTON

nizational environment. Other authors speculate about how variations


in relational type, structural constraints, and culture might be associ-
ated with intentionality as well. Aylor (chap. 6, this volume) suggests
that the maintenance of long-distance relationships might be more con-
scious, particularly since face-to-face time has to be planned
(Westefeld & Liddell, 1982). S imilarly, Rabby and Walther (chap. 7,
this volume) argue that online relationships do not allow for passive
means of uncertainty reduction, thus more intentional strategies are
likely used in these relationships. They argue that people are more con-
scious of self-presentation on-line than they are in face-to-face interac-
tion. Haas (chap. 5, this volume) indicates that more metarelational
communication occurs in gay and lesbian relationships, which suggests
that maintaining such relationships might be more intentional than
maintaining other romantic relationships. On the other hand, Yum and
Canary (chap. 13, this volume) argue that people entertain low expec-
tations for the conscious enactment of communication strategies in Ko-
rea. Finally, Emmers-Sommers (chap. 9, this volume) suggests that
frequently the process of maintenance is not recognized consciously,
whereas the lack of maintenance is.
In sum, the intentionality of maintenance is much discussed, but infre-
quently studied. In part, this disconnect is likely due to the difficulty of
assessing the degree of consciousness of maintenance enactment. As
Bargh (1984) argued, people are generally unaware of cognitive pro-
cesses, and if people are unaware of their processing, they can not report
it. Nevertheless, a clear consensus exists among the scholars in this vol-
ume that intentionality constitutes a central issue when considering the
role of communication in relational maintenance. Future researchers
should more carefully disentangle how and when maintenance is
achieved intentionally, and how and when it is achieved unintentionally.

Putting it Together: The Foundation

The goal of this chapter is not just to highlight the insights of the previous
chapters in this volume but also to develop a model that might assist future
research. Thus far, the two dimensions that have been identified can be
used as a foundation for that graphical representation. Specifically, if rela-
tionships can be mapped on a continuum from purely voluntary to purely
involuntary, and if the maintenance process can be mapped from the
wholly intentional to the wholly unintentional, then perhaps these two
continua can be placed to capture variations in the maintenance experi-
ence (see Fig. 14.1).
When placing these two continua perpendicular to each other, four
quadrants are created. The first quadrant consists of the intentional
AN EPILOGUE ++i- 305

Voluntary

Quadrant
2

Intentional Unintentional

Involuntary

FIG. 14.1. A foundation for understanding maintenance.

maintenance of voluntary relationships. This quadrant consists of the


bulk of maintenance research published thus far (e.g., Haas, 1999;
Messman et al., 2000; Stafford & Canary, 1991). The second quadrant
consists of the unintentional maintenance of voluntary relationships.
This has not been as widely researched as the previous quadrant (see
Dainton & Aylor, 2002; Dainton & Stafford, 1993; Stafford et al., 2000).
The third quadrant consists of the intentional maintenance of involun-
tary relationships. This quadrant includes much of the workplace mainte-
nance research (see Waldron, chap. 8, this volume), as well as the recent
work on undesired relationships (Hess, chap. 5, this volume). The fourth
and final quadrant includes the unintentional maintenance of involuntary
relationships. Likely to be the least common maintenance situation,
306 +=+ DAINTON

there is very little existing research that could be placed here. At best,
some of the research into family relationships might be placed here (see
Vogl Bauer, chap. 2, this volume).
Although this foundation provides a template for understanding areas of
research, the chapters presented in this volume also illustrate the impor-
tance of yet another consideration for understanding relational mainte-
nance: the importance of context. Unlike the dimensions of choice and
intentionality, contextual variations do not clearly lie on a continuum. In-
stead, contextual levels can be overlaid on the foundation as a whole, such
that each context can be considered within each quadrant created by the
choice and intentionality continua.
It should be no surprise that context is viewed as an important variable
in maintenance. Indeed, this book was in essence designed to capture the
influence of context through a focus on structural, cultural, and contextual
variations. However, in reading each chapter it becomes clear that context
not only differentiates between chapters, but is also a consideration within
chapters. For example, Ballard-Resich, Zaguidoulline, and Weigel (chap.
12, this volume) use an ecological approach to discuss spheres of influence
in maintaining Russian marriages. Specifically, they discuss the couple
sphere, the immediate social sphere, and the societal influences sphere as
contextual influences on marital maintenance in Russia. Each of these lev-
els can be considered contexts of

CONTEXTS i=OR UNDfXSTANDING MAINTENANCE

In trying to capture the contextual variations the authors considered, it be-


came clear to me that four levels are typically evident when studying rela-
tional maintenance. These levels include the self, or psychological and
individual influences on maintenance; the system, which refers to the
maintenance efforts that take place within the relational system by the re-
lational partners; the network, which refers to interaction between the sys-
tem and the larger circle of family, friends, community, etcetera; and the
culture, which refers to historical patterns of ideas, beliefs, rules, and roles
for that relational type. Accordingly, these levels can be overlaid on the
foundation of the model, creating the finished model appearing in Fig.
14.2. The next sections discuss each context.

‘A focus on contexts emerges from numerous chapters in this volume, as well as previous dis-
cussions of maintenance. However, it is important to acknowledge that Ballard-Reisch and
(1999) model, which is based on (1989) ecological systems approach,
provided a language for the framework described herein. My thinking about context has been
greatly influenced by both and Ballard-Reisch and work.
AN EPILOGUE w 30?

Voluntary

Intentional
Intel

Involuntary
FIG. 14.2. The completed model.

The S& Context

Canary and Stafford (1993,1994) suggested that noninteractive processes


complement interactive processes in relational maintenance. These au-
thors looked specifically at how an locus of control affects the
maintenance process. Other scholars have looked at the influence of emo-
tions on maintenance (Emmers Sommers, chap. 9, this volume), how
self-image is related to maintenance (Hess, chap. 5, this volume), and how
identity might affect the maintenance process (Rabby & Walther, chap. 7,
this volume; Waldron, chap. 8, this volume). Of course, the most fre-
quently studied variable in this context is sex and/or gender (e.g., Dainton,
Zelley, & Langan, chap. 4, this volume; Yum & Canary, chap. 13, this vol-
ume; Stafford, chap. 3, this volume; Stafford et al., 2000).
308 -t+e DAINTON

It should be noted that these concentric circles can be understood in


two ways. On the one hand, they can be understood as influences on the
maintenance process. For example, regarding the self context, an individ-
attachment style influences how she or he maintains her or his rela-
tionship (Gaines et al., 2000). On the other hand, one could also consider
the use of this context as the maintenance process itself. To illustrate,
Rusbult, Drigotas, and Verrette (1994) discussed cognitions such as dero-
gation of alternatives as a maintenance activity. Accordingly, the context
represents both influences upon maintenance and a means of maintenance
achievement itself.

The System Context

(chap. 1, this volume) overview of the history of maintenance re-


search makes it clear that the majority of maintenance research published
thus far has been focused on the identification of behaviors used within the
relational system for maintenance purposes, as well as the impact of those
behaviors on the relationship itself. Although other contexts are acknowl-
edged, empirical studies have focused almost exclusively on dynamics in-
ternal to the relationship. This is not meant to be a criticism per se. Using a
Western understanding of personal relationships, relationships require
work, and that work is typically done by the individuals in the relationship.
As Dindia (chap. 1, this volume) discusses, numerous typologies have
been developed to identify the behaviors used within a system for the pur-
pose of relational maintenance. Of these typologies, the one developed by
Stafford and Canary (199 1) is not only the most commonly used typology,
it also has proven to work across contexts. For example, although devel-
oped as a typology of maintenance behaviors used by heterosexual, roman-
tic partners in the United States, Vogl-Bauer, Kalbfleisch, and Beatty
(1999) used Stafford and five strategies to examine parent-child
relational maintenance. Ballard-Reisch, Weigel, and Zaguidoulline (1999)
used the typology to study Russian relational maintenance, and
Yum and Canary (chap. 13, this volume) used the strategies to compare
Korean and U.S. couples in terms of their relational maintenance. Finally,
Haas and Stafford (1998) f ound that the Stafford and Canary strategies
were used by gay and lesbian couples. Clearly, these uses of the Stafford
and Canary typology provide support for the notion that specific commu-
nicative behaviors are used within the system context, and that these same
behaviors are used by different types of relational systems.
Even within a Western perspective, which privileges the systemic con-
text when considering relational maintenance, other contexts are still im-
portant, and are likely to have reciprocal influences with systemic efforts.
For example, a lack of family support might prompt an individual to reas-
AN EPILOGUE =+- 30?

sure his or her partner less (Felmlee, Sprecher, & Bassin, 1990). Or, ten-
sion between team members in an organization can influence the ability of
co-workers who are not directly involved in that tension to maintain their
workplace relationship. In sum, much of previous research has focused on
the systemic context, and some has focused on the relationship between
the self-context and the systemic context, but little has looked at the rela-
tionship between the network context and the systemic context, or the
cultural context and the systemic context.

--i-heNetwork Context
Relationships do not exist in a vacuum, and numerous chapters in this vol-
ume explicitly acknowledge the importance of social networks in relation-
ship maintenance. As Waldron (chap. 8, this volume) notes, any given
relationship is “nested within a complex system of vertical and horizontal
networks.” Although he was talking particularly about workplace relation-
ships, this complex system holds true for other relational forms, as well.
For example, both Haas (chap. 5, this volume) and Gaines and Agnew
(chap. 11, this volume) argue that a lack of societal support for gay and les-
bian relationships and for intercultural relationships makes network sup-
port even more important for couples in these relational types. Even when
there is societal support, however, network support is important. Yum and
Canary (chap. 13, this volume) say that networks are particularly impor-
tant for relational maintenance in Korea, as dating and marriage are typi-
cally initiated by the extended family. Further, network influences such as
the marriage of one friend can impact the ability of friends to maintain
their friendship relationship (see Dainton et al., chap. 4, this volume).
Despite the relative importance of the extended network on the mainte-
nance of relationships, few studies have directly assessed how social networks
inhibit or facilitate relational maintenance. Indeed, although one of Stafford
and (199 1) maintenance strategies is the use of social networks, this
strategy is almost always discussed as something done by an individual or the
dyad, as opposed to something that happens within the network itself. An ex-
ception is the work of Klein and Milardo (2000), who have studied how third
parties influence couple conflict. Future research should continue to explic-
itly focus on the role of the network on relational maintenance.

The CuItut-e Context

The final context is that of the culture itself. Cultures include values and
beliefs about relationships, as well as rules for enacting them (Smith,
1966). Such values have enormous implications for the process of relation-
ship maintenance. Yum and Canary (chap. 13, this volume), for example,
310 -i+=+ DAINTON

found that cultural rules may be more important than systemic mainte-
nance efforts in Korea. Further, Ballard-Reisch et al. (chap. 12, this vol-
ume) discuss the cultural view of marriage in Russia, which suggests that
marriage involves two halves uniting. This cultural belief is vital; as
Ballard-Reisch et al. ask, “How does one maintain a relationship with
other half? Is such maintenance necessary?”
The aforementioned examples come from chapters devoted to an exami-
nation of maintenance in cultures outside of the U.S., and so the role of cul-
ture is more direct and obvious. Because so much of maintenance research
has focused on White, middle-class, romantic relationships in the U.S.,
however, the role of culture might not always be so obvious. Indeed,
Stafford (chap. 3, this volume) questions whether research to date has actu-
ally uncovered effective maintenance strategies, or whether it has simply ex-
posed cultural ideology about relationship enactment. At its core, such a
critique provides not only good fodder for discussion, it also illustrates most
clearly the impact that culture might have on relationship maintenance.

Summary

In reviewing Fig. 14.2, then, what we see is that all four contexts can be
studied within all four quadrants. As previously discussed, the majority of
maintenance research falls within Quadrant 1, with most work falling in
the systemic context (Area B) . The clear conclusion is that there are many
avenues of future research available if we wish to complete the picture.
Moreover, it is clear that we are a long way away from the development of a
macro theory of relationship maintenance. Indeed, one of the criticisms
that can be made of maintenance research in general is that published em-
pirical investigations of relationship maintenance have been largely
atheoretical; those studies that do make use of theory have borrowed theo-
ries that were developed for other communication or relational processes.
A review of the chapters of this book indicates that four primary theories
are used in maintenance research: social exchange theory (including eq-
uity), uncertainty reduction theory, dialectical perspectives, and systems
approaches. The remainder of this chapter considers the implications for
the use of these theories given the model developed thus far.

THlEORtETlCAL IMPLICATIONS

Social Exchange

In short, social exchange approaches suggest that people get into and stay
in relationships based on the rewards and costs of those relationships. As
Canary and Zelley (2000) assert, two social exchange theories are promi-
AN EPILOGUE -+ fill

nent in relational maintenance research: interdependence theory (and par-


ticularly the modification of this theory by Rusbult and colleagues
(Rusbult, Drigotas, Verette, 1994) called the investment model), and eq-
uity theory. Both of these theories view social exchange variables as ante-
cedents to maintenance, and both include a consideration of costs (for a
detailed overview, see Canary & Zelley, 2000). Although these theories
have been used effectively to explain maintenance processes, the research
that has used these theories has focused almost entirely on the systemic
and intentional maintenance of voluntary relationships (i.e., Quadrant 1B
in the framework). The question arises as to how effective social exchange
approaches might be in explaining other areas of the model.
First, regarding the extent to which social exchange explains involuntary
relationships, the results are mixed. On the one hand, Waldron (chap. 8,
this volume) describes research that uses social exchange to explain the
maintenance of workplace relationships. Specifically, leader-member ex-
change approaches argue that because communication resources are lim-
ited, leaders maintain strong workplace relationships with only a select few
employees; leaders maintain such relationships by investing more commu-
nication and allowing greater autonomy to the selected employees (i.e.,
the leader provides more rewards to them). Although workplace relation-
ships are typically conceived as involuntary, in this case, however, it may be
that because the leader selects which members she or he will focus their re-
sources on, such relationships would fall more on the voluntary end of the
continuum.
Perhaps more involuntary are family relationships. It is not clear that so-
cial exchange concepts can fully explain such relationships, however. In
past research, equity theory was only partially successful in explaining the
maintenance of parent-child relationships. Vogl-Bauer et al. (1999) found
that although parents were most satisfied when their relationship with an
adolescent child was equitable, children preferred being overbenefited.
Accordingly, social exchange theories are not consistent in their ability to
explain the maintenance of nonvoluntary relationships.
Social exchange theories also have not been consistently used to explain
both intentional and unintentional maintenance efforts. In part, this is be-
cause social exchange theories are presumed to operate under the assump-
tion that relational partners sustain relationships through conscious acts of
reinforcement, testing, and strategic development (Duck, 1986; Stafford,
chap. 3, this volume). Although much maintenance research using social
exchange has been biased toward a consideration of maintenance strategies
(e.g., Canary & Stafford, 1992, 2001; Messman et al., 2000), social ex-
change theories in and of themselves do not negate the importance of unin-
tentional behavior. In fact, in their theory of interdependence, Thibaut and
Kelley (1959) argued that most of the time people behave in routinized
ways: “only when these routines break down . . . need the individual return
‘712 w DAINTON

to a consideration of consequences . . . at these critical junctures the person


may become deliberate and thoughtful as to consequences” (p. 29). Thus,
social exchange theories do allow for both intentional and unintentional
maintenance. In fact, Canary and Stafford (1994), who used equity the-
ory, argued for both routine and strategic maintenance. However, these
approaches have not frequently been used to explain unintentional behav-
ior. This is likely because social exchange processes themselves are predi-
cated on some level of conscious processing; although individuals in
relationships might maintain their relationships through routines, social
exchange processes are typically invoked at times when these routines are
broken-when an individual feels over- or underbenefited, or when expec-
tations are not met (Hatfield, Traupmann, Sprecher, Utne, & Hay, 1985).
For example, Canary and Stafford (1992) used perceptions of equity to
predict an use of maintenance. Accordingly, although social
exchange approaches do not preclude the use of unintentional mainte-
nance, they are not frequently used to explain such efforts.
Finally, how effective are social exchange approaches for understanding
all four contexts of maintenance? The answer is, not completely. Social ex-
change approaches are inherently concerned with both the self-context (in
terms of an perception of rewards, costs, equity, etc.), as well
as the system context (e.g., in terms of the interdependence of outcomes).
These approaches are less directly associated with the network context, al-
though the network context is considered. For example, Thibaut and
(1959) interdependence theory focuses on two concepts, the
comparison level and the comparison level of alternatives. The comparison
level includes an expectations for the relationship. Although
there are numerous sources for these expectations, they are partly derived
from the social network.
The comparison level of alternatives refers to the extent to which
partner compares favorably to perceived alternatives, including alternative
partners and activities. The degree to which that network pro-
vides alternatives to the current relationship (either directly, e.g., a mother
introducing her daughter to other promising partners, or indirectly, be-
cause the individual has a large network that already includes many poten-
tial partners) determines the stability of the present relationship.
Accordingly, it is clear that although networks are not of primary concern
in social exchange approaches, such approaches at least indirectly consider
the network context.
The final context, the cultural context, presents the greatest challenge
to social exchange approaches. Social exchange approaches presume that
relationships are maintained due to either equality of reward-cost ratios,
or to the equity of such ratios among relational partners. However, neither
equality nor equity are consistently linked to either relational satisfaction
or stability in other cultures (Berman, Murphy-Berman, & Singh, 1985;
AN EPILOGUE +a- 313

Lujansky & Mikula, 1983). A ccordingly, social exchange might be limited


to Western notions of relationships, and as such, of limited value in ex-
plaining the maintenance of relationships in other cultures.

Uncertainty Reduction Theory

Although not frequently identified as a maintenance theory per se, several


chapters in the present volume allude to the importance of uncertainty re-
duction in the maintenance of relationships (e.g., Aylor, chap. 6, this vol-
ume; Emmers-Sommer, chap. 9, this volume; Rabby & Walther, chap. 7,
this volume), and the theory has been used in research to explain mainte-
nance processes (Dainton & Aylor, 2001; Emmers & Canary, 1996). Un-
certainty Reduction Theory (URT) proposes that when individuals
encounter someone or something new, they experience uncertainty; un-
certainty is uncomfortable, so they try to reduce it (Berger & Bradac,
1982). Although uncertainty reduction theory is most often considered a
theory of relational development, the theory is also applicable to estab-
lished relationships (see e.g., Planalp & Honeycutt, 1985; Planalp,
Rutherford, & Honeycutt, 1988). Because social exchange concepts influ-
enced the development of URT (Berger, 1987), this theory has some of the
same strengths and weaknesses. Nevertheless, the theory deserves focused
attention in terms of its ability to account for maintenance as proposed by
the model.
First, as compared to social exchange, URT may be more successful in
explaining the maintenance of involuntary relationships. Indeed, the the-
ory proposes that there are three conditions that affect the motivation to
reduce uncertainty: high incentive value, deviations from expected behav-
ior, and anticipation of future interaction (Berger, 1987). These motiva-
tions are closely aligned with the reasons that Hess (chap. 5, this volume)
proposes for why unwanted relationships are maintained. For example,
Hess argues that external barriers such as social ties, work ties, or proxemic
ties can explain why unwanted relationships are maintained. This is similar
to condition of anticipated future interaction. Moreover, Hess as-
serts that some relationships are unwanted because of negative behaviors
on the part of the disliked partner; typically, negative behaviors are per-
ceived as negative because they deviate from behavioral norms. In short, it
appears that uncertainty reduction theory may be effective in explaining
both voluntary and involuntary relationships.
Similar to social exchange theories, however, URT may be biased to-
ward an explanation of intentional behavior. That is, conceptually uncer-
tainty is presumed to be a generative mechanism causing individuals to
consciously select ways of reducing that uncertainty. Nevertheless, it very
well may be that maintenance behavior is both a cause and an effect of un-
314 -ee DAINTON

certainty; a failure to perform unintentional maintenance might


serve as an uncertainty-inducing event for an individual, which would
cause that individual to perform intentional maintenance to reduce the un-
certainty. Dainton and Aylor (2001) f ound that all five of Stafford and Ca-
(1991) maintenance strategies were negatively correlated with
relational uncertainty, and that the use of assurances was a negative predic-
tor of uncertainty, whereas the use of openness was a positive predictor of
uncertainty in a regression equation. However, Dainton and Aylor did not
determine the causal links between the variables, and they did not study
the intentional versus unintentional performance of maintenance. Accord-
ingly, as with social exchange, the theoretical bias might be toward a focus
on intentional maintenance, but unintentional maintenance is not pre-
cluded.
Regarding the applicability of URT to each of the four contexts, again,
URT is similar to social exchange. As with social exchange, URT functions
best at the self-context, as the central variable of interest is an
certainty. It also can be used at the level of the system; a relational uncer-
tainty approach would suggest that one system behavior is often
the cause of the other system uncertainty. Third, the ties be-
tween the network context and URT are also weak and indirect, as are the
ties between the network context and social exchange. In the case of URT,
two clear links exist. First, one of the primary means of uncertainty reduc-
tion is active strategies, which include gathering information about an un-
certainty inducing event from members of social network.
Conversely, it is possible that members of a network can create
relational uncertainty. Emmers and Canary (1996), for example, found
that third-party involvement was a frequently mentioned uncer-
tainty-inducing event.
Finally, as with social exchange, URT may be a culturally specific ap-
proach to understanding relationships. One of the central means of classi-
fying cultures includes uncertainty avoidance (Hofstede, 1980). As such,
individuals from cultures with a high tolerance for uncertainty are unlikely
to find the experience of uncertainty as a primary motivator for perform-
ing relational maintenance. Accordingly, URT is not a strong contender for
a cross-cultural theoretical understanding.

D&&s

Dialectical perspectives of interpersonal relationships have become in-


creasingly more popular over the past decade. These perspectives are quite
different from the two theories discussed above, if for no other reason than
that dialectical perspectives deny the possibility of maintenance as typi-
cally conceived (Baxter, 1994). Instead, dialectical perspectives assume
that the central characteristic of relationships is change. Accordingly, this
AN EPILOGUE e 315

approach focuses on the management of contradictory tensions, such as


the tension between predictability and novelty, or between autonomy and
connection (Baxter, 1994). From a dialectical perspective, no “steady
state” can be achieved.
Although most of the research using dialectical perspectives has focused
on romantic relationships (Canary & Zelley, 2000) or friendships (Rawlins,
1994), there have been a few studies that indicate that dialectical tensions
are significant in unwanted relationships as well. Cissna, Cox, and Bochner
(1990), for example, investigated the dialectical tensions within a
stepfamily. Bridge and Baxter (1992) studied friends who were also work
associates. It should be noted, however, that neither of these relational
types are entirely undesired. Accordingly, the dialectical tensions experi-
enced between members of purely unwanted relationships are unknown.
Nevertheless, it is likely that tensions do exist in involuntary relationships,
making dialectical perspectives appropriate for this sort of research.
Turning to the intentionality continuum, because most dialectic ap-
proaches are grounded in a humanistic perspective, scholars studying
maintenance from a dialectic perspective tend to make the assumption of
intentional, goal-oriented actors. For example, Rawlins (1994) depicted
“individuals as conscious, active selectors of possible choices from a field
that is partially conceived by them, partially negotiated with others, and
partially determined by social and natural factors outside of their control”
(p. 277). Because of this assumption, such approaches tend to focus more
often on intentional maintenance efforts (e.g., Baxter, 1994). Dindia
(2000), however, argued that dialectical perspectives allow that partners
both routinely and strategically respond to dialectical tensions.
In terms of the four contexts, dialectical perspectives shine. For in-
stance, Baxter (1994) d i ff erentiated between internal and external dialec-
tics. Internal dialectics refer to tensions experienced within the system.
Such tensions might reflect an contradictory desires, which
would represent the self-context but also contradictory desires between
the partners, which would represent the system context. External dialec-
tics, according to Baxter, are tensions experienced between the couple and
the larger social system. External tensions in part reflect the network con-
text. For example, Baxter (1994) described the inclusion-seclusion con-
tradiction, which refers to the tension between the desire to be
integrated with their network and their desire for isolation. However, ex-
ternal dialectics might also reference the cultural context; con-
ventionality-uniqueness contradiction refers to the social pressures to
conform to conventional ways of relating as prescribed by the culture ver-
sus a desire to create a unique relational culture.
Although one of the strengths of a dialectical perspective is its ability
to cross all contexts, this flexibility might also be prove to be problematic
for endorsing such a perspective for understanding all of maintenance.
yc: w DAINTON

The tensions that exist in different relational forms (e.g., friendship,


marriage, workplace relationships) do not always translate to other rela-
tional forms (Canary & Zelley, 2000). Moreover, Goldsmith (1990) ob-
served that dialectics are culturally bound, so even the same relational
type might experience different dialectics in different cultures. As Ca-
nary and Zelley (2000) argued, in and of itself the likelihood that dialecti-
cal tensions do not cross cultural boundaries is not a weakness. Still, at
present dialectical perspectives might better serve as a macrolevel means
for understanding relationships rather than an axiomatic framework for
predicting and explaining relational maintenance. The challenge of fu-
ture research using a dialectical perspective is to develop the theory such
that specific conclusions can be drawn , yet the general applicability of the
. . 1
perspective is not lost.

Systems Approaches
Stafford (1994) proposed that a systems perspective is ideal for the study
of relational maintenance. Indeed, (1983) study relied on systems
ideas in suggesting that maintenance sustains a equilibrium.
Several of the chapters in this anthology have described a systems ap-
proach to understanding varying relational types. Like social exchange and
dialectical perspectives, there is not a single systems theory that is used;
rather, systems approaches comprise a constellation of theories that share
common metatheoretical assumptions and concepts. In short, systems ap-
proaches center on mutual influence between system members, as well as
between subsystems, systems, and suprasystems.
Because systems approaches focus on the interrelations of parts to form
a whole, no distinction is made between voluntary and involuntary associa-
tions. As long as interaction occurs to bind two or more people together, a
system is formed, regardless of any desire-or lack of a de-
sire-for that interaction. As such, systems approaches can be fruitfully
used for all relational types. Indeed, Waldron (chap. 8, this volume) uses
systems concepts when providing an example of the mutual influence of an
unwanted workplace relationship on the larger network in which it is em-
bedded, as well as the influence of that larger network on the unwanted
workplace relationship. Accordingly, systems perspectives can effectively
be used for both ends of the choice continuum.
Systems perspectives also do not differentiate between intentional and
unintentional communication. One of the most common systems theories
used by communication scholars is the pragmatic perspective of the Palo
Alto group (Watzlawick, Bavelas, & Jackson, 1967). Although misquoted
and frequently misunderstood, the Palo Alto first axiom, on the
impossibility of not communicating, directly addresses the notion that not
AN EPILOGUE ++I- 31,:

all communication need be intentionally encoded. Thus, systems perspec-


tives can also be used to understand both intentional and unintentional
maintenance enactment.
Finally, as described previously, systems perspectives are inherently
concerned with the various levels of contexts described by the mainte-
nance model. For example, (1989) ecological approach
discussed the reciprocal influences between the environment and the indi-
vidual as they relate to each other. As such, the self, system, network, and
culture contexts are of primary concern, as are the relations within and be-
tween them.
Despite the implicit usefulness of systems approaches for capturing the
breadth of the maintenance framework, however, there are reasons for
why systems approaches have not been adopted whole-heartedly by those
who study maintenance. First, numerous practical problems arise in trying
to conduct true systems research. Stafford and Dainton (1995), for exam-
ple, identified statistical problems such as unit of analysis and independ-
ence of data concerns, as well as substantive concerns over the importance
of studying systems over time. Most problematic for conducting research
from a systems perspective might be the necessity of avoiding linear expla-
nations and incorporating multiple influences. As Stafford and Dainton
noted, “if everything outside a [family] system has the ability to influence
the system, then everything (e.g., peers, teachers, political systems, etc.)
can be viewed as an intervening variable that must be taken into account”
(p. 18). Because of these challenges, systems theories stand as a useful
metaphor for understanding relational maintenance rather than a specific
theory that can be tested by research (Fitzpatrick & Ritchie, 1992).

CONCLUSIONS

The chapters presented in this volume are instrumental in capturing the


breadth of maintenance research being conducted at the beginning of the
2 1st century. As the model developed in this chapter attests, we are mov-
ing toward a more complete understanding of the means by which relation-
ships are maintained through an expansion of the types of relationships
considered, the variations in how maintenance is achieved, and a consider-
ation of multiple contexts that both impinge upon and contribute to the
sustenance of relationships.
Although we are making progress, the framework utilized in this chap-
ter points to areas where further research is needed; the preponderance of
focus on Quadrant 1B (and to some extent 1A) of the model suggests that
we have much left to learn about relational maintenance. Moreover, a re-
view of the most frequently mentioned theories used by maintenance
scholars indicates that no current theory, at least as presently used and un-
$18 -t+= DAINTON

derstood, can effectively capture the entirety of relational maintenance.


This is not an advocation privileging breadth over depth; numerous sub-
stantive theories have proven to be very effective in explaining the mainte-
nance of some relational types using some of the contexts. Indeed, in
addition to the four theories described previously, several context-specific
theories have been discussed in previous chapters, and have proven to be
very useful. Waldron (199 1), f or example, has developed a theory of inter-
action goals for the organizational setting. Walther (1996) developed a so-
cial information processing theory to explain computer-mediated
communication, and it has significant application to understanding the
maintenance of on-line relationships. Finally, Burleson and Samter (1994)
found support for his social skill similarity model of friendship functioning.
Nevertheless, when trying to understand relational, contextual, and cul-
tural variations in maintaining relationships through communication, our
present theories are limited in their applicability and scope when function-
ing alone. As scholars of interpersonal relationships (and people who ap-
preciate and are frustrated by our own web of interpersonal relationships)
there is much yet to be learned.

REl=lZRlZNClES

Ayres, J. (1983). Strategies to maintain relationships: Their identification and per-


ceived usage. Communication Quarterly, 3 1, 62-67.
Ballard-Reisch, D. S., & Weigel, D. J. (1999). C ommunication processes in marital
commitment: An integrative approach. In J. M. Adams & W. H. Jones (Eds.), Hand-
book of interpersonal commitment and relationship stability (pp. 407-424). New
York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Ballard-Reisch, D. S., Weigel, D. J., & Zaguidoulline, M. G. (1999). Relational mainte-
nance behaviors, marital satisfaction, and commitment in Tatar, Russian, and mixed
Russian-Tatar marriages: An exploratory analysis. Journal of Family Issues, 20,
677-697.
Bargh, J. A. (1984). Automatic and conscious processing of social information. In R. S.
Wyer Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handb oo k of sociaZ cognition (Vol. 3). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Baxter, L. A. (1994). A dialogic approach to relationship maintenance. In D. J. Canary
& L. Stafford (Eds.), C ommunication and Relational Maintenance (pp. 233-254).
New York: Academic Press.
Baxter, L. A., & Dindia, K. (1990). Marital perceptions of marital mainte-
nance strategies. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 187-208.
Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (1996). Relating: Dialogues and dialectics. New
York: Guilford.
Berger, C. R. (1987). C ommunicating under uncertainty. In M. E. Roloff & G. R. Miller
(Eds.), Interpersonal processes: New directions in communication research (pp.
39-62). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Berger, C. R., & Bradac, J. J. (1982). U ncertainty and the nature of interpersonal com-
munication. In C. R. Berger & J. J. Bradac (Eds.), Language and social knowledge:
Uncertainty in interpersonal relationships (pp. l-l 3). London: Edward Arnold.
AN EPILOGUE =+ 319

Berman, J. J., Murphy-Berman, V, & Singh, I? (1985). C ross-cultural similarities and


differences in perceptions of fairness. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 16,
55-67.
Bridge, K., & Baxter, L. A. (1992). Blended f riendships: Friends as work associates.
Western Journal of Communication, 56, 200-225.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989). Ecological systems theory. Annals of Child Development,
6, 187-249.
Burleson, B. R., & Samter, W (1994). A social skills approach to relationship mainte-
nance: How individual differences in communication skills affect the achievement
of relationship functions. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and
relutionul maintenance (pp. 6 l-90). New York: Academic Press.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1992). Relational maintenance strategies and equity in
marriage. Communication Monographs, 59, 243-267.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1993). P reservation of relational characteristics: mainte-
nance strategies, equity, and locus of control. In I? J. Kalbfleisch (Ed.), Znterpersonul
communication: Evolving interpersonal relationships (pp. 237-259). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (1994). Maintaining relationships through strategic and
routine interaction. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relu-
tional maintenance (pp. 3-22). New York: Academic.
Canary, D. J., & Stafford, L. (2001). Equity in the preservation of personal relation-
ships. In J. H. Harvey &A. Wenzel (Eds.), C1 ose romantic relationships: Muinte-
nunce and enhancement (pp. 133-l 51). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Canary, D. J., & Zelley, E. D. (2000). C urrent research programs on relational mainte-
nance behaviors. In M. E. Roloff (Ed.), Communication Yearbook 23 (pp. 305-339).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Cissna, K. N., Cox, D. E., & Bochner, A. I? (1990). The dialectic of marital and parental
relationships within the stepfamily. Communication Monographs, 57, 44-6 1.
Dainton, M., & Aylor, B. (2001). A relational uncertainty analysis of jealousy, trust, and
maintenance in long-distance versus geographically close relationships. Communi-
cation Quarterly, 49, 172-l 88.
Dainton, M., & Aylor, B. (2002). A comparison of routine versus strategic maintenance
efforts. Communication Monographs, 69, 52-66.
Dainton, M., & Stafford, L. (1993). Routine maintenance behaviors: A comparison of
relationship type, partner similarity and sex differences. Journal of Social and Per-
sonal Relationships, 10, 255-27 1.
Dindia, K. (2000). Relational maintenance. In C. Hendrick & S. S. Hendrick (Eds.) ,
Close relationships: A sourcebook (pp. 287-300). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Dindia, K., & Baxter, L. (1987). Strategies for maintaining and repairing marital rela-
tionships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 4, 143-l 58.
Dindia, K., & Canary, D. J. (1993). Definitions and theoretical perspectives on main-
taining relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 163-l 73.
Duck, S. W (1986) H umun Relationships. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Emmers, T. M., & Canary, D. J. (1996). The effect of uncertainty reduction strategies
on young relational repair and intimacy. Communication Quarterly, 44,
166-182.
Felmlee, D., Sprecher, S., & Bassin, E. (1990). Th e d issolution of intimate relation-
ships: A hazard model. Social Psychology Quarterly, 53, 13-30.
Fitzpatrick, M. A., & Ritchie, D. (1992). C ommunication theory. In I? Boss, W.
Doherty, &S. Steinmetz (Eds.), Sourcebook off umily theories. New York: Plenum.
320 ++=+ DAINTON

Gaines, S. O., Jr., Work, C., J o h nson, H., Page Youn, M. S., & Lai, K. (2000). Impact of
attachment style and self-monitoring on responses to accommodative
dilemmas across relationship types. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17,
767-789.
Goldsmith, D. (1990). A dialectic perspective on the expression of authonomy and
connection in romantic relationships. Western Journal of Speech Communicution,
54, 537-556.
Gottman, J. M., & Carrere, S. (1994). Why men and women get along? Develop-
mental roots and marital inequities. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communi-
cution and relational maintenance (pp. 203-229). New York: Academic.
Guerrero, L. K., Eloy, S. V, & Wabnik, A. I. (1993). Linking maintenance strategies to
relationship development and disengagement: A reconceptualization. Journul of So-
ciul and Personal Relationships, 10, 273-283.
Haas, S. M. (1999, May). S ociul support us relational maintenance in guy mule couples
coping with HIV/Aids. Paper presented at the annual conference of the Interna-
tional Communication Association, San Francisco.
Haas, S. M., & Stafford, L. (1998). An initial examination of maintenance behaviors in
gay and lesbian relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15,
846-855.
Hatfield, E., Traupmann, J., Sprecher, S., Utne, M., & Hay, M. (1985). Equity in close
relationships. In W Ickes (Ed.), Compatible and incompatible relationships (pp.
9 l-1 7 1). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Hofstede, G. (1980). consequences. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Klein, R. C. A., &Milardo, R. M. (2000). Th e social context of couple conflict: Support
and criticism from informal third parties. Journal of Social and Personal Relution-
ships, 17, 618-637.
Lujansky, H., & Mikula, G. (1983). C an equity theory explain the quality and stability
of romantic relationships? British Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 101-l 12.
Messman, S. J., Canary, D. J., & Hause, K. S. (2000). Motives to remain platonic, eq-
uity, and the use of maintenance strategies in opposite-sex friendships. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 67-94.
Planalp, S., & Honeycutt, J. M. (1985). E vents that increase uncertainty in personal re-
lationships. Human Communication Research, 11, 593-604.
Planalp, S., Rutherford, D. K., & Honeycutt, J. M. (1988). Events that increase uncer-
tainty in personal relationships II: Replication and extension. Human Communicu-
tion Research, 14, 516-547.
Rawlins, W K. (1994). B eing there and growing apart: Sustaining friendships during
adulthood. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational
maintenance (pp. 275-294). New York: Academic Press.
Rusbult, C. E., Drigotas, S. M., & Verette, J. (1994). The investment model: An inter-
dependence analysis of commitment processes and relationship maintenance phe-
nomena. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and relational
maintenance (pp. 115-l 40). New York: Academic Press.
Smith, A. G. (1966). C ommunicution and culture. New York: Holt, Rinehart, &
Winston.
Stafford, L. (1994). T racing the threads of spider webs. In D. J. Canary & L. Stafford
(Eds.), Communication and relational maintenance (pp. 297-306). New York: Aca-
demic Press.
Stafford L., & Canary, D. J. (199 1). Maintenance strategies and romantic relationship
type, gender, and relational characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relution-
ships, 8, 2 17-242.
AN EPILOGUE ++i- 321

Stafford, L., & Dainton, M. (1995). P arent-child interaction within the family system.
In T J. Socha & G. Stamp (Eds.), Parents, children, and communication: Frontiers of
theory and research (pp. 3-2 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Stafford, L., Dainton, M., & Haas, S. (2000). M easuring routine and strategic relational
maintenance: Scale development, sex versus gender roles, and the prediction of re-
lational characteristics. Communication Monographs, 67, 306-323.
Thibaut, J. W, & Kelley, H. K. (1959). Th e socialpsychology of groups. New York: Wiley.
Vogl-Bauer, S., Kalbfleisch, P J., & Beatty, M. J. (1999). P erceived equity, satisfaction,
and relational maintenance strategies in parent-adolescent dyads. Journal of Youth
and Adolescence, 28, 27-49.
Waldron, V (1991). Achieving communication go& in superior-subordinate relation-
ships: The multi-functionality of upward maintenance tactics. Communication
Monographs, 58‘289-306.
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interper-
sonal, and hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23, 3-43.
Watzlawick, I?, Bavelas, J. B., &Jackson, D. D. (1967). Pragmatics of human communi-
cation: A study of interactional patterns, pathologies, and paradoxes. New York:
Norton.
Westefeld, J. S., & Liddell, D. (1982). Coping with long-distance relationships. Journal
of College Student Personnel, 23, 550-55 1.
This page intentionally left blank
Authorindex

A Attridge, M., xiii, xvi, 8, 26, 109, 110,


121,210,226,291, 293
Acitelli, L. A., 55, 74 Attwood, L., 257, 258, 272
Adams, J. M., 69, 74, 255, 272 Atwater, L., 166, 182
Adams, J. S., 41, 47 Aune, R. K., 187, 198, 199, 202
Adams, l?, 194, 203 Axelrod, R., 114, 121
Aylor, B., 55, 75, 129, 130, 134, 137,
Adams, R. G., 79, 80, 97, 98, 99
Adkins, M., 148, 159 138, 303,305,313, 314, 319
Afifi, W tti292, 93, 98, 188, 189, 193, Ayres, J., 7, 10, 15, 18, 26, 31, 47, 88,
89, 90, 98, 106, 121, 133,
Agnew, C. R., 232, 233, 234, 237, 238, 137, 182, 316,318
239, 242, 246, 247, 249, 250,
251,252,253
Ainsworth, M. D. S., 86, 98 B
Ajzen, I., 238, 249
Albrecht, T A., 165, 177, 182 Bachrach, C., 23 1, 253
Aleshin, Y A., 265, 266, 269, 273 Badzinski, D. M., 31, 32, 33, 48, 52, 76
Allen, M., 24, 27 Bagarozzi, D., 199, 202
Alperson, B. L., 215, 216, 230 Baker, A., 145, 146, 159
Altman, I., 105, 121, 185, 202 Ballard-Reisch, D. S., 36, 49, 62, 63, 65,
Ambert, A., 35,47 67, 69, 71, 74, 77, 256, 257,
Andersen, l? A., 199, 203 258, 261, 262, 264, 265, 266,
Ansell, E., 193, 203 267, 268, 271, 272, 274, 275,
Antonov, A. I., 270, 272, 273 306, 308,318
Argyle, M., 279, 280, 293 Banks, A., 286, 293
Armour, S., 128, 137 Banks, S. l?, 286, 293
Armstrong, J. G., 86, 98 Baptiste, D. A., Jr., 232, 233, 247, 249
Arriaga, X. B., 233, 238, 239, 242, 247, Barajas, L., 246, 250
249, 252,253 Barbee, A. P, 89, 90, 91, 98, 99
Arutiunian, M. Y., 265, 269, 272 Barbour, L. S., 112, 124
Bargh, J. A., 304, 318
524 -c= AUTHORINDEX

Barker, J. B., 175, 182 Boutenko, I. A., 255, 272


Barnes, H., 44, 47 Bowlby, J., 86, 99, 245, 250
Barry, B., 174, 182 Boxer, A. M., 35, 47
Bartholomew, K., 87, 98, 245, 246, 249, Boyd, S., 152, 162
252 Boye, D., 118, 121
Bassin, E., 309, 319 Bradac, J. J., 88, 90, 99, 318
Bavelas,J. B., 316, 321 Bradbury, T. N., 88, 99
Bayer, C. L., 32, 39, 49 Braiker, H. B., 9, 26
Baym, N. K., 145, 159 Braithwaite, D. O., 36, 48
Baxter, L. A., 4, 6, 7, 8, 9, 12, 13, 15, Brandon, D. l?, 141, 159
18, 19, 26, 27, 42, 43, 44, 47, Brashers, D., 148, 159
71, 74, 79, 83, 84, 85, 89, 98, Brennan, K. A., 236, 252
99, 106, 121, 133, 138, 187, Bresnahan, M., 278, 289, 295
189, 193, 195, 198, 199, 202, Bridge, K., 315, 319
209, 219, 220, 226, 227, 277, Brinkman, R., 103, 121
288,293,294,302,303 Brock, G. W, 263, 273
Beatty, M. J., 40, 41, 42, 49, 61, 63, 77, Brommel, B. J., 33, 36, 38, 48
308, 311, 314, 315, 318, 319, Bronfenbrenner, U., 256, 272, 306, 317,
321 319
Beavin, J., 112, 116, 117, 124 Brown, B. B., 44, 48
Beckett, C. S., 197, 204 Bruess, C. J. S., 17, 26
Bedell, B. T, 119, 124 Brunner, H., 149, 160
Bell, A. I?, 211, 213, 227 Buckingham, M., 22, 26
Bell, R. A., 11, 12, 15, 18, 19, 26, 105, Bulcroft, R. A., 44, 48
121, 147, 156, 159 Bullis, C., 193, 202
Bern, S. L., 215, 227, 246, 249, 266, Burgess, E. WI, 52, 74
272 Burgoon, J. K., 66, 75, 117, 121, 147,
Berger, C. R., 54, 74, 80, 98, 313, 318 150, 162
Berger, H. A., 97, 98 Burke, l? L., 46, 49, 90, 91, 92, 99
Berger, R. M., 210, 213, 227 Burleson, B. R., 20, 26, 85, 86, 88, 89,
Berk, S. F., 266, 272 94, 95, 96, 99, 318, 319
Berman, J. J., 312, 319 Buss, D. M., 92, 99, 213, 227, 230, 279,
Bernard-Paolucci, T., 128, 138 293
Berscheid, E., 53, 69, 77, 105, 114, 118, Buswell, B. N., 194, 203
121, 123, 233, 234, 246, 249, Buunk, B. P, 6, 28, 40, 49, 83, 99, 233,
250 239, 240, 243, 244, 245, 252,
Bettinger, M., 213, 228 263, 274
Bettini, L., 93, 97, 101 Byrne, D., 105, 121
Bhushan, R., 32, 33, 35, 36, 47
Bies, R. J., 114, 124
Birchler, G. R., 185, 195, 202
Blasband, D., 214, 227 c
Bledsoe, K. L., 91, 100, 232, 246, 248,
250,251 Cahill, D., 165, 174, 177, 179, 183
Blehar, M. C., 86, 98 Cahn, D. D., 116, 121
Blieszner, R., 79, 80, 97, 98, 99 Calabrese, R. J., 156, 159
Blumstein, I!, 211, 212, 213, 214, 216, Cambra, R. E., 290, 295
227 Cameron, C. A., 46,48
Bochner, A. P., 71, 74, 315, 319 Canary, D. J., xiii, xiv, xvi, 2, 6, 7, 14,
Bogdan, R. J., 107, 121 15, 18, 26, 27, 28, 31, 32, 33,
Bond, J. T., 24, 26 39, 48, 49, 51, 52, 53, 54, 56,
Bond, M., 278,279,280,293,296 57, 60, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66,
Booth-Butterfield, M., 33, 47 67, 68, 69, 70, 71, 72, 73, 75,
Bormann, E. G., 117, 121 76, 77, 79, 81, 82, 83, 88, 89,
Boss, P.G., 263, 264, 266, 272 92, 97, 99, 100, 101, 119,
AUTHORINDEX =st- 325;

123, 129, 130, 131, 133, 137, Cox, D. E., 315, 319
138, 139, 153, 160, 174, 176, Cox, S., 174, 182
182, 187, 188, 193, 197, 198, Cramer, E. M., 243, 250
199, 202, 204, 209, 219, 220, Crohn, J., 233, 235, 250
221, 225, 227, 230, 262, 264, Cubbins, L., 255, 257, 270, 274, 275
273, 275, 277, 280, 282, 284, Culnan, M. J., 142, 160
286, 287, 293, 293, 294, 295, Cunningham, M. R., 89, 90, 98
296, 300, 301, 302, 305, 307, Cupach, W, 131, 137
308, 310, 311, 312, 313, 314, Curtis, I?, 142, 145, 160
315, 316, 319, 320 Cutlip, J., 144, 160
Carballo-Dieguez, A., 212, 213, 230 Cutrona, C., 219, 227
Carbaugh, D., 132, 133, 139
Cardell, M., 215, 229
Carlson, C. J., 45, 46, 48 D
Carpenter, D., 129, 134, 137
Carrere, S., 255, 273, 302, 320 K. J?,148, 162
Carter, B., 32, 34, 36, 37, 48
Cate, R., 135, 138 Dailey, D. M., 212, 215, 227
Dainton, M., 6, 15, 16, 17, 27, 38, 39,
Chalfin, J., 233, 250 42, 49, 54, 55, 57, 60, 61, 63,
Chang, H., 278, 284,294 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 69, 70, 71,
Chen, G., 278, 294 72, 74, 75, 77, 91, 102, 129,
Chester, A., 151, 160 130, 131, 134, 135, 136, 137,
Choi, S. C., 283, 294 138, 209, 219, 220, 221, 222,
Choi, S. H., 280, 282, 295 225, 227, 230, 262, 264, 265,
Christensen, A., 105, 123 267, 273, 275, 277, 292, 294,
Cicirelli, VI G., 37, 48 301, 303, 305, 313, 314, 317,
Cissna, K. N., 315, 3 19 319, 321
Clark, H. H., 117, 122 Daley, J., 128, 139
Clark, M. S., 82, 101 Dallinger, J. D., 61, 76
Clark, R. A., 94, 99 Daly, J. A., 11, 12, 15, 18, 19, 26, 105,
Clark, W M., 209, 227 121,133,137
Clarke, E., 92, 102 Darley, J. M., 117, 122
Clasen, D. R., 44, 48 Davila, J., 88, 99
Clemente, P., 136, 137 Davis, K. E., 87, 101
Clingempeel, W G., 39, 49 Davis, M. S., 4, 8, 12, 13, 27, 187, 195,
Cloven, D. H., 187, 188, 204 202
Coates, D. L., 248, 250 DeGrasse, R., 145, 160
Cochran, S. D., 212, 229 Dellmann-Jenkins, M., 128, 130, 138
Coffman, C., 22, 26 Delongis, A. M., 219, 227
Cole, A. L., 269, 272 Denton, W H., 86, 99
Cole, C. L., 214, 227, 269, 272 DeRidder, R., 116, 122
Cole, T., 88, 90, 99 Derlega, V J., 90, 91, 92, 99, 102
Coleman, M., 104, 122 Detweiler, J. B., 119, 124
Collins, N. L., 86, 87, 99 Deutsch, M., 81, 99
Collins, W A., 35, 48 Dickens, W. J., 105, 122
Coltrane, S., 266, 272 Diggs, R. C., 57, 61, 62, 75
Connor, W. D., 260, 272 Dimitrova, D., 144, 162
Constant, D., 141, 160 Dimmick, J., 136, 143, 155, 156, 161
Contarello, A., 279, 280, 293 Dindia, K., xiv, xvi, 2, 4, 7, 9, 12, 13,
Cooper, P J., 104, 122 15, 18, 19, 21, 24, 26, 27, 28,
Courtright, J. A., 199, 202 31, 32, 39, 48, 79, 88, 99,
Coyne, J. C., 219, 227 100, 130, 133, 138, 186, 187,
Cox, C. E., 233, 234, 238, 240, 250 189, 193, 197, 198, 202, 209,
Cox, C. L., 232, 242, 243, 244, 250,
253
$~o ++ AUTHOR INDEX

219, 227, 277, 289, 293, 294, Ficara, L., 129, 130, 133, 138
300, 302,303,318,319 Findlay, C., 284, 294
Dixon, D., 131, 138 Fine, G. A., 173, 182
Dixon, J. T, 80, 98 Finello, K., 103, 122
Dodds, D., 152, 160 Fineman, S., 174, 182
Dougherty, T., 176, 182 Finn, S. E., 215, 229
Dreher, G., 176, 182 Fischthal, A., 166, 182
Drigotas, S. M., 120, 122, 232, 238, Fishbein, M., 238, 249
239, 240, 241, 242, 244, 249, Fisher, H., 213, 227
250, 252; 253; 284; 285, 295; Fitzpatrick, M. A., 31, 32, 33, 37, 39,
308,311,320 41, 48, 49, 52, 65, 66, 76,
Droege, K., 94, 100 213, 227,263,274,317,319
Dsilva, M., 169, 184 Fleming, J. H., 117, 122
Duck, S. W., 2, 16, 27, 39, 48, 52, 53, Flett, G. L., 119, 122
54, 55, 75, 76, 104, 106, 113, Floyd, K., 141, 142, 145, 146, 161
116, 122, 124, 133, 138, 154, Foa, E. B., 234, 250
160, 239, 250, 262, 273, 311, Foa, U. G., 234,250
319 Foreman, C., 165, 176, 177, 184
Duffy, S. M., 212, 227 Foster, C. A., 237, 242, 246, 253
Dunn, R., 129, 139 France, N., 104, 119, 123
Duong, T., 235, 251 Freeman-Witthoft, B., 97, 98
Dybvig, K. C., 293, 294 Freund, H. A., 97, 98
Friedman, R., 144, 160
Fritz, J. M. H., 106, 108, 117, 122
E Frost, K., 152, 160
Frymier, A., 278, 294
Ebesu Hubbard, A. S., 187, 198, 199, Fuiman, M., 92, 100
202 Fulk, J., 157, 160
Edgar, T., 213, 227 Fung, P, 150, 160
Egland, K. L., 92, 100 Fuqua, D., 90, 91, 92, 99
Elicker, J., 87, 94, 95, 102
Elkins, L. E., 90, 100
Ellemers, N., 34, 49 G
Eloy, S. V, 15, 27, 68, 76, 186, 199,
203, 277,279,294,302,320 Gaertner, L. A., 246, 250
Emmers, T. M., 188, 189, 193, 198, Gaines, S. O., Jr., 91, 100, 231, 232,
199,202,313,314,319 233, 234, 235, 237, 238, 240,
Emmers-Sommer, T. M., 193, 197, 199, 245, 246, 247, 248, 249, 250,
203
251, 308,320
Etcheverry, P E., 238, 250 Gal, S., 257, 258, 259, 273
Evans, K., 193, 203 Galinsky, E., 24, 26
Gallois, C., 34, 49
J= Galvin, K. M., 33, 36, 38, 48, 104, 122
Ganong, L. H., 104, 122
Gao, G., 278, 294
Faraj, S., 145, 161 Garcia, B. F., 232, 246, 248, 250, 251
Farris, K. R., 91, 100, 232, 246, 248, Gardner, R. R., 147, 159
250,251 Garton, L., 144, 162
Faulkner, S. L., 92, 93, 98 Gebhard, P H., 213, 228
Feeney,J., 245, 246, 250 Gerstel, N., 129, 134, 138
Fehr, B., 80, 88, 89, 90, 100 Giddens, A., 166, 181, 182
Felmlee, D., 309, 3 19 Gilbert, D. G., 113, 124
Ferara, K., 149, 160 Gilbertson, J., 21, 24, 27
Festinger, L., 243, 250 Giles, H., 34, 49
AUTHORINDEX +=+I- 327

Gilmartin, B. D., 214, 227 Hamilton, M., 216, 229


Gilstrap, S., 246, 250 Hample, D., 61, 76
Glass, S. I?, 213, 227 Handel, G., 41, 42, 48
Godson, J., 259, 274 Hansson, C., 258, 273
Goffman, E., 210, 227 Harden, J. M., 107, 122
Goldsmith, D., 316, 320 Hardin, D., 246, 250
Golish, T. D., 35, 37, 48 Harrison, T., 181, 183
Golod, S. I., 268, 269, 273 Harry, J., 211, 216, 228
Gonzalez, C., 11, 12, 15, 18, 19, 26, Hartos, J. L., 35, 48
105,121 Harvey, J. H., xiv, xvi, 105, 123
Gonzalez, M., 133, 137 Hashimoto, T., 278, 279, 295
Goodwin, R., 284, 289, 291, 293, 294 Hatfield, E., 53, 76, 280, 294, 312, 320
Gordon, A., 232, 233, 251 Hause, K. S., 15, 26, 57, 60, 63, 64, 67,
Gordon, R. A., 174, 176, 182 75, 76, 79, 82, 88, 89, 92, 99,
Gottman, J. M., xiv, xvi, 2, 6, 27, 72, 101, 130, 133, 137, 153, 160,
76, 94, 100, 112, 118, 122, 301,305; 311,320
198,203,255,273, 302,320 Hause, K. W, 119, 123, 129
Govaerts, K., 131, 138 Hay, M., 53, 76, 312, 320
Gozman, L. Y., 265, 266, 269, 273 Hays, R. B., 80, 88, 89, 90, 100
Graen, G., 166, 167, 176, 181, 182 Haythronite, C., 144, 162
Granrose, C. S., 232, 246, 248, 250, Hazan, C., 245, 251
251 Heider, F., 108, 109, 122
Green, R., 213, 228 Heiman, M., 32, 34, 36, 37, 48
Greene, J. O., 107, 122 Helgeson, V. S., 129, 138, 223, 228
Griffin, E., 44, 48 Henderson, M. C., 91, 100, 246, 250,
Grob, L., 21, 27 279
Grosnick, W A., 212, 229 Henton, J., 135, 138
Gross, H., 129, 134, 138 Henson, D. F., 293, 294
Gudykunst, W. B., 278, 289, 294 Hess, J. A., 22, 27, 104, 106, 109, 110,
Guerrero, L. K., 15, 27, 68, 76, 87, 100, 111, 114, 116, 117, 122
186, 199, 203, 277, 279, 294, Hetherington, E. M., 39, 49
302, 320 Hewitt, P L., 119, 122
Guldner, G., 130, 131, 138 Heyman, S., 278, 289, 294
Gulia, M., 144, 162 Hicks, D., 246, 250
Gulley, M. R., 89, 90, 98 Hiemstra, G., 149, 160
Gunn, C. W., 155, 158, 160 Hilton, J. L., 117, 122
Gunn, D. O., 155, 158, 160 Hindin, M., 231, 253
Gunn, L. K., 241, 252 Ho, M. K., 233, 235, 251
Gurko, R. A., 263, 264, 266, 269, 272 Hofstede, G., 248, 251, 278, 294, 314,
Gurko, T. A., 266, 269, 273 320
Gurung, R. A. R., 235, 251 Hogan, M. J., 270, 272, 273
Gwynne, G., 151, 160 Holahan, C. J., 113, 123
Hollingshead, A. B., 141, 159
Holman, T. B., 263, 273
H Holt, G. R., 278, 284, 294
Holt, I!, 131, 132, 138
Homans, G. C., 219, 228
Haas, S. M., 54, 55, 57, 60, 61, 63, 66, Honeycutt, J. M., 193, 204, 313, 320
67, 70, 71, 74, 76, 77, 91, Horowitz, L. M., 87, 98, 245, 249
102, 209, 210, 219, 220, 221, Horvath, A. M., 278, 289, 295
223, 225, 228, 230, 266, 267, Howes, C., 94, 100
275, 301, 303, 305, 308, 320, Hughes, D. K., 239, 242, 252
321 Hunt, M. H., 165, 169, 184
Hale, J. L., 66, 75, 117, 121 Hunter, C. H., 199, 203
Hall, B., 165, 177, 182 Hunter, J. E., 278, 289, 295
528 +==+ AUTHORINDEX

Hupka, R. B., 199, 203 Kelley, H. H., 4, 6, 9, 26, 28, 63, 76,
Hurst, M. H., 55, 76 105, 109, 123, 124, 219, 230,
Huston, M., 209, 210, 216, 228 232, 233,234,251,252
Huston, T. L., 96, 101, 105, 123 Keltner, D., 194, 203
Kerns, K. A., 87, 100
Kiesler, S., 141, 160
Kilmer, H., 131, 135, 136, 138
Kim, K., 233, 278, 289,294
Kim, M. S., 246, 250, 278, 279, 289,
Ickes, WI, 231, 237, 247, 249, 250 294,295
Iizuka, Y., 279, 280, 293 Kinney, T. A., 106, 112, 123
Infante, D., 116, 123 Kinsey, A. C., 213, 228
Isabella, L. A., 173, 177, 180, 183 Kirschner, R., 103, 121
Ishii, S., 278, 294 Klein, D. M., 39, 40, 41, 48
Iwaniszek, J., 24.5, 246, 252 Klein, R. C. A., 309, 320
Kligman, G., 257, 258, 259, 273
Kline, S. L., 136, 143, 155, 156, 161
J KIopf, D. W., 278, 290, 294, 295
Knapp, J. J., 214, 228
Knapp, M. L., 3, 19, 27
Jablin, F. M., 22, 23, 27, 165, 167, 173, Knox, D., 128, 129, 134, 137, 138
175, 177, 183 Knudson, K., 152, 160
Jackson, D. D., 112, 116, 117, 124, Kojetin, B. A., 117, 122
316, 321 Kozac, E. B., 212, 229
Jacobson,D., 146, 160 Kram, K. E., 173, 177, 180, 183
Jacquet, S. E., 239, 252 Kramer, M. WI, 177, 179, 183
Jandt, F. E., 213, 227 Krippendorff, K., 112, 123
Jay, K., 211, 228 Kugler, K., 194, 203
Jenkins, E., 179, 183 Kurdek, L. A., 211, 212, 213, 214, 215,
Johansen,R., 145, 160 216, 217, 226, 228,229,230
Johnson, A. J., 88, 89, 90, 96, 100 Kurland, G. J., 91, 100, 232, 245, 250
Johnson, D. J., 197, 204, 242, 243, 245,
246, 251, 252
Johnson, H., 251, 308, 320
Johnson, M. l?, xiii, xvi, 96, 101, 114,
116, 123,239,251,257, 273
Jones, D. C., 88, 90, 92, 100 Lai, K., 251, 308, 320
Jones,W. H., 193, 194, 203, 255, 272 Land, J., 210, 229
Jorgensen,l? F., 199, 203 Lane, M., 33,48
Jung, J., 199, 203 Langan, E., 21, 27
Langston, C. A., 233, 249
Lapidus, G., 258, 273
K Lara, J. K., 91, 100
Larson, J. R., 168, 183
Le, B., 234, 251
Kacmar, K. M., 174, 176, 184 Lea, M., 147, 150, 160, 161
Kahneman, D., 119, 123 Leach, M. S., 36, 48
Kalbfleisch, I? J., 40, 41, 42, 49, 61, 63, Leary, M. R., 193, 203
77, 308,311, 321 Lee, J., 22, 23, 27, 165, 167, 173, 175,
Kamo, Y., 278, 280, 287, 294 183,214
Kaplan, R. E., 6, 7, 27, 169, 183 Lee, J. A., 214, 229
Karis, T. A., 233, 238, 239, 249, 251 Lee, K., 46, 48
Kelleher, M., 96, 101 Lee, S. W, 282, 295
Kellermann, K. A., 147, 159 Lefcourt, H. M., 119, 123
Lempert, L. B., 112, 123
AUTHOR INDEX =a- 32:

Lenin, V I., 258, 273 Matthies, B. K., 246, 250


Levine, R., 278, 279, 295 Mattison, A. M., 210, 216, 217, 218,
Levinger, G. A., 7, 8, 27, 105, 109, 123 222,229
Levitt, M. J., 104, 119, 123 Mayer, J. D., 119, 124
Levy, M. B., 87, 101 Mazanec, M., 84, 85, 98
Lewin, K., 7, 27 McCandlish, B. M., 217, 229
Lewis, R. A., 52, 76, 212, 229, 239, 251 McClintock, E., 105, 123
Lewis, R. J., 92, 102 McCroskey, J. C., 289, 290, 295
Liddell, D., 128, 131, 132, 139, 304, McGoldrick, M., 31, 32, 34, 36, 37, 48
321 McGraw, K. M., 193, 195, 203
Liden, K., 258, 273 McNeilis, K. S., 55, 75
Lim, T, S., 280, 282, 283, 295 McWhirter, D. I?, 210, 216, 217, 218,
Lin, Y. W, 278, 295 222, 229
Lipkus, I., 234, 241, 245, 252, 288, 295 Mendola, M., 211, 213, 229
Liu, J. H., 233, 235, 237, 250 Merkle, E. R., 141, 160
Lloyd, S., 135, 138 Meshterkina, E., 255, 257, 270, 274,
Locke, H. J., 5, 28 275
Lott, L., 33, 34, 49 Messman, S. J., 57, 60, 63, 76, 79, 82,
Loving, T J., 238, 249 88, 89, 92, 93, 101, 119, 123,
Lowenthal, M. F., 79, 102 301,305,311, 320
Lujansky, H., 313, 320 Mettetal, G., 94, 100
Lund, M., 130, 138 Metts, S., 187, 188, 189, 193, 196, 198,
Lyde, M., 246, 250 199, 202,203
Lymon, S. M., 199, 204 Meyer, l? R., 148, 161
Lynch, F. R., 210, 229 Mikula, G., 313, 320
Lynch, J. M., 210, 214, 216, 217, 229, Mikulincer, M., 87, 101
230 Milardo, R. M., 20, 28, 96, 101, 212,
229,309,320
Millar, F. E., 199, 202
M Miller, G. R., 147, 159
Miller, J. B., 33, 48
Maddock, G., 270, 272 Miller, R., 165, 176, 177, 184
Maddock, J. W., 270, 273 Miller, R. S., 186, 189, 195, 203
Maguire, K., 130, 134, 138 Mitchell, W. J., 142, 161
Maines, J., 128, 138 Mitchell-Kernan, C., 236, 252
Malysheva, M., 255, 257, 263, 270, Miyahara, A., 278, 289, 295
273, 274, 275 Miyake, K., 246, 251
Mamonova, T., 258, 273 Mongeau, l?, 129, 130, 133, 138
Manstead, A. S., 193, 203 Monsour, M., 91, 92, 101
Marecek, J., 215, 229 Montemayor, R., 35,44, 48
Marelich, W D., 91, 100, 232, 245, Montgomery, B. M., 3, 6, 26, 28, 43, 49,
246, 250 71, 74, 83, 84, 98, 101, 106,
Mares, M., 37, 48 121,302,318
Markus, M. L., 142, 157, 160 Moore, D. S., 193, 203
Martin, C. E., 213, 228 Moos, R. F., 113, 123
Martz, J. M., 112, 114, 124, 232, 236, Morelli, G., 246, 251
242, 252 Morrow, G. D., 197, 204, 245, 246, 252
Mason, L., 143, 161 Moss, B., 129, 139
Matheson, C. C., 94, 100 Morton, H. W., 259, 274
Matskovsky, M. S., 261, 270, 272, 273, Morvant, P, 258, 274
274 Munley, M. E., 80, 98
Matsumoto, Y., 278, 289, 294 Murnen, S. K., 105, 121
Matthews, M., 259, 274 Murphy-Berman, V, 312, 319
Matthews, S., 93, 97, 101 Myrick, F. L., 213, 227
330 M AUTHOR INDEX

N Paulson, R., 214, 229


Pearson,J. C., 10, 15, 17, 26, 28, 133,
139, 173, 183
Nardi, l? M., 89, 93, 101 Pedhazur, E. J., 241, 242, 247, 25 1
Nass, C., 143, 161 Peplau, L. A., 105, 123, 211, 212, 213,
Negel, L. A., 193, 203 214,215,216,226,227,229
Negroponte, N., 158, 161 Perlman, D., xiii, xiv, xvi, 5 3, 52, 76, 77,
Nelligan, J. S., 87, 102 105, 123
Nelson, J., 33, 34, 49 Perry, B. J., 66, 77
Neudecker, T. E., 46, 49 Petersen, A. C., 35, 47
Newcomb, T. M., 108, 123 Peterson, C., 90, 100
Nezlek, J., 90, 102 Peterson, D. R., 105, 123
Nicholson, J., 84, 85, 98 Peterson, G. W., 32, 38, 40, 41, 42, 49
Nishida, T., 278, 289, 294 Peterson, M. R., 116, 123
Nix, C. L., 60, 67, 76, 77, 89, 95, 101 Petronio, S., 34, 49, 71, 77
Noll, A. M., 157, 161 Philpott, J., 18, 26
Noller, P, 33, 34, 35, 37, 39, 41, 49, Pierce, G. R., 113, 120, 123
245,246,250, 263, 274 Pietropinto, A., 213, 229
Norton, R., 5, 28 Pisklakova, M., 255, 257, 270, 274, 275
Nussbaum, J. F., 93, 97, 101 Pistole, M. C., 87, 101
Pittam, J., 142, 143, 149, 161
Pittman, G., 84, 85, 98
0 Planalp, S., 193, 204, 313, 320
Pollack, C., 144, 161
T. G., 39, 49 Pomeroy, W. B., 213, 228
Odden, C., 173, 177, 181, 183 Porterfield, E., 233, 238, 239, 243, 249,
Oliker, S. J., 7, 28 251
Olson, D. H., 39, 44, 47, 49, 255, 261 Pott, M., 246, 251
270,274 Poulsen, S., 20, 28
Omdahl, B. L., 106, 108, 117, 122 Powell, R. D., 233, 238, 239, 249, 251
Omoto, A., 105, 121 Power, T. G., 35, 48
Oravec, J., 45, 46, 49 Prager, K. J., 219, 229
T., 150, 160 Precker, M., 103, 123
Ossana, S. M., 209, 210, 213, 229 Prins, K. S., 83, 99
Oxley, D., 90, 100 Prisbell, M., 66, 73, 77

P K
Padesky,C., 216, 229 Rabby, M. K., 82, 101, 142, 153, 156,
Page,M. S., 91, 100, 232, 246, 250 161, 180
PageYoun, M. S., 246, 248, 251, 308, Ragsdale,J. D., 63, 64, 66, 77, 266, 274
320 Ramey, J. R., 214, 229
Palucki, L. J., 91, 100 Ramkissoon, M., 246, 250
Park, M. S., 290, 295 Rankin, C., 66, 77
Park, S-Y., 279, 280, 287, 288, 296 Rawlins, W. K., 81, 83, 84, 85, 88, 93,
Parks, M., 71, 76, 77 94, 95, 96, 97, 101, 315, 320
Parks, M. R., 141, 142, 145, 146, 147, Ray, G., 20, 28
159, 161, 201, 203 Razlogov,K. E., 255, 272
Pataki, S. l?, 82, 101 Read, S. J., 86, 87, 99
Patterson, B. R., 93, 97, 101, 197, 204 Reece, R., 215, 229
Patterson, D. G., 2 10, 229 Reilly, M. E., 214, 216, 217, 229, 230
Paul, E. L., 96, 101 Reis, H. T., 90, 102, 232, 245, 246, 250,
Paulson, C., 214, 229 278, 296
AUTHORINDEX -=+ 331

Remien, R. H., 212, 213, 230


Renner, M., 129, 139
Repinski, D. J., 35, 48
Reske, J. R., 128, 129, 131, 134, 135, Sabatelli, R. M., 63, 77
139, 158, 162 Saghir, M. T., 211, 230
Reyna, J., 149, 162 Sahlstein, E., 21, 27, 135, 136, 139
Rheingold, H., 145, 161 Salaff, J., 144, 162
Rholes, W S., 87, 102 Salovy, P, 119, 124
Rice, R. E., 145, 157, 161 Samp, J.A., 193, 196, 199, 204
Richardson, R. A., 141, 160 Samter, W., 20, 26, 85, 86, 88, 89, 94,
Rijsman, J. B., 116, 122 96,99,318,319
Rimashevskaya,N., 255, 257, 258, 270, Sanchez-Hucles, J., 92, 102
274,275 Sanders, R. E., 150, 161
Rindfuss, R., 131, 138 Sarason,B. R., 113, 120, 123
Rintel, E. S., 142, 143, 149, 161 Sarason,I. G., 113, 120, 123
Rios, D. 1?5:32, 234, 246, 248, 250, Sato, S., 278, 279, 295
Schaefer, E. F., 117, 122
Ritchie, D., 65, 76, 317, 319 Schaefer, J. A., 113, 123
Roberto, K. A., 83, 101 Schapiro, L., 259, 274
Roberts, L. D., 142, 144, 145, 146, 161 Scharfe, E., 246, 252
201,203 Schmitt, J. F!, 211, 212, 214, 215, 226,
Robertson, L. R., 260, 261, 274 229,230
Robins, E., 211, 230 Schmitz, J., 157, 160
Rogers,L. E., 199, 202 Schratter, A., 193, 203
Rohlfing, M. 128, 129, 131, 132, 136, Schruijer, S. G., 116, 122
138 Schuh, R., 21, 27
Roloff, M. E., 187, 188, 204, 282, 295 Schullo, S. A., 215, 216, 230
Rose, S. M., 80, 88, 89, 90, 91, 92, 96, Schulman, L., 147, 159
97,102 Schwartz, J. E., 106, 124
Rosenberg,M., 246, 251 Schwartz, l?, 33,34,49,209,210,211,
Rosenblatt, I? C., 233, 238, 239, 243, 212, 213,216,227,228,229
249, 251 Schwebel, A., 129, 139
Roth, D. M., 86, 98 Scott, J. l?, 83, 101
Rothbaum, F., 246, 251 Scott, M. B., 199, 204
Rotter, J. B., 119, 123 Seeger,M., 259, 274
Roudakova, N. V, 258,268, 274 Segrin, C., 107, 122
Roush, l?, 166, 182 Segrist, A. E., 215, 229
Rubenstein, T. S., 94, 102 Selfe, C. L., 148, 161
Rubin, A. M., 214, 230 Selinger, M., 87, 101
Rubin, Z., 105, 123 Sellen, A. J., 157, 161
Rusbult, C. E., 6, 28, 40, 49, 109, 112, Semic, B. A., 68, 69, 75
114, 120, 122, 123, 124, 196, Semin, G. R., 193, 203
197, 204, 212, 227, 232, 233, Sensibaugh,C., 92, 100
234, 235, 236, 237, 238, 239, Serafica, F. C., 80, 90, 96, 102
240, 241, 242, 243, 244, 245, Serovich, J. M., 209, 227
246, 249, 250, 251, 252, 253, Shackelford, T K., 213, 230
263, 274, 278, 284, 285, 288, Shaffer, L. S., 97, 98
295,308, 311,320 Shapiro, C., 82, 101
Rushing, B., 128, 138 Shaver, P, 245, 251
Rutherford, D. K., 193, 204, 313, 320 Shea, B. C., 10, 15, 28, 133, 139, 173,
Rutt, D. J., 55, 76 183
Ryu, D., 157, 160 Sherrod, D., 89, 93, 101
332 -+= AUTHORINDEX

Shibazaki, K., 236, 252 Steers, W N., 91, 100, 246, 250
Shimizu, H., 279, 295 Stephen, E., 131, 138
Shulman, S., 87, 94, 95, 102 Stephen, T., 129, 130, 133, 139, 154,
Sias, I? M., 165, 173, 174, 177, 179, 158, 162
181,183 Stevens, J., 241, 242, 247, 252
Sidelinger, R. J., 33, 47 Stipek, D., 194, 204
S&man, S. J., 1, 23, 28 Stone, A. A., 106, 124
Sillars, A. L., 197, 204, 269, 274 Stone, G., 131, 132, 138
Silver, M. E., 104, 119, 123 Strejc, H., 55, 76
Silverman, B., 258, 274 Strube, M. J., 112, 124
Silverthorn, K., 199, 203 Suhr, J., 219, 227
Simenauer, J., 213, 229 Summers, S., 232, 245, 250
Simmons, C. H., 279, 295 Surra, C. A., 239, 242, 252
Simon, E. I?, 43, 47, 220, 227 Swanberg, J. E., 24, 26
Simpson, J. A., 87, 102 Swensen, C., 130, 131, 138
Singh, P, 312, 319 Swigger, K., 149, 162
Slovacek, C., 151, 162 Sysenko, V A., 262, 275
Slovik, L. F., 234, 241, 245, 252, 288,
295
Smith, A. G., 309, 320 T
Smith, J., 152, 160
Smith, K., 84, 85, 98 Taing, P, 233, 250
Smith, L. M., 144, 161 Takahashi,Y., 246, 250
Snyder, M., 105, 121 Tangney,J. P, 194, 204, 205
Soloman, D. H., 193, 196, 199, 204 Taylor, D. A., 105, 121, 185, 202
Spanier, G. B., 5, 28, 52, 76, 214, 227, Taylor, S. E., 223, 228
239, 251 Teachman, J. D., 255, 275
Spears, R., 147, 150, 160, 161 Tepper, B., 169, 176, 181, 183
Sperling, V, 258, 260, 274 Tesch, S. A., 95, 97, 102
Spiegelhoff, M., 15, 28 Thacker, R. A., 165, 174, 183
Spitzberg, B. H., 92, 100, 199, 203, Thibaut, J. W, 4, 6, 28, 63, 76, 109,
293,295 124, 219, 230, 232, 233, 234,
Sprecher, S., 53, 76, 77, 263, 274, 280, 251,252, 312, 321
294, 309,312, 319,320 Thompson, A. E, 213, 230
Springer, C., 193, 203 Thompson, S., 152, 160
Sproull, L., 141, 145, 160, 161 Thomsen, D. G., 113, 124
Sroufe, L. A., 87, 94, 95, 102 Thomson, E., 231, 253
Stafford, L. S., xiii, xvi, 7, 14, 15, 16, Thornton, A., 231, 253
18, 26, 27, 28, 32, 33, 38, 39, Ticoll, D., 45, 46, 49
42, 49, 51, 52, 53, 54, 55; 56, Tidwell, L. C., 147, 149, 151, 162
57, 60, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65. 66, Ting-Toomey, S., 278, 289, 294, 295
67, 68, 69, 70, 71; 72; 73; 74; Tolhuizen, J. H., 18, 19, 28
75, 76, 77, 79, 82, 88, 91, 97, Traupmann, J., 53, 76, 312, 320
99, 102, 129, 130, 131, 133, Triandis, H. C., 248, 252, 279, 295
134, 135, 136, 137, 138, 139, Tripp, T. M., 114, 124
143, 153, 155, 156, 158, 160, Tschann,J. M., 91, 102
161, 162, 174, 176, 182, 187, Tucker, M. B., 236, 252
197, 199, 202, 204, 209, 219, Tuller, N. R., 213, 215, 226, 230
220, 222, 225, 227, 228, 230, Turkle, S., 142, 162
262, 264, 265, 267, 272, 273, Tversky, A., 119, 123
274, 275, 277, 280, 282, 284,
286, 293, 294, 295, 301, 303, u
305, 307, 308, 311, 312, 314,
316, 317, 319, 320,321 Uhl-Bien, M., 167, 176, 182
Steen, S., 33, 34, 49 Utne, M., 53, 76, 312, 320
AUTHORINDEX +a- 337,

Utz, S., 142, 146, 162 Weisz, J., 246, 251


Wellman, B., 20, 28, 144, 162
V Wenzel, A., xiv, xvi
Werking, K. J., 91, 102
Vangelisti, A. L., 3, 19, 27, 32, 36, 38, West, A. M., 91, 100
49, 193,205 West, J. T, 112, 124
Van Lange, I? A. M., 233, 242, 243, 244, est, L., 84, 85, 98
249,252, 253 Westefeld, J. S., 132, 139, 304, 321
Vannoy, D., 255, 257, 265, 266, 267, Wexler, M. O., 232, 233, 238, 240, 245,
268,274, 275 250
Verbrugge, L. M., 95, 102 Whaley, K. L., 94, 102
Verette, J., 197, 204, 232, 234, 239, Wheeler, L., 90, 102, 278, 296
241, 245, 252, 284, 285, 288, Wheeless, L. R., 149, 162
295,308, 311, 320 White, J. M., 39, 40, 41, 48
Verma, J., 278, 279, 295 White, L. K., 263, 275
Vincent, J. I?, 185, 195, 202 Whitehurst, R. N., 214, 228
Vogl-Bauer, S., 40, 41, 42, 49, 61, 63, Whitely, W, 176, 182
77, 308,311, 321 Whitney, G. A., 234, 241, 245, 252,
von Kolke, A., 279, 295 288, 295
Whittemore, G., 149, 160
Wieselquist, J., 237, 242, 246, 253
W Wigley, III, C. J., 116, 123
Williams, C., 211, 230
Wabnik, A. I., 15, 27, 68, 76, 186, 203, Williams, G. D., 269, 272
277, 279, 294,302,320 Williamson, R. C., 5, 28
Wagner, G. J., 212, 213, 230 Wilmot, W W, 3, 8, 26, 28, 132, 133,
Waite, L. J., 231, 253 139,269, 274
Wakabayashi,M., 166, 176, 181, 182 Wilson, S. R., 279, 295
Waldron, V, 21, 22, 23, 28, 163, 166, Wilson, T., 145, 160
168, 169, 173, 175, 176, 177, Winstead, B. A., 90, 91, 92, 99, 102
178, 181, 183, 184,318,321 Wiseman, J. P, 85, 102, 104, 113, 124
Wall, S., 86, 98 Witcher, B. S., 242, 243, 244, 253
Wallace, L. A., 15, 26, 57, 60, 64, 75, Witt, I? L., 149, 162
79, 82, 88, 99, 129, 130, 133, Wolicki, J. S., 144, 160
137, 153, 160 Wood, J. T, 104, 124, 266,275
Walster, E., 53, 69, 77, 118, 121 Work, C., 251, 308, 320
Walther, J. B., 86, 98, 142, 144, 147, Wright, K. D., 118, 124
148, 149, 150, 151, 152, 156, Wright, I? H., 80, 81, 87, 91, 102, 118,
162, 180,201, 205, 318, 321 124
Watson, M. A., 214, 230 Wright, T. L., 213, 227
Watson, M. R., 174, 182
Watzlawick, P, 112, 116, 117, 124, 316,
321 Y
Wayne, S. J., 165, 174, 176, 183, 184
Weber, M. D., 80, 98
Weigel, D. J., 36, 49, 62, 63, 65, 67, 69, Yanowitch, M., 258, 274
71, 74, 77, 256, 257, 261, Yarab, P, 92, 100
262, 264, 265, 266, 267, 271, Yi, J. S., 250, 279, 280, 287, 288, 296
272, 275, 306,308,318 Yoon, H. J., 278, 289, 295
Weigel, R. R., 266, 275 Young, A., 211, 228
Weinberg, M., 211, 230 Young, S. L., 193, 205
Weinberg, M. S., 211, 213, 227 Yum, J. O., 278, 281, 282, 296
Weiss, L., 79, 102 Yum, N., 246,250
Weiss, R. L., 185, 195, 202 Yum, Y., 282, 285, 286, 287, 288, 290,
Weiss, R. S., 80, 102 291,293, 296
334 -I++ AUTHORINDEX

Zachs, E., 213, 228


Zaguidoulline, M. G., 62, 74, 257, 264,
265,266, 271,272,308,318
Zdravomyslova, 0. M., 265, 272
Zelley, E. D., xiv, xvi, 6, 26, 57, 68, 70,
75, 81, 83, 87, 99, 102, 310,
311,315, 316,319
Zembrodt, I. M., 241, 245, 246, 252,
288,295
Zormeier, M. M., 92, 100
Zuo, J., 263, 275
subject Index

Accommodation, 24 l-242 Definitions of Maintenance, 2, 9, 10,


Affinity, 11 53-54,187-l 88,194,209,
Alternatives, 235-236, 242-243, 285 219,264,300-301
Attachment, 86-88, 245-246 Developmental Issues, 34-38, 93-97,
Attributions, 196 269
Dialectics, 3, 6, 42-44, 83-85, 106,
B 218, 220-221,314-316

Barriers, (see External Forces)


lz

c Emotions, 6, 105, 108-109, 174-175,


194-195
Equity, 41-42, 53, 56, 63-64, 81-83,
Centripetal vs. Centrifugal Forces, 7-9, 53 209,216, 220,312
Choice, (see Nonvoluntary/Voluntary) Expectations, 64, 129, 135, 136, 200,
Circumplex Model, 44 231,266
Commitment, 67, 69-70, 131, 196, Expression vs. Suppression, 6-7
197,212-214,233-234, 238, External Forces (including Barriers),
239,242-245271,279 xiii, 7-8, 53, 109-l 10, 112,
Communication Channels, 13 3- 13 5, 289
153-158
Comparison Level (CL), 234-23 5
Comparison Level of Alternatives I=
(Clalt), 235-236, 239, 312
Control, 67
Culture, 61-62, 186, 246, 247-248, Facetoface Communication, 25, 45,
256,257-262,278-281, 134-135, 144, 145, 147-148,
286-289,290,291-292, 151, 152,201
309-310,312-313,314,316 Friendship, 60, 67, 79-98, 173-174

335
330 -+a SUBJECT INDEX

Gay/Lesbian Relationships, 61, 209-226 Nonvoluntary/Voluntary Distinction (in-


Goals, 107, 168, 197 cluding Choice), 20, 22-23,
32,104-105,109,301-302,
311-313,315,316
H
0
Homosexual Relationships, 61, 209-226
Hyperpersonal, 15 l-l 52, 158
Online Relationships, 25, 144-145,
146~147,152,201
Openness (see Self-Disclosure)

Ideology (see also Culture), 65, P


257-259,262,278-279,281
Intentionality (see also Routine/Strate-
Perceptions, 15-16, 35-36, 65
gic), 56, 73, 287, 302-304,
311-312,313-314,315 Personality, 113, 119, 197, 245-247
Interdependence, 4-5, 6, 38, 64, Power, 164, 216-217
233-234
Intimacy, 8, 20, 92, 105-106, 152, 279
R
Investment, 6, 120, 196, 234, 236-237,
285,288
Relational Characteristics and Mainte-
nance (see also Commitment,
Intimacy, Satisfaction, Trust),
67-68,68-70,280,286
Jealousy, 134 Relational Continuity, 1, 23-25
Relational Development, 18, 19,
145-146
Relational Termination, 18, 19
Relationship Type, 9, 13, 64-65, 73,
180-181, 197-198, 268-269
Listening, 33-34, 197 Repair Strategies, 13-l 4, 95-200
Long Distance Relationships (defini- Rituals, 8-9, 17, 36
tion), 129-l 30 Routine/Strategic Maintenance (see
Longitudinal Approaches, 68, 2 17-219 also Intentionality), 10, 16-l 7,
54-56,57,58-59,72-73,85,
220,302-304
M

Maintenance Behaviors, 9-l 4, 18-20, 5


33,56-57,58-61,88-89,
113-118,132~133,153-155, Satisfaction (Relational), 3-4, 5, 11-12,
164~173,219-220,221, 13, 51, 66-67, 69-70, 13 1,
240-245,264-266,308 196, 197, 212,234-235,
Mediated Communication (see also 263-264,287
Technology), 24, 25, 82, 89, Self, 116, 307-308, 314
134-135 Self Disclosure (including Openness and
Metacommunication (see Metacommunication), 7, 8, 9,
SelfDisclosure), 8-9 43,71,73-74,222
SUBJECT INDEX =+- 337

Sex Differences, 13, 15, 16, 66, 71,


90-91,92,215-216,257-260,
263,266-267
Sex Roles, 2 14-2 16
Sexual Harassment, 176-l 77
Social Exchange (see also Equity), 4, 6,
40-41, 167,219,233,
310-313
Social Information and Deindividuation
Theory, 150-151, 158
Social Information Processing Theory,
147-148
Social Skills, 85-86, 31 O-3 13
Stigma, 210, 221, 232
Strategic/Routine Maintenance, 16-l 7,
542Wb;7, 72-73,220,

Superior/Subordinate, 2 l-22, 167,


169-173
Synchrony, 143-l 44, 149
Systems Theory, 38-39, 167-l 68,
316-317

Technology (see also Mediated Com-


munication), 25, 45-46, 141,
180,201
Transgressions, 188-l 95
Trust, 67, 131, 212

Uncertainty Reduction, 133, 147, 198,


111-114

You might also like